Tracy Dobie Jonathan Lesser April 28, 2011 CTA Report Template (Part 3): Student Interview Choosing the Prompt/Problem (Q1) Which problem or prompt will you give students? (Q2) What are you hoping to get out of the student interview? (i.e., assess prior ability or test an instructional tactic)? In order to assess students’ prior knowledge, as well as access students’ typical or routine ways of solving problems, we asked the students to think aloud as they solved two different fraction comparison problems. We also prompted the students to think about and explain ways of solving the problems other than their initial strategies. The problems we provided to the students were as follows: Which fraction is larger: 3/5 or 4/7? Which fraction is larger: 35/76 or 47/99? In addition to asking students to solve fraction comparison problems such as the ones above, we asked the students to think aloud as they tried to develop a real life word problem using the fractions 3/5 and 4/7. The goal of this question was to assess how – and whether – students relate the fraction problems they see in school to their own lives. Selecting Students (Q3) Which students will you interview? We interviewed two 5th grade students. Neither of these students expressed a strong interest towards mathematics or fractions. The students also found math to be both easy and hard, varying with the problem. Think aloud / Interview Please see video files of the interviews and image files of the scratch work. Coding / Identification of errors and omissions Please see DobieLesser_NoviceInterviewCoding.xlxs Debriefing A significant finding that emerged from the data was that both of the novices were equipped with limited approaches to solving the fraction comparison problems. While both students were able to get the right answers, they both immediately began performing a standard algorithm once the problem was read (student 1: lines 7, 23; student 2: lines 7, 27). The experts, on the other hand, began by considering different facets of the problem that could possibly have lead to simpler solutions than finding a common denominator. As the novices lack this strategy for reasoning through the problem, they may fail to recognize other ways of solving different fraction problems that might be more efficient than their standard algorithmic approach. Furthermore, the novices lacked an ability to come up with alternative ways to solve the problem (student 2: 14-16, 35). The novices seemed to rely exclusively on problem solving strategies that they were taught explicitly as opposed to being able to reason about fractions through other means, even when provided with alternative approaches. Student 1 stated that representing the second fraction problem graphically would be “harder because this [the LCM algorithm] is the first way that I learned” (33), while student 2 stated that she couldn’t solve the first fraction problem using a number line because “we haven’t learned how to do that” (22). This is in contrast to our experts, who were able to use adaptive strategies to solve the problem using a number line despite not having generated that as a possible solution. In addition, the novices seemed to be familiar with other quick mnemonics to solve problems that run counter to the type of essential understanding laid out by the experts. Our second expert stated that one of the requisites for understanding fraction comparison problems is to know that each whole is divided up into “equal” parts, and that “larger” or smaller “pieces” do not hold comparative meaning. Student 2, however, started to outline a strategy to compare fractions based on the size of the pieces as derived from the denominator size, but quickly became confused and didn’t know how to capitalize on her proposed strategy (student 2: 14-15). Regarding motivation to work with fractions and perceived ability to solve fraction problems, the novices echoed two salient beliefs about mathematics that we view as problematic. Student 1 stated that she does not like fractions: “I don’t think there’s a fun way to learn fractions” (47), while student 2 stated, “I don’t love math because I’m not great at it” (47). Student 1’s statement implies she is not engaged with the material she is currently learning, while student 2’s statement implies that she has a low selfconception of her mathematical abilities, leading to a negative view of the subject. It is important to keep in mind, however, that these statements were made despite the fact that both students were able to accurately perform the operations to yield the correct answers in both of our think-aloud problems. We may be able to infer from this that while students are being taught how to solve fraction comparison problems, they are not really taught about fraction comparison problems; there is a lack of understanding apparent in their actions and responses. Both students stated that they do not encounter fractions in their everyday life (student 1: 40, student 2: 42), while they both agreed that their parents and teachers probably interact with fractions in their lives. The students’ current school experiences are absent of connections between the work they are doing and their everyday lives, yielding dissociated mathematical procedures and disinterested students. The experts, on the other hand, were able to recall several examples of fraction problems they encounter in their everyday lives (in addition to numerous alternative strategies for solving fraction comparison problems), likely contributing their status as “experts” who can solve fraction problems with confidence and enthusiasm.