Critical Analysis LEVEL IB Citation (APA) Ebbels, S. (2007). Intervention for Verb Argument Structure in Children With Persistent SLI: A Randomized Control Trial. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 1330-1349. Background The author noted a lack of studies that investigate language intervention with school-aged children with SLI. Children with a Specific language impairment past the early school years show severe effects on the child’s education and social adjustment. Objective This article is comparing 2 interventions to improve use of verb argument structures in pupils with persistent specific language impairment (SLI). One intervention was called “syntactic-semantic therapy” (shape coding) the other intervention focused only on verb semantics called “semantic therapy” Is it necessary for a syntactic-semantic approach or is it enough to focus on semantics alone? Search Strategy The author investigated interventions targeting morphology, formation or comprehension of syntactic structure, active and passive sentences, dative construction, and reversible sentences involving prepositions such as under/over. Only one single case study focused on improving verb argument structure. Selection Criteria 27 pupils with SLI were recruited and assigned to one of three groups, each divided onto two phases. Assignment was random (9-syntactic-semantic therapy, 9-semantic therapy, and 9 to the control therapy). Had intelligible spontaneous speech, no hearing impairment, neurological dysfunctions, structural abnormalities, or diagnosis autism or Asperger syndrome. Required to have expressive and receptive scores -1.5 SD below the mean and Performance IQ no lower than -1.5 SD below the mean. Data Collection/ Analysis Participants in the syntactic-semantic therapy- first 2 sessions focused on change-of-location verbs, the next 4 focused on change-of-state verbs, and the final 3 focused on alternating verbs. Therapy was based on a shape coding system using shapes colors and arrows to describe the different parts of speech, and different kinds of phrases. Participants in the semantic therapy targeted one or two verbs each session but followed the same schedule as the syntactic-semantic therapy. For each verb the therapist and participant created a detailed written definition of the verb by asking participant to say everything he or she knew about the verb meaning. After a definition was achieved the participant or therapist would act it out. CD 601 Level I, II, III Studies 1/8 Participants in the control therapy attempted to improve ability to form inferences when comprehending texts with no focus on argument structure. Introduced to the idea of looking for cues, and were presented with sentences and stories typed with large gaps between the lines. In between the lines they wrote all the extra information figured out from clues. Main Results Both the syntactic-semantic and semantic therapy groups showed greater normalized gains then the control. The groups were significantly different from post therapy for targeted verbs and control verbs but lacked significance at follow-up. Both therapy groups showed significant effect of therapy for correct linking. Neither therapy groups showed significant effect on obligatory arguments. Only the syntactic-semantic group showed positive gain score for optional arguments. No significant difference in morphological errors. Author’s Conclusions Both syntactic-semantic and semantic therapy groups made significantly greater gains in their overall use of verb argument structure than those in the control therapy. Thus these interventions can effectively improve verb argument structure in older pupils with SLI as well. Overall, it provides evidence for the effectiveness of intervention for verb argument structure for older people with SLI. CRITICAL APPRAISAL Reasons for Inclusion This study compares two therapies for improving syntax and semantics. Both of these therapy approaches were “less natural” and states that therapy approaches that are more naturalistic than the approach used in the study have led to greater generalization to discourse contexts. Though both are effective, more research could be done to see if longer therapy increased generalization. Strengths Randomization of the participants; randomization of transcription, analysis, and coding; assignment of random sequence was predetermined; large sample size Threats Multiple treatment interference; Order effects; Gains were not maintained; lack of control of effect of outside therapy; Level I “Grade” A CLINICAL “BOTTOM-LINE ” Clinical Bottom Line CD 601 Though syntactic-semantic therapy and semantic therapy methods have been proved to be effective in improving syntax there is little carryover after therapy is discontinued. More natural methods have been shown to not only increase syntax but to build connections that carryover into everyday conversation outside of therapy. Level I, II, III Studies 2/8 Critical Analysis LEVEL IIB Citation (APA) Camarata, S. M. (1994). Comparison of Conversational-Recasting and Imitative Procedures for Training Grammatical Structures in Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 37, 1414-1423. Purpose/Rational Many argue that direct imitation is required for target acquisition and is superior to modeling procedure. Those findings do not compared if imitation-based therapy results would differ if in more natural environments. Conversational recast already naturally occurs in many situations and could be viewed as natural. The purpose of this study was to compare conversational recast training to direct imitation on the effectiveness in spontaneously generating a variety of morphological and syntactical structures. Research Question(s) Is conversational recast training more effective in spontaneously generating a variety of morphological and syntactical structure than direct imitation? Literature Review Many studies have found when comparing naturalistic and imitative procedures that there is no significant difference or their findings are inconsistent due to the similarity between the two approaches. Studies have shown many children with SLI have successfully learned syntax by using conversational recast developed for children that acquire language normally. Design Quasi- experimental IV(s) Immitation-based and conversation-based therapy conditions. DV(s) TOLD2-P; audiometric screening; Leiter International Performance Scale; TACL-R; mother-child free play conversation; Sampling Strategy Non-randomized Participants 21 children; one female, one male; children with specific language impairment; primarily expressive deficits. Intervention Investigated Imitative- child imitated the target following clinician model and prompt while paired with a picture or object stimulus followed by a verbal and/or token reinforcement. After reaching 90% correct responses, the model was taken away. After reaching 90% correct response without the model or prompt, clinician and client participated in a more interactive context with models, imitation prompts, and reinforcement with new stimulus. Fading occurred in clinician assistance as client reached the criterian. CD 601 Level I, II, III Studies 3/8 Conversational training- indirectly elicited attempts of target. Using toys the clinician would ask the client to discuss what they are doing with the toys. The clinician would recast using the targeted context i.e. relative clauses. The recast retains the semantic base of original utterance but clinician provides a model of the target form. This allows for easy processing. Coded as either spontaneous or elicited speech from the client. Main Findings In conversation- based treatment, subjects required a mean of 31.1 presentations to achieve elicited production and a mean of 63.6 presentations to produce targets spontaneously. For imitation treatment, subjects required a mean of 2.2 clinician presentations to achieve elicited production and a mean of 150.7 clinician presentations to produce targets spontaneously. Though clinician presentations were required to generate spontaneous production under the conversational based treatment but the elicited productions were generated much more rapidly under imitation treatment. Significantly fewer sessions were required for spontaneous production within conversation-based therapy though required more sessions to generate elicited production. Fewer clinician presentations were required for spontaneous productions with conversational treatment and overall spontaneous production was higher than imitation treatment. Imitation treatment is designed to get larger number of productions. Higher levels of generalizations are found with conversation-based therapy. CRITICAL APPRAISAL Reasons for Inclusion The ability to generalize a concept learned in therapy is extremely challenging for many of our clients. This study not only shows the success in generating the target responses but shows their ability to spontaneously use target responses in conversation. Strengths Large sample size; Threats Order effects; Non-representative sample; Multiple treatment interference; Limitations on sampling opportunities; differences in goals assigned randomly; No information on participant selection. Level II “Grade” B CLINICAL “BOTTOM-LINE ” Clinical Bottom Line CD 601 Imitation is tightly controlled and allows for easy productions from the client. Broader linguistic knowledge is needed for generalized spontaneous production. Conversational based therapy allows for a comparison between the clients productions to the clinicians recast thus allowing child to transfer Level I, II, III Studies 4/8 information from those comparisons to long term memory. Imitation may work for children with lower developmental level but previous studies have found children with higher developmental level succeed most with conversational-based therapy. Some studies have found lcients can only learn under an imitation approach while some can only learn with a conversational approach. Other studies have also indicated a hybrid approach of the two could be beneficial for clients. CD 601 Level I, II, III Studies 5/8 Critical Analysis LEVEL III Citation (APA) Nippold, M. (2008). Expository Discourse in Adolescents with Language Impairments: Examining Syntactic Development. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. Purpose/Rational This study looked at syntactic development in 8th graders after, at kindergarten, each participant had been identified as having specific language impairment (SLI), nonspecific language impairment (NLI), or typical language development (TLD). Expository discourse occurs in many settings used every day including school. It is important to examine all types of discourse genres when developing syntax for adolescence with language impairment. Research Question(s) 1. Would adolescents with SLI an NLIE show better use of syntax during and expository task than a conversational task? 2. Would language-sampling tasks reveal syntactic deficits in the SLI and NLI groups? In particular, would an expository task differentiate between adolescents with impaired and typical language development more effectively than a conversational task? Literature Review Students with language disorders can expect to show deficits in syntactic development when speaking in a variety of discourse genres, including conversational, narrative, and expository. They also tend to use short simple sentences. This may not be found in the standardized tests. In language samples you can find more detailed information necessary for intervention planning. They believe an expository task is a better tool than a conversational task for examining syntactic development. Design Correlational IV(s) Expository Task; Conversational Task DV(s) 5 core subtests; narrative task; Block Design and Picture Completion subtest; Clinical evaluation of language Fundamentals. Sampling Strategy Stratified cluster sample Participants 444 students enrolled in 8th grade public schools in Iowa or Illinois with American English as their primary language. All had participated in an epidemiological study of language impairment. PIQ must be higher than 85. Each child was then placed into three groups- specific language impairment, nonspecific language impairment, and typical language development. Intervention Investigated Two subtests were given to examine syntax from the CELF-3. Then a language sample was elicited by same examiners of standardized tests. The examiner then began a conversational interview asking questions like “can you tell me something about your school.” Then the client chose to talk about their favorite game or sport to elicit expository discourse. CD 601 Level I, II, III Studies 6/8 Main Findings Standardized tests showed the TLD group still in 8th grade outperformed SLI and NLI and SLI outperformed the NLI like stated in the earlier study. All three groups performed at a higher level when speaking in the expository genre than in the conversational genre on all syntactic variables. The conversational task showed no difference between the groups while the expository task showed that the TLD group outperformed SLI and NLI on Mean length of T and TLD outperformed NLI on relative clauses. CRITICAL APPRAISAL Reasons for Inclusion Expository discourse is a part of the curriculum for high school and middle school children. Though conversation is a more natural way of therapy more is needed to tap into the complexity of syntax. States to not just focus on language form but consider content and use of language by considering how much the client knows about the topic, and how interested and motivated the client is for talking about the topic. Strengths 3 investigators created a reference document and checked all documents twice after coded; All points of disagreement were discussed until reached agreement; Large sample size; Representative sample. Threats Multiple testing; Unable to perform an objective evaluation of participant’s knowledge and enjoyment of chosen activity; Evaluated expository discourse in only one contexts. Level III “Grade” B CLINICAL “BOTTOM-LINE ” Clinical Bottom Line CD 601 When working on syntax, a conversation is a natural therapy activity. It is important to elicit expository discourse and complex utterances to really find where the client struggles. All people use shorter simpler sentence in conversation. When doing natural therapy techniques, it is important to ensure your activities are challenging enough to find deficits and work on improving and modeling those correct sequences. When eliciting expository discourse you are pulling in authentic texts from the curriculum and assisting with generalizing the skills learned in therapy to material working on in the schools. Level I, II, III Studies 7/8 CD 601 Level I, II, III Studies 8/8