Brien 1 Jack Brien Mrs. Nicole Varty English 1020, Sec 055 10 November 2015 2015 A New Perspective on Psychology Beginning in the twentieth century, psychology experienced an unprecedented amount of growth as a science and an academic discipline among the scientific community. This rapid growth led to new ideas and concepts explored by many different psychologists, leading to various schools of thought, or “perspectives.” These perspectives have shaped psychology into the far-reaching science it is today, however many of the very perspectives that helped popularize psychology are in danger of being discarded by “mainstream psychology” (Elkins 278, Overskeid 131). This discarding of perspectives may lead to unforeseen and possibly even negative consequences, as there have already been negative impacts on certain perspectives simply by being unpopular (Elkins 278, Koch 629). Although the severity of the situation is not immediately present and noticeable, how can one know what might happen if perspectives such as humanism, cognitivism, psychoanalysis and others like them are left to wither on the vine? With these matters in mind, it makes sense to propose solutions for this looming decision that many psychologists will have to face: whether or not perspectives should be abandoned. The proposal that is most feasible, deployable and researchable is as follows. Psychologists should continue to explore and update perspectives, such as humanism, that have been neglected rather abandon them, because psychology as a whole is a hub which brings various fields and concepts together using these very same perspectives in order to further advance the sciences. Brien 2 Building off of this proposal, a concise and clear solution emerges: continue to explore and experiment with different perspectives, despite that particular school of thought falling out of favor with the mainstream. Of course, these perspectives must be recognized or used by the American Psychological Association (APA) and any work must be peer reviewed as per their standards (Bieling and Hadjistavropoulos 156-157). The reasoning behind this, is that there is always the possibility that one of the newly unpopular perspective might once again be picked up by the mainstream, or vice versa in the case of cognitivism (Overskeid 131). Little can be done to predict the ever-changing field of psychology, especially in an age where rapid advancements in technology often immediately reshape all of science as a whole. There are also the possibilities of breakthroughs, which can happen at any time within any area of study, that fundamentally shift all of psychology, for better or for worse. These breakthroughs are especially influential in psychology, where science is often on the cutting edge of research and development, and the mind and its behavior are still considered to be one of the greatest mysteries within science (Wilke 135). Breakthroughs such as that of the humanistic perspective on psychology, which many saw as a revolution in psychology by adding a new appreciation for the human experience (Bugental 563). Who knows what might have happened if psychology as a whole had ignored humanism as a perspective just because at its conception it wasn’t considered mainstream. Psychology as it is known today might simply not exist, and the advent of humanism is not the only example. How can it possibly be known what modern psychology would be like if John B. Watson had not challenged the popular views of the functionalists and psychoanalysts of his day, for his ideas on behaviorism (Watson 166) would provide a foundation for generations of behaviorists, including many psychologists of today. Brien 3 Despite the solution being a fairly straightforward one, there are others who have in the past and present felt an alternate solution to be much more suitable: do nothing. By simply allowing these perspectives to essentially “die off,” these psychologists and researchers feel that less time is wasted on what is perceived as trivial work. For even with the evidence displayed before, there are others who believe that it is the non-mainstream perspectives that hold psychology back through their smaller size. One of the more famous denouncements of the offshoot perspectives is, ironically enough, B. F. Skinner on humanistic and cognitive psychology. Published in the journal The American Psychologist, “Whatever Happened to Psychology as the Science of Behavior?” by B. F. Skinner was the great behavioral psychologist’s attack against the forces that he believed were holding psychology back: the smaller and newer perspectives. In the paper he expresses his distaste for what he calls “the antiscience stance of humanistic psychology, the practical exigencies of the helping professions, and the cognitive restoration of the royal House of Mind…” (784) and how they have all “worked against the definition of psychology as the science of behavior” (784). He then goes on to say that “Psychology should confine itself to its accessible subject matter and leave the rest of the story of human behavior to physiology” (785). B. F. Skinner’s paper is one of the prime examples of why psychology should not abandon its numerous perspectives. Although his goal was to call for psychology to become a field of study that only concerns itself with the study of human behavior, his arguments of ignoring these less popular perspectives so that psychology could advance would ultimately turn against him. For soon after his paper was published, cognitive psychology quickly was adopted into the mainstream due to its appeal as a branch of psychology rooted more in technology, biology, and chemistry. Behaviorism was quickly left by the wayside, and it would be quite some Brien 4 time until psychologists would feel the perspective warranted serious consideration again (Overskeid 132). Skinner’s prized behaviorism was no longer the dominating perspective, and with its ebb in power also came a decreased interest in his own school of thought, radical behaviorism (Skinner 781). If the argument of doing nothing was followed, it would not be a stretch to assume that radical behaviorism as it exists today wouldn’t be here. Instead, because many behaviorists stuck with the perspective and continued to explore new ideas (Overskeid 134). The concept of this proposal is even further strengthened by humanistic psychology’s findings during this time, for despite its status as outside the mainstream, one of its preeminent psychologists Carl Rogers was nominated for the Nobel Peace Prize in 1987 (Elkins 280). So even though there have been and will be plenty of arguments to do so, it appears that in the past doing nothing with the perspectives has not been proven effective in improving psychology as a whole. Rather it seems that by allowing the continuation of experiments and findings by perspectives, despite their status being outside “mainstream psychology,” psychology as a whole benefits and learns. This is of course all occurring within the past and present standards of credibility as outlined by the American Psychological Association (Bieling and Hadjistavropoulos 156-157). Now that the solution has been presented and compared with others, some might ask, “What are the actual goals of the solution?” By continuing to examine and keep current each perspective, it is the hope that: present perspectives are kept from simply becoming history, psychology does not become a single-perspective science, and growth occurs among new schools of thought. These goals are all realistic as well as attainable. Preventing present perspectives from becoming just history is simple in that as long as psychologists are not discouraged or prevented from studying and researching as they have been in the past (Elkins Brien 5 279, Donlevy 92, Krampen and Schui 61-62, Skinner 784), very few to no perspectives should be lost. Contrary to what some psychologists believe or believed (Overskeid 133, Skinner 784), psychology should not be a one perspective science, as it has been shown that even polar opposite perspectives such as psychoanalysis and humanism can still have some overlap while also covering their own areas (Hansen 24-27), further demonstrating the need for psychology to be diverse as possible. Finally, growth occurring among new schools of thought is probably the easiest of all, as even today perspectives such as positive psychology are slowly growing in popularity (Krampen and Schui 61-63), and one would be hard pressed to find evidence to the contrary. With these goals in mind, next one can move on to the actual steps used to implement the purposed solution to the psychological perspective problem. Once again, the amount of steps as well as their simplicity highlight the overall feasibility of the aforementioned solution. First, the APA should review all of the current schools of thought and deem them worthy or not of being called a perspective according to guidelines set by the organization. Next, using this information, the APA should find and identify which perspectives might be struggling in comparison to their counterparts, such as positive psychology (Krampen and Schui 61-62), and provide any monetary or bodily support. Through these and other programs, the next step is achieved, where abandonment of perspectives is avoided by citing its importance within history and by ensuring that ideas that have been peer reviewed and evaluated are all shared equally, no matter the current trends. The very last step in order to implement this solution is to review how the various schools of thought, both major and minor, are represented in textbooks and academic curriculum, for in order to effectively apply anything at all, consideration for future generations must be some part of the plan. Brien 6 When considering at a solution, probably one of the most important parts is what kind of positive or negative consequences it may have. Looking at the solution of continuing to test and examine with different perspectives despite their favorability (Donlevy 92), rather than avoiding or ignoring them (Skinner 784), it is plain to see the positives. By following the steps outlined beforehand, one of the positives is psychologists, the APA in particular, have a much closer view on each perspective, allowing for a more detailed review of its impact. Another positive is providing psychology with a more unified, and less fragmented image as a science. For if the APA and other psychologists become more open to the ideas of other perspectives that they had previously disregarded, a more unified and self-aware field of psychology emerges. Specifically when implementing the last step from the paragraph above, one of the best positive outcomes is the future interest created by exposing more schools of thought to up and coming psychologists, giving them more opportunities to find their interests and to have access to more knowledge from different perspectives. On top of providing positive outcomes, the solution might also avoid negative ones. As stated previously, with the implementation of the proposition and the following of all the steps outlined, it is possible that abandonment of legitimate perspectives is no longer a concern. Another negative outcome avoided is once again, the suppression of perspectives that aren’t quite as popular by the followers of the more mainstream one. For if every perspective is allowed to exist in its own way within the framework, there is no need for perspectives to feel threatened by other, foreign perspectives. Of course, all of these various steps, positive and negative outcomes, and goals all rely on exactly how feasible this proposal is. Technically speaking, if all the steps were followed and the subsequent policy changes were made, then enforced, and the textbooks and curriculum updated, then it would most likely be a lengthy process. Although it may appear simple on paper, the APA Brien 7 council must meet, then agree on the policy, and then finally implement and enforce it, all of which takes time. There will also more likely than not be opposition to the solution, as demonstrated by the various sources who showed they were against the various smaller perspectives from their onsets (Overskeid 133, Skinner 784). Despite all of this, after a span of a few years, this policy could be working and changing the face of psychology. In the past the APA council has been forced to deal with things quickly, such as the passing of the policy banning all members from any forms of participation in enhanced interrogation techniques in 2015 after only a few weeks of public outcry (Bohannon, news.sciencemag.org). Probably the lengthiest part of the entire implementation would be the new curriculum and text book changes, which are long processes as it stands today, but shouldn’t be seen as a major hindrances. Overall, the policy is fairly feasible and could be implemented relatively quickly if it came to it. The proposal is a straightforward one: Psychologists must be allowed to continue in modernizing and exploring within perspectives that are not considered mainstream instead of abandoning them because when psychology is viewed as a whole, it is seen as a science that unites as well as advances many different concepts and fields together by utilizing those very same perspectives that some deem unnecessary. No longer should the perspectives within psychology fight to be “mainstream.” Rather, they should all work in tandem to achieve new heights in research and ideas, allowing for the science of psychology as a complete discipline to come to greater prominence. Students please be open-minded to the ideas of others, even if you don’t personally believe them valid. Teachers, psychologists and other faculty: consider the points made in this paper by various psychologists throughout history as each of you decides whether this is the correct way of solving this problem. Brien 8 Works Cited Bugental, J. F. T. “The American Psychologist: Humanistic Psychology: A New Breakthrough.” 18 Vol. American Psychological Association, 1963. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Bohannon, John. "APA Overhauling Policies and Leadership after Torture Report." ScienceInsider. AAS, 14 July 2015. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Elkins, David. “Why Humanistic Psychology Lost Its Power and Influence in American Psychology: Implications for Advancing Humanistic Psychology.” Journal of Humanistic Psychology. 2009. 267-91. Web. 18 Oct. 2015. Hadjistavropoulos, Thomas, and Peter J. Bieling. "When Reviews Attack: Ethics, Free Speech, and the Peer Review Process." Canadian Psychology. 2000. 152-9. ProQuest. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Brien 9 Hansen, James T. “Journal of Counseling and Development: Psychoanalysis and Humanism: A Review and Critical Examination of Integrationist Efforts with some Proposed Resolutions.” 78 Vol. American Association for Counseling and Development, 01/2000. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Joseph, Stephen, and P. Alex Linley. “A Positive Psychological Theory of Therapeutic Practice.” Positive Therapy. John Wiley & Sons Inc, 2004. Web. 5 Nov. 2015. Koch, Sigmund. “Psychological Science Versus The Science-Humanism Antinomy: Intimations Of A Significant Science Of Man.” American Psychologist 16.10 (1961): 629-639. PsycARTICLES. Web. 20 Oct. 2015. Overskeid, Geir. "THEY SHOULD HAVE THOUGHT ABOUT THE CONSEQUENCES: THE CRISIS OF COGNITIVISM AND A SECOND CHANCE FOR BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS." The Psychological Record. 2008. 131-51. ProQuest. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Brien 10 Schui, Gabriel. “Health and Well-being: Bibliometric Analyses on the Emergence and Present Growth of Positive Psychology.” 2 Vol. Applied Psychology Blackwell Publishing, 03/2010. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Skinner, B. F. “The American Psychologist: Whatever Happened to Psychology as the Science of Behavior?.” 42 Vol. American Psychological Association, 1987. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Watson, John B. “Psychological Review: Psychology as the Behaviorist Views it.” 20 Vol. American Psychological Association, 1913. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Wertz, F. J. “The Role of the Humanistic Movement in the History of Psychology.” 38 Vol. The Journal of Humanistic Psychology. Sage Publications, 01/1998. Web. 10 Nov. 2015. Wilke, Marko. "The Neuronal Basis of Intelligence: A Riddle, Wrapped in a Mystery?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2007. 172. ProQuest. Web. 10 Nov. 2015.