Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research XI 2015 Lectures and Hands-on Tutorials on Methodological Aspects of Language Acquisition Research General Information Keynote speaker: Harald Clahsen Invited speakers: Judy Clegg, Pelagia Derizioti, Paul Leseman Pim Mak, Chloë Marshall, Judith Rispens Jason Rothman, Ludovica Serratrice Tutorials Articulography CHILDES Hayo Terband Jacqueline van Kampen Eye Tracking: Reading Computational Methods Christina Bergmann and Raquel Garrido Alhama Eye Tracking: Visual LENA & Analysis of World Paradigm Spontaneous Speech Iris Mulders Pim Mak Petra van Alphen Multilevel Analysis PRAAT Preferential Listening/Looking Huub van den Bergh Willemijn Heeren Annemarie Kerkhoff and Maartje de Klerk ERP Ethics Caroline Junge Maartje de Klerk Lexical databases LimeSurvey Emmanuel Keuleers Martijn van der Klis SPSS Statistics with R Roeland van Hout Hugo Quené CONTENTS LOCATION 3 BY PUBLIC TRANSPORT / AIR 3 BY CAR 3 ACCOMMODATION 3 MAP 4 SUGGESTED LUNCHPLACES 5 PROGRAMME 6 ABSTRACTS 7 Talks 7 Tutorials 14 Poster abstracts 19 2 LOCATION EMLAR X will be mainly held at Kromme Nieuwegracht 80, 3512 HM Utrecht (Netherlands). The registration desk will be in the small cantina next to room 0.06, where the plenary lectures are given. The lectures and most of the tutorials (unless otherwise specified) will take place in this building, although the Poster Session will take place in Trans 10, room 0.07. NOTE: The Eye-Tracking: Reading and Preferential Looking/Listening tutorials are the only ones which are not held in Kromme Nieuwegracht 80, but at a different location: Janskerkhof 13, which is at a walking distance from the main location. For directions see the map. E-mail and internet can be used in room 1.08 at Kromme Nieuwegracht 80. The login and password are available at the registration desk. BY PUBLIC TRANSPORT / AIR The main Dutch airport at Amsterdam (Schiphol) is connected to Utrecht by a direct train, as are most other Dutch cities. For train schedules and prices, see the website of NS, the Dutch railways. Utrecht Centraal is the name of the railway station you will want to use. While waiting for your luggage you can buy a train ticket at the yellow/blue self-service ticket machine. Walking from Utrecht Centra(a)l Station takes 15-25 minutes. For directions, see the map below. There is also a good bus network. The nearest stop is Domplein, served only by bus line 2 (Museumkwartier Ringlijn) and also close is Janskerkhof, served by lines 11 (Uithof/UMC WKZ), 3 (Homeruslaan), 4 (Burg. F. Andreaelaan), and 8 (Overvecht Zuid). To plan a trip by public transport within the Netherlands, use the 9292ov site. BY CAR (NOT RECOMMENDED) You are most likely to find (expensive) parking spots at Janskerkhof, Lepelenburg, Kruisstraat or Springweg (indicated on signs from main roads). Traffic in Utrecht is restricted and can take a lot of time, as in most old city centers. ACCOMMODATION For a list of suggestions for accommodation please check the website (NB: Prices may have changed; we do not officially endorse these establishments in any way.) 3 MAP The map shows the railway Station Utrecht Centra(a)l . The poster session and drinks will be at Trans 10, room 0.07. Kromme Nieuwegracht 80 is where all lectures, as well as most tutorials will be held. The Eye:-Tracking: Reading and Preferential Looking/Listening tutorials will be held at Janskerkhof 13. When walking through Utrecht, the Dom tower (112 meters) is a nice reference point; see the picture below. It is marked as a dark blue circle. (A) Utrecht Centraal, (B) Trans 10, (C) Kromme Nieuwegracht 80, (D) Janskerkhof 13, circle: Dom church tower The pictures below are the Dom church and the entrance of Trans 10. 4 The pictures above are the entrance of Kromme Nieuwegracht 80 and Janskerkhof 13, respectively. SUGGESTED LUNCHPLACES Name Bakkerswinkel Bigoli Brasserie Domplein Broers Café de Vingerhoed Café Orloff Hofman Lokaal de Reunie Schrans Winkel van Sinkel Address Wittevrouwenstraat 2 Schoutenstraat 12 Domplein 20 Janskerkhof 9 Donkere Gaard 11 Donkere Gaard 8 Janskerkhof 17a ‘t Wed 3a Trans 10 Oudegracht 158 Phone 030 2667999 030 2368848 030 2322895 030 2343406 030 2319659 030 2321679 030 2302470 030 2310100 (Chipknip only!) 030 2303030 Apart from this list there are many more places in the vicinity of the workshop venue. 5 Progrmam: Wednesday, April 15th 9.30 – 10.15 Registration 10.15 – 10.30 Welcome (director UiL OTS Prof. dr. Frank Wijnen) 10.30 – 11.15 Jason Rothman University of Reading and UiT – the Arctic University of Norway Empirical and theoretical issues in third (or more) language acquisition 11.15 – 11.45 Coffee break 11.45 – 12.30 Ludovica Serratrice The University of Manchester Analogical reasoning, cognitive flexibility and syntactic priming 12.30 – 13.15 Pim Mak Utrecht University What speech corpora cannot tell us. Insights from eye tracking in research on bilingualism and specific language impairment. 13.15 – 14.30 Lunch break 14.30 – 16.30 Tutorial Session I 16.30 – 18.00 Poster session and drinks Trans 10, room 0.07 Thursday, April 16th 9.00 – 9.45 Paul Leseman Utrecht University Longitudinal modeling of language development in bilingual contexts: competition and transfer 9.45 – 10.30 Judy Clegg University of Sheffield Language trajectories in large population cohorts: reflections on using the ALSPAC Cohort 10.30 – 11.00 Coffee break 11.00 – 13.00 Tutorial session II 13.00 – 14.30 Lunch break 14.30 – 16.30 Tutorial Session III 16:45 – 17:45 Keynote lecture: Harald Clahsen Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism Grammatical constraints in language learners’ spoken language comprehension 17.45 – 18.30 Free time 18.30 Workshop dinner Friday, April 17th 9.30 – 10.15 Chloë Marshall UCL – Institute of Education, University College London Verbal fluency tasks as a window onto the organization and access of the lexicon 10.15 – 11.00 Judith Rispens University of Amsterdam (Sub)lexical processing in children with developmental language disorders 11.00 – 11.30 Coffee break 11.30 – 12.15 Pelagia Derizioti Max Planck Institute Neurogenomics of speech and language disorders: The road ahead 12.15 – 12.30 Poster prize and closing All lectures are in Kromme Nieuwegracht 80, room 0.06 6 ABSTRACTS Talks Harald Clahsen Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism Grammatical constraints in language learners’ spoken language comprehension The human language processor is capable of rapidly integrating grammatical information with Information from other sources during reading or listening. Yet, little is known of how (child and adult) language learners make use of grammatical cues along with other information sources during language comprehension. For the present talk, I will address this general question by investigating one specific phenomenon, the so-called plurals-in-compounds effect, the avoidance of plurals inside compounds in English and other languages (e.g. *rats eater vs. rat eater), which is subject to both structural and non-structural constraints. I will present new evidence from both compound-internal modifiers in English and German (e.g., rote Paprika(s)fresser ‘red pepper(s) eater’). Results will be reported from graded linguistic judgements and from visual world eyemovement experiments focusing on the role of grammatical constraints in both child and adult language learners’ spoken language comprehension. Our results indicate clear differences between child and adult learners in this domain. Whilst children seem to rely more upon structural properties of language (in the present case, morphological cues) during comprehension, adult learners showed earlier sensitivity to non-structural constraints. 7 Jason Rothman University of Reading and UiT – the Arctic University of Norway Empirical and Theoretical Issues in Third (or More) Language Acquisition In this talk, I will present the recent trends in the growing literature regarding formal linguistic approaches to adult multilingual acquisition. I will explain the formalized models of the L3/Ln initial stages, which seek to model the role of previous linguistic experience (transfer) as well as methodological issues that one needs to consider to properly test between them (e.g. selection of groups and how to counterbalance them, levels of exposure to the L3 at time of testing, issues of proficiency in the L2, differences regarding type of bilingual (simultaneous vs. successive), etc.. I will also discuss the implications of these initial stages models for development, inclusive of discussing methodological restrictions that obtain when testing L3/Ln developmental sequencing and ultimate attainment. I will review much of my own empirical work to expand on the above issues as well as show you recent methodological developments, such as incorporating EEG/ERP into our research program. Ludovica Serratrice The University of Manchester Analogical reasoning, cognitive flexibility and syntactic priming The mapping between events and linguistic constructions is rarely one-to-one. For example, the same transitive event could be described in English with an active construction (‘The dog chased the cat’), a subject cleft construction (‘It was the dog that chased the cat’) or a passive (‘The cat was chased by the dog’), amongst others. The specific choice of syntactic construction will depend on a range of different factors, including the perspective that the speaker chooses to take on the event, and the level of accessibility of that syntactic construction in previous discourse. An integral part of language development is the mastering of syntactic choice, i.e. the ability to choose different linguistic constructions (e.g. an active or a passive) to describe the same event 8 as a function of perspective taking. Research on children’s language development has exploited the structural priming paradigm to assess the extent to which children’s syntactic choices are affected by what they hear in the input and has generally shown that, although children can be successfully primed to use constructions of interest (e.g. passives, double object datives), there is considerable individual variation that is not yet well understood. As structural priming has been interpreted as evidence for implicit learning, a better understanding of the determinants of priming will give us a better insight into mechanisms of language learning in general. In this presentation I will report the results of a syntactic priming study with 47 monolingual English-speaking 5-year-olds. Passive and active constructions were primed in a within-subjects experimental design; measures of short-term memory, verbal working memory, language comprehension, sentence repetition, cognitive flexibility and analogical reasoning were included as predictors of the likelihood of priming. Individual differences will be discussed in the context of the relative contribution of these predictors. Methodological issues concerning the use of the syntactic priming methodology in the developmental literature will also be addressed. Pim Mak Utrecht University What speech corpora cannot tell us. Insights from eye tracking in research on bilingualism and specific language impairment Speech corpora can tell us much about the linguistic development of children. For example, the errors that children make provide an insight in their linguistic abilities. However, if children make certain mistakes, this may be due to lack of knowledge, but also to other factors, such as processing limitations. Processing research, for example using eye tracking, can provide further evidence for the source of the errors children make. In this talk, I compare language production and processing of typically developing monolingual Russian children with (1) monolingual Russian children with specific language impairment (SLI) and with (2) bilingual Russian-Dutch children. The latter two groups make similar errors in speech production. However, eye tracking experiments, using the Visual World Paradigm, show that in processing these two groups have different profiles: The bilingual children perform as well as monolingual children with typical language development in processing, thereby showing an understanding of constructions in which they make many errors in in production. This was not the case for children with SLI. This suggests that the production errors have different causes in the two populations. 9 Paul Leseman Utrecht University Longitudinal modeling of language development in bilingual contexts: competition and transfer The presentation is based on a number of longitudinal studies into bilingual development of preschoolers in immigrant communities in the Netherlands, focusing on the interplay of exposure to languages in the home environment and children’s language processing mechanisms. The bilingual children are from Moroccan-Berber-Dutch and Turkish-Dutch immigrant communities. Native Dutch monolinguals serve as a comparison group. The presentation will highlight (and explain) statistical models to track development of language skills (e.g., vocabulary, narrative skills) in both L1 and L2, changes in L1 and L2 language exposure, and the interrelations between development and exposure. Furthermore, issues of measuring exposure will be addressed. The usefulness of a longitudinal approach with change/growth modeling for teasing apart positive and negative mechanisms in situations of complex bilingualism will be discussed, referring in particular to competition between languages for exposure time and transfer of knowledge and skills across languages. Judy Clegg University of Sheffield Language trajectories in large population cohorts: reflections on using the ALSPAC Cohort Judy Clegg, PhD, Department of Human Communication Sciences, University of Sheffield, UK. Longitudinal studies of children with speech, language and communication needs (SLCN) are important in understanding their developmental trajectories and the provision needed to maximize their psycho-social outcomes. However, such studies are considerably small in size and clinically 10 focused (Clegg et al., 2005; Clegg et al., 2012). Therefore, research is now analyzing speech, language and communication trajectories in general population cohorts. This presentation will explore the methodologies involved in these data sets, the advantages this approach offers as well as the inherent challenges involved. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) will be presented to illustrate recently published research regarding the 1) links between early language development and social, emotional and behaviour al functioning and 2) the role of pragmatics in the relationship between social disadvantage and adolescent behaviuor. Finally, future research with large data sets will be considered including the mechanics of accessing these data sets. Delegates will learn about 1) the structure and content of general population cohorts; 2) the methodologies used to answer questions of these cohorts; 3) the advantages and disadvantages of these data sets and 4) the procedure for accessing this data. Chloë Marshall UCL – Institute of Education, University College London Verbal fluency tasks as a window onto the organization and access of the lexicon Verbal fluency tasks are widely used experimentally and in language and cognitive assessments. Participants are given a limited amount of time, most usually one minute, to name as many items as they can that belong to particular categories, for example “animals”, “foods”, “words beginning with the letter A”, etc. Fluency tasks yield a wealth of data, beyond merely the number of items produced, and these data shed light on how the participant’s lexicon is organised and accessed. The tasks can be carried out with children and adults, and my colleagues and I are the first to adapt them to signed languages. In this talk I will present data from a study of deaf children who use British Sign Language (BSL), some of whom are identified as having specific language impairment (SLI) in their signing (Marshall et al, 2013). The overall number of BSL responses produced by the group of children with SLI on a semantic fluency task did not differ from their typically developing deaf signing peers, but they were slower in getting started on the task and some made sign-finding errors. We conclude that vocabulary is not severely impaired in deaf children with SLI, and that lexical organisation is not different to that of typical signers, but that access to signs is more effortful, possibly reflecting a phonological impairment. References: 11 Marshall, C. R., Rowley, K., Mason, K., Herman, R., & Morgan, G. (2013). Lexical organisation in deaf children who use British Sign Language: Evidence from a semantic fluency task. Journal of Child Language, 40, 193-220. Marshall, C. R., Rowley, K. & Atkinson, J. (2014). Modality-dependent and -independent factors in the organization of the signed language lexicon: Insights from semantic and phonological fluency tasks in BSL. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 587-610. Judith Rispens University of Amsterdam (Sub)lexical processing in children with developmental language disorders Children who are diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI) and / or developmental dyslexia (DD) experience impairments in their oral language and / or their literacy development. There is overlap in the linguistic profiles of children with SLI and DD. In this talk similarities and differences between SLI and DD will be discussed. The focus will be on investigating the effects of neighborhood density on lexical word recognition and of phonotactic probability on non-word repetition. Pelagia Derizioti Max Planck Institute Neurogenomics of speech and language disorders: The road ahead Traditional screening approaches such as linkage analysis and genome-wide association studies have been extremely fruitful in identifying genetic factors associated with speech and language disorders. Linkage mapping of a large three-generational pedigree identified FOXP2as the first gene to be clearly implicated in a monogenic form of developmental verbal dyspraxia. Individuals 12 carrying FOXP2 sequence variants have difficulties in mastering the coordinated orofacial movements required for speech. Although etiological mutations of FOXP2are rare, the gene provides a valuable molecular window into neurogenetic mechanisms contributing to human spoken language. FOXP2 encodes a transcription factor with roles in brain development. Recently, next generation DNA sequencing in cases of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and intellectual disability (ID) revealed pathogenic mutations in FOXP1 and TBR1, two brain transcription factors known to interact with FOXP2. These findings highlight molecular links between FOXP2-related speech/language disorder and other neurodevelopmental disorders that are also characterized by language deficits. Undoubtedly, next generation sequencing holds great promise for uncovering the key genomic changes that affect our speech and language abilities, not only in relevant disorders, but also in the general population. However, in order to understand the biological impact of identified genetic variants it will be essential to assess their functional significance. The success of such efforts will depend on synergies between diverse research methodologies, including bioinformatics and experimental analyses using model systems, as well as integration of human genome sequences and functional gene network datasets. 13 Tutorials Articulography CHILDES Computational Methods ERP Ethics Hayo Terband Jacqueline van Kampen Christina Bergmann and Caroline Junge Maartje de Klerk Raquel Garrido Alhama Eye Tracking: Reading Eye Tracking: Visual World Paradigm LENA & Analysis of Spontaneous Lexical databases Speech Emmanuel Keuleers LimeSurvey Martijn van der Klis Iris Mulders Pim Mak Petra van Alphen Multilevel Analysis PRAAT Preferential Listening/Looking SPSS Statistics with R Huub van den Bergh Willemijn Heeren Annemarie Kerkhoff and Roeland van Hout Hugo Quené Maartje de Klerk Abstracts Articulography: Hayo Terband (UU) This workshop will introduce you to 3D Electromagnetic Articulography or EMA as it is currently used in spoken language research. EMA is a kinematic tracking technique to measure articulatory movements. A set of transmitter coils produces a magnetic field that generates currents in tiny sensors placed on the surface of the articulators. As the sensors move through the field, they are tracked by computer. In the first hour we will demonstrate the procedure from set up to measurement together with the accompanying software and discuss the ins and outs of the experimental setup. In the second hour, participants will get hands-on experience with data analysis and we will give some theoretical background on the different measures and some examples of how they can be used to study speech and language development. CHILDES: Jacqueline van Kampen (UU) This workshop will introduce you into CHILDES, a system for sharing and studying child language databases. We will discuss its history and success, and then demonstrate the transcription tools and conventions (including how to work with video). Finally, we will focus on the tools for the analysis of CHILDES data. Computational Methods: Christina Bergmann (Radboud University) and Raquel Garrido Alhama (UvA) Finding recurring patterns in speech is a fundamental ability for children acquiring their first language. This ability relies on statistical learning. Insights in the workings of this mechanism in children, however, are hard to obtain, because we only have indirect evidence of internal processes. Computational models allow us to study learning in greater detail than child studies. Additionally, simulations do not suffer the time constraints present in child studies. In this workshop we will 14 investigate the ability to find patterns using data from actual child experiments. We will replicate findings from child studies using a recent model of statistical learning. Participants will get hands-on experience with the use of the programming language Python, a high level scripting language ideally suitable for both beginners and advanced scientific computing. At the end of the workshop, participants will have built a working model and thus gained insight into the possibilities and limitations of computational modeling in child language research. Lexical databases: Emmanuel Keuleers (Ghent) Lexical databases such as CELEX, Lexique, SUBTLEX, childlex, Wordnet, the MRC psycholinguistic database, CoCa, …), are structured collections of information about words in a language. In contrast to corpora, lexical databases contain compiled information about each word, such as its frequency, phonology, morphology, grammatical function, semantics, etc. For researchers in psycholinguistics, lexical databases are indispensable for setting up and analysing experiments and for building computational models. The aim of this first session is to develop an understanding of the information that is available in lexical databases, how that information can be used in psycholinguistic research, and what the most common problems are when using them. I will start by tracing the historical context in which lexical databases were developed and discuss current advances. Then, we will explore different lexical databases and give an overview of the tools that can be used to extract and manipulate information from them. There will be an emphasis on correctly interpreting information from lexical databases, with a focus on understanding frequency measures. At the end of this session, participants should be confident about obtaining and interpreting data from lexical databases. Eye Tracking: Reading: Iris Mulders (UU) This workshop will introduce you to the eye-tracking methodology as it is currently used in reading research. In the first part, we will discuss the basics of the methodology, Dos and Don’ts concerning stimulus design and analysis, and well-known eye-tracking measures in reading, like first fixation, regression path, total reading time etc: how do we compute these measures, and how are they interpreted? In the second part of the workshop, we will look at some data to lend more concreteness to these issues. Eye Tracking: Visual World Paradigm: Pim Mak (UU) This workshop will introduce you to the Visual World Paradigm as it is currently used in psycholinguistic research. In a typical VWP experiment participants listen to sentences or a short discourse while looking at a scene (often a cartoon-like picture) on a computer screen while their eye fixations are monitored. The method has been shown to be sensitive to various language processes, ranging from auditory word recognition to grammatical ambiguity resolution (and beyond). 15 It has the important advantage that it provides clues about what happens prior to a critical word or sentence region. Another advantage is that this technique can provide a relatively direct insight into the interpretations listeners assign to the language perceived, which in reading-based methods can only be assessed indirectly. Furthermore, many interesting questions about language comprehension can only be answered by “situating” language within a real-world context. Finally, the visual world paradigm can be relatively easily applied with individuals of all ages and competences. However, as in reading research, it is not always easy to link the eyemovements patterns of your participants to higher cognitive functions such as language. In the light of this more general debate, we will discuss issues concerning experimental design, data collection and statistical analysis, which will be illustrated with data from recent experiments in our lab. These experiments cover a range of topics, including verb semantics, the processing of noun gender, the processing of causal relations and discourse coherence. ERP: Caroline Junge (UU) In this tutorial we will talk about EEG: what is it, where does it comes from, and how do you get to an ERP. The scalp-recorded EEG, a real-time reflection of neuronal activity, is one of the tools we can use to study the machinery behind language processing and language acquisition. In this tutorial, we’ll look at what we can do with this research tool, as well as what the constraints are. LENA (Language Environment Analysis) and the Analysis of Spontaneous Speech: Petra van Alphen (Kentalis) In this tutorial I will give an overview of the Language ENvironment Analysis system (LENA) and its application in early screening, research and treatment of language delays and disorders. I will then focus on its use in research in the light of our current project (colllaboration of Kentalis and NSDSK) in which we use LENA to collect data of 30 preschoolers (2;6-3;6) with specific language impairment and 30 normally-developing children (in progress). The data of this project will be used to explain how LENA software works, what it generates and what can be done with the information it provides. Furthermore, the possibilities and limitations of LENA will be discussed. Multilevel Analysis: Huub van den Bergh (UU) Many datasets a kind of natural hierarchy exists. For instance, repeated measurements are in fact measurements within subjects, or, to give another example, students are nested within classes which themselves are nested within schools. This nesting of observations have been an enormous problem for data analysis; in the first example both the variance within subjects (between measurements) and between subjects have to be estimated simultaneously. In the second example the differences within classes (between students) and between classes (within schools) as well as the variance between schools have to be estimated. Failing to do so always results in an underestimation of the total variance, and therefore the null hypothesis will be rejected too easily. Today the magic words are ‘multilevel analysis’. Multilevel analysis can be carried out in SPSS. In this hands-on tutorial the principles of multilevel analysis will be dealt with and participants should be able to carry out (simple) multilevel analysis afterwards. However, the tutorial is only open to 16 participants who have some knowledge of statistical testing (i.e. they should know concepts like variance, null hypothesis and testing statistics (t, F, χ2). Preferential Listening/Looking: Annemarie Kerkhoff & Maartje de Klerk (UU) In this tutorial, we’ll first provide a background on the methodology of preferential listening and looking by discussing the designs as well as different studies that have used these methods. After the introduction, we’ll run actual experiments in the Babylab to show you what equipment and which skills are needed to run thse experiments. We’ll also show you clips of babylab experiments from our research project to demonstrate the ins and outs of subject/data inclusion and exclusion and the reliability of coding the experiments. Both the first part, theoretical in nature, and the second part, the hands on approach, should lead to understanding of Babylab experiments. PRAAT: Willemijn Heeren (UU) In this tutorial you will learn the basics about how to use the program Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2014) for speech analysis and speech manipulation. Part of the tutorial will consist of hands-on practice with the software. Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David (2014). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Retrievable from http://www.praat.org/ SPSS: Roeland van Hout (Radboud University) Using SPSS to produce a few statistics is different from handling complex data in a larger research project. The data set can become too large to put all data in one file. Sometimes it is necessary to import and export data from and to other sources. In other cases, a distinction needs to be made between the levels of analysis. In this session four aspects of SPSS data handling and analysis will be dealt with: 1. Why keeping syntax? The window system in SPSS has many advantages. Using ‘oldfashioned’ syntax seems to have the advantage that more procedures and more options are available. However, an essential difference is that by using syntax you can save en redo the (complex) operations required to get the statistical analyses done. 2. Why using different files? In a larger research project you may collect so many data that they need to be split into different files. How should you do that and how can you use the function merge? Another option is to apply the exchange (import/export) facilities, for instance with EXCEL. 3. How to switch between speakers and occurrences? In dealing with a corpus, the basic level of handling data is represented by the occurrences of the linguistic phenomenon to be analyzed. Nevertheless, it is often necessary to calculate scores on the level of the speakers or participants. This level switch can be handled by using the function of aggregate. 4. How to change the structure of the data matrix? For frequency analysis, it is good to know what the weight function may do, for reliability analysis you need sometimes to transpose your matrix structure, and it may happen that you need to repeat an analysis for different groups. How useful are options like weight, transpose, and split? This list can be expanded by implementing questions of the participants, including questions about specific statistical procedures and techniques. 17 Statistics with R: Hugo Quené (UU) This workshop will introduce the R programming environment for statistical analysis. Contrary to SPSS which is procedure-oriented (commands are verbs, e.g. “compute”), R is object-oriented (objects are nouns, e.g. “factor”). In this workshop, we will try to ease the learning curve of using R for your data analysis. Experience with statistical software is NOT required! We will use data simulation as well as real data sets, to explore topics like t-tests, χ2 tests, and regression. We will also show how R produces publication-quality figures. Lime Survey: Martijn van de Klis (UU) LimeSurvey is a free and open-source on-line survey application. LimeSurvey allows users to quickly create intuitive, powerful, online question-and-answer surveys that can work for tens to thousands of participants without much effort. For researchers, big pluses are that LimeSurvey is easy to use, is modifiable to a great extent and can be installed on university servers (thus keeping the data in-house). In the first part of this tutorial we will cover some aspects of survey creation with LimeSurvey. We’ll cover question types like scales (Likert, Thurnstone), audio/image presentation and arrays. We’ll talk about what LimeSurvey can and can’t do with regard to randomization. We’ll go over different access modes for participants, and will also cover issues with opening up your survey to a broad public. In the second part you are able to implement a survey yourself on the Utrecht University LimeSurvey server. We’ll provide some example survey ideas, but you’re free to try and implement your own. Ethics: Maartje de Klerk (UU) This tutorial is about the ethical aspects of doing research with human subjects, ranging from subject recruitment to data analysis and storage. My aim is to familiarise you with the laws and codes of conducts that currently apply in the Netherlands, but also present you with some ethical dilemmas. We will discuss the grey areas, which are open to subjective interpretation. The setting is informal and participants of this tutorial are asked to actively join the discussion. 18 Poster Session Abstracts The acquisition of control in European Portuguese: A study on comprehension Celina Agostinho It is generally assumed that children have early knowledge of control in complement clauses (complements of promise are the exception), although very young children may have non-obligatory control readings of obligatorily controlled subjects (MacDaniel & Cairns, 1990b; McDaniel et al., 1991). As an explanation, it has been suggested that the syntactic representation of complement clauses is non target-like in child grammar (McDaniel et al., 1991), or alternatively that developmental effects follow from lexical acquisition (Sherman & Lust 1993). A comprehension task based on McDaniel and Cairns (1990a, b) was applied to 64 children (aged 3-5) and 20 adults. It comprises four test conditions: (i) subject control, transitive and ditransitive verbs, (ii) object control, direct and indirect objects, (iii) sentential subjects, and (iv) cases of pragmatically determined interpretation (pedir para ‘ask’). Children identify obligatory and non-obligatory control contexts, choosing an sentence-external antecedent only in sentential subjects. Subject control is also unproblematic except for the case of prometer ‘promise’, which could result from intervention effects (Friedmann, Belletti & Rizzi 2009; Orfitelli 2012 for similar effects on A-movement) if control is movement (Hornstein, 1999). However, these effects may also result from a misanalysis of the argument structure of verbs with two arguments (a DP and a clausal argument). Recently, Santos, Gonçalves & Hyams (2013) showed that children aged 3-5 misanalyse object control verbs as taking a single internal argument. This analysis predicts better comprehension results with object control verbs presenting more cases of misanalysis. Our data shows that this prediction is confirmed. (i) a. O esquilo consegue trazer uma maçã. The squirrel manage-3PS bring-INF an apple. The squirrel manages to bring an apple. 19 b. O galo promete ao coelho cozinhar o jantar. The rooster promise-3PS to+the rabbit cook-INF the dinner The rooster promises the rabbit to cook dinner. (ii) a. O ouriço proíbe o esquilo de tomar banho no lago. The hedgehog forbid-3PS the squirrel of take-INF bath in+the lake The hedgehog forbids the squirrel to bathe in the lake. b. O cisne diz ao pato para arranjar comida. The swan tell-3PS to+the duck to get-INF food The swan tells the duck to get food. (iii) Sair da lagoa chateia o elefante. Leave-INF from+the lagoon bother-3PS the elephant To leave the lagoon bothers the elephant. (iv) O touro pede ao burro para tocar à campainha. The bull ask-3PS to+the donkey to ring-INF to+the bell The bull asks the donkey to ring the bell. Friedmann, N., A. Belletti & L. Rizzi. (2009) Relativized relatives: Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua 119. 67-88. Hornstein, N. (1999) Movement and control. Linguistic Inquiry 30: 69-96. McDaniel, D. & H. S. Cairns (1990a) The child as informant: Eliciting linguistic intuitions from young children. In Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 19 (5), 331-344. ---. (1990b) The processing and acquisition of control structures by young children. In Lyn Frazier & Jill de Villiers (Eds.) Language Processing and Language Acquisition Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics 10, 313-325. McDaniel, D., H. S. Cairns & J. R. Hsu (1991) Control principles in the grammar of young children. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics. 1.121-38. Orfitelli, R. (2012) Argument Intervention in the Acquisition of A-Movement. PhD Dissertation, UCLA. Santos, A.L., A. Gonçalves & N. Hyams (2013) Aspects of the acquisition of control and ECM-type verbs in European Portuguese. Talk given at Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition. University of Oldenburg. Sherman, J.C. & B. Lust (1993) Children are in control. Cognition 46, 1-51. 20 Double delay in bilingual specific language impairment? Counter-evidence from Italian-German bilingual children with SLI Anna-Lena Scherger, Bergische Universität Wuppertal/Germany Successive as well as simultaneous bilinguals may lag behind monolinguals in selective grammatical domains within language acquisition (Müller & Hulk 2001; Paradis et al. 2007). Children with specific language impairment (SLI) show a delay in language acquisition as well. These children show a late start of language acquisition and a protracted delay in their language development as a whole (Leonard 2014). Research on bilingual SLI discusses a conceivable double delay in bilingual children with SLI (Orgassa & Weerman 2008; Paradis 2010; Leonard 2010). The present study provides counter-evidence against such a double delay in German-Italian SLI (ages 4 and 7). Longitudinal studies of bilingual children with SLI (n=3) are compared to cross-sectional data of monolingual typically developing (TD) children (n=18), monolingual children with SLI (n=10) and bilingual TD children (n=14). Data is analysed in terms of target-like case marking (accusative and dative) in German. Bilingual children with SLI show no cumulative effect of bilingualism and SLI, i.e. effects of bilingualism and SLI are not additive and do not result in a double delay compared to monolingual SLI and bilingual TD children. Leonard, L. (2014): Children with specific language impairment. 2nd edition. Cambridge: MIT Press. Leonard, L. (2010): Language combinations, subtypes, and severity in the study of bilingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 31(2), 310-315. Müller, N. & Hulk, A. (2001): Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4(1), 1-21. Orgassa, A. & Weerman, F. (2008): Dutch gender in specific language impairment and second language acquisition. Second Language Research 24, 333-364. Paradis, J. (2010): The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 31(2), 227-252. Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E. & Crago, M. (2007): French-English bilingual children´s acquisition of the past tense. In: Caunt-Nulton, H., Kulatilake, S. & Woo, I. (Eds.): BUCLD 31 proceedings. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, 497–507. 21 The acquisition of mood by monolingual speakers of European Portuguese Alice de Jesus Although the literature on acquisition of verbal mood is scarce, some previous studies suggest that the subjunctive is acquired late, becoming stable only around the age of 10 (Blake, 1983). This takes place long after relevant syntax, morphology (Gili Gaya, 1972) and basic cognitive capacities associated with ToM (Papafragou, 1998) are available. This paper presents the first set of data on the acquisition of the subjunctive / indicative mood contrast in European Portuguese (EP) complement clauses and its main goal is to determine which semantic values guide children in the distribution of mood. Concerning the adult grammar, in EP (as most Romance languages), the indicative is selected when a veridical epistemic attitude is expressed towards the embedded proposition, while the subjunctive is selected when both nonepistemic or nonveridical epistemic attitudes are expressed (Marques, 2009). An elicited production task was applied to 80 children (aged from 4 to 9) and to a control group of 20 adults, consisting of a sentence completion task of different types of matrix predicates: epistemic (veridical and nonveridical) and nonepistemic (veridical and nonveridical). The results show that, from the age of 4, children already use the subjunctive on a variety of contexts. However, the youngest children only seem to be sensitive to epistemicity, producing subjunctive in complements of nonepistemic verbs and indicative with epistemic verbs. As they develop, they become more sensitive to the combination of epistemicity and veridicality, also using the subjunctive in epistemic nonveridical contexts. It is then proposed that epistemicity precedes veridicality. Blake, R. 1983. "Mood Selection among Spanish Speaking Children, Ages 4 to 12.", The Bilingual Review, 10: 21-32. Gili Gaya, S. 1972. Estudios de lenguaje infantil. Barcelona: Bibliograf. Marques, R. 2009. “On the selection of mood in complement clauses”, in L. Hogeweg, H. de Hoop and A. Malchukov (eds.), Cross-linguistic Semantics of Tense, Aspect, and Modality, John Benjamins, 179-204. Papafragou, A. 1998. “The acquisition of modality: Implications for theories of semantic representation”, Mind and Language, 13: 370-99. 22 Reading comprehension in beginning first and second language readers Liza van den Bosch, Radboud University, Behavioural Science Institute A considerable number of children in primary education are confronted with the challenging task of learning to read in their second language. Already early in the process of formal reading instruction, in particular these children are found to have reading comprehension difficulties despite accurate word decoding skills. This research project seeks to investigate the early reading comprehension development of second language readers compared to first language readers. In a two-fold longitudinal study the precursors and development of word-totext integration are investigated throughout the second and third grade of primary school. Furthermore, eye-tracking is used to gain insight into children’s on-line word-to-text integration processes to explain reading comprehension. Prior to the start of the longitudinal study, a pilot study has been conducted. In this experimental study, 20 Turkish-Dutch L2 learners and 20 Dutch L1 learners (third grade) performed an anaphoric reference task and a causal relation task while their eye movements were recorded by means of eye tracking technology. Furthermore, their syntactic knowledge, decoding skills, and vocabulary knowledge were measured using standardized tests. Besides validating the use of eye tracking to investigate the reading process in young, beginning readers, the aim of this study was to gain more insight in the role of syntactic knowledge in text processing and reading comprehension of children from different linguistic backgrounds. 23 Neurobiological correlates of written text comprehension in young adults Galperina E.I., Kornev A.N., Nagornova Zh.V. Shemyakina N.V. Saint-Petersburg State Pediatric medical University The study deals with neurophysiological basis for written text comprehension. The main aim of the paper is to discuss EEG/ERP preliminary results obtained within complex investigation that consisted of a) psychological and reading skills assessment, and b) EEG/ERP/fMRI examination of written text comprehension. First, 102 subjects (typically-developed young adults, Russian native speakers) were assessed by the reading comprehension test; then, 15 percentile of the highest (HS) and lowest score (LS) participants (N=10) were included in neurophysiological stage of the study. In categorical decision tasks they were presented phrases with metaphoric/literal meaning and random letter sequences. The EEG spectral power analysis revealed sensitivity of theta (4-7.5Hz) and alpha2 (10-13Hz) frequency bands for differences between reading metaphoric and literal phrases. Reading of metaphoric phrases revealed higher values of the EEG power in theta band in occipital regions with a focus in the right hemisphere and in the central-frontal regions, with some accent to the left. Reading of metaphoric phrases was also characterized by higher alpha2 spectral power in the right parietal cortex, in comparison with reading of the literal phrases. The ERP study highlighted differences between reading metaphoric and literal phrases only in the HS group: significant differences (p < 0.05) were observed in the parietal cortex. This corresponds with the results of previous studies that support involvement of associative areas in the processing of figurative phrases. In the LS group neurophysiological correlates had no significant difference. This could be related to the text processing specificity in LS participants. Supported by RFBR grant №15-06-08349a. 24 L2 Acquisition of Portugues Articles by Serbo-Croatian Native Speakers Radovan Miletić Faculty of Social Sciences and Humanities of the Universidade Nova de Lisboa, Portugal The aim of this PhD project is to analyse the acquisition of L2 articles in European Portuguese by Serbo-Croatian (SC) native speakers (whose L1 lacks articles) focussing on their production. We want to test two hypothesis: Fluctuation Hypothesis (FH) (Ionin, Ko & Wexler, 2004) and Syntactic Misanalysis Model (SMM) (Trenkić, 2007). For L2 learners whose L1 lack articles, FH assumes full Universal Grammar access and predicts ‘fluctuation’ between specificity or definiteness patterns until the appropriate value is set. On the other hand, SMM predicts higher article omission rates in Art+Adj+N vs. Art+N sequences through misanalysis of articles as adjectives. Hence, learners assign articles some lexical meaning related to referent identifiability, which leads to omission due to overload of attentional resources (as it is more costly to process elements of meaning). Besides simple DPs, we will also focus on less explored complex DPs involving the [±DP ofGEN ±DP] construction in singular contexts. The projects addresses the following research questions: do SC learners rely on one pattern (specificity or definiteness) or do they ‘fluctuate’ between the two? Does the presence of an adjective affect article production leading to higher omission rates? Does the logical interpretation of [+DP ofGEN +DP] singular first-mention contexts require less time to process than the pragmatic interpretation of [-DP ofGEN +DP] singular contexts? Are SC learners driven by the uniqueness presupposition or by the quantificational reading in [±DP ofGEN -DP] singular contexts? Participants will complete two on-line tasks: a fill-in-the-blank task testing four [±definite, ±specific] conditions, with simple and complex DPs in singular contexts; and a task testing Art+(Adj)+N+(Adj) conditions using a lexical visual stimulus. These experiments will be conducted using DMDX (Forster & Forster, 2003). Forster, Kenneth I. & Jonathan C. Forester. (2003). DMDX. A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 35 (1), 116-124. Ionin, Tania; Heejeong Ko & Ken Wexler. (2004). Article semantics in L2 acquisition: The role of specificity. Language Acquisition 12, 3–70. Trenkić, Danijela. (2007). Variability in L2 article production: Beyond the representational deficit vs. processing constraints debate. Second Language Research 23, 289–327. 25 The effect of explicit training on the prosodic production of L2 sarcasm by Dutch learners of English Laura Smorenburg*, Joe Rodd*, Aoju Chen * presenting Our previous research suggests that Dutch learners of (British) English are not able to express sarcasm prosodically in their L2. The present study investigates whether explicit training on the prosodic markers of sarcasm in English can improve learners’ realisation of sarcasm. Sarcastic speech was elicited in short simulated telephone conversations between Dutch advanced learners of English and a native British English-speaking ‘friend’ in two sessions, fourteen days apart. Between the two sessions, participants were trained by means of (1) a presentation (2) directed independent and (3) evaluation of participants’ production and individual feedback in small groups. L1 British English- speaking raters subsequently evaluated the degree of sarcastic sounding in the participants’ responses on a five-point scale. It was found that significantly higher sarcasm ratings were given to L2 learners’ production obtained after the training than that obtained before the training; explicit training on prosody has a positive effect on learners’ production. 26 The Processing of Morphosyntactic Cues in wh-Questions: An Eye-Tracking Study Atty Schouwenaars (University of Oldenburg), Esther Ruigendijk (University of Oldenburg) & Petra Hendriks (University of Groningen) atty.schouwenaars@uni-oldenburg.de Object questions, in which the object precedes the subject, are difficult to comprehend for children due to a strong subject-first bias (among others; [1]). The ability to identify who is doing what to whom in sentences with a non-canonical word order by the use of case-marking does not develop until the age of 7 ([2]). Children seem to be even less sensitive to the verb agreement cue ([3] for Dutch, [4] for Italian). The aim of this study is to find out (1) when and to what extent German children make use of these cues in their interpretation of object questions, and (2) whether different cues lead to different processing (also for adults), in terms of different eye-gaze patterns. We tested 7-10 year old German children (n=33), and an adult reference group. A picture selection task with eye-tracking was carried out to test the comprehension subject, object and passive wh-questions disambiguated by case and/or agreement. Offline data confirm that children (as well as adults) respond less correctly on object questions, than on subject and passive questions. Offline data do not indicate differences between the different types of cues, but online data do: The eye-gaze patterns show that for both adults and children, object questions disambiguated later in the sentence by agreement and case cues (on the second NP) lead to incorrect first interpretations. Our eye-tracking study shows that German children are sensitive to both case marking and verb agreement and do revise their incorrect first interpretations of object questions. [1] Friedmann & Novogrodsky (2011). Lingua; [2] Dittmar et al. (2008). Child Development; [3] Metz et al. (2010). New developments in the acquisition of Dutch; [4] De Vincenzi et al. (1999). Proc. of the Eur. Conf. on Cogn. Sci. 27 Prosodic Focus Marking in Child L2 Zenghui Liu, Utrecht University This study investigates prosodic marking of focus in Child L2 speakers of Mandarin, by adopting a semi-spontaneous experimental approach. The child L2 speakers are sequential bilinguals who speak Bai as their native language at home and learned Mandarin at school, and all of them were recruited from a Bai-speaking community in southwest of China. A previous study has shown that Bai is a language marks focus prosodically. However, native speakers of Bai only use duration rather than pitch as the only prosodic cue to signal focus, which is different from Mandarin. Out investigation aims to reveal whether child L2 speakers of Mandarin will mark focus prosodically, and whether they differentiate different focus types. Further, the present study shows specific prosodic cues are used by child L2 speakers of Mandarin, and a developmental path of prosodic focus marking for Child L2 speakers. 28