mec12626-sup-0008-DataS1

advertisement
1
Supporting information for online publication
2
3
Insect collection, rearing and sampling
4
For Experiment 1, a plaster nest containing the colony (brood, workers and the queen) was
5
put into a plastic box. The box was separated from a second plastic box (the foraging area)
6
containing a frozen cricket, sugar water and water tubes. Food and water were supplied twice a
7
week at regular days in the foraging area only. Ants needed to cross from the nest area to the
8
foraging area through a plastic tube 100 cm long to get access to the food. The top of the rearing
9
boxes and the external face of the tubes were covered with Teflon to prevent ants from escaping.
10
We assigned workers to two treatment groups according to the observed behavior at the moment of
11
collection and independently of the age or size. We labeled as “foraging workers” (out) all the
12
individuals that were collected in the foraging area in the process of handling food, water or sugar
13
water, while we labeled as “non-foraging workers” (in) all the ants that were sampled within the
14
nesting chamber in close proximity to the brood pile. Based on previous observations, foraging
15
behavior in fire ants is consistent across time: Mirenda and Vinson (1981) reported that once a
16
worker appeared in the foraging area for the first time then it was seen there regularly until death
17
and Sorensen et al. (1984) observed in a series of experiments that foragers never went back to
18
nursing tasks. These observations are supported by more recent findings in another ant species
19
where different behavioral phenotypes of workers showed strong differences in the spatial location
20
and spatial fidelity (Mersch et al. 2013).
21
For Experiment 2, we prepared two colony fragments from each of 10 colonies. We placed
22
the mother queen in one colony fragment (queenright or QR); the other fragment was queenless
23
(QL). Each fragment was housed in a plastic pencil box containing a small nesting chamber on one
24
side, and a small foraging area on the other side with a frozen cricket, water and sugar water (Figure
25
S1). In all colony fragments we transferred equal amounts of brood (a full 0.5ml plastic tube),
26
workers (5ml) and winged females (virgin queens, ca. 10) from the original colony. After 5 days, we
1
27
collected foraging and non-foraging workers as above from each fragment. We opted for this time
28
window since 5 days seemed a suitable amount of time for behavioral changes to occur: in previous
29
studies it has been observed that 48 hours are required to observe a reduction in the rate of sexual
30
larvae execution by queenless workers (Klobuchar and Deslippe 2002) and it has been shown that
31
the level of workers’ aggression towards conspecifics significantly decreases during the first 5 days
32
post-orphaning (Vander Meer and Alonso 2002).
33
Samples were collected between 1:30pm and 5:00pm for Experiment 1 and between 9:00am
34
and 1:00pm for Experiment 2. Specimens were flash frozen in dry ice and stored at -80 °C until
35
processed for molecular work.
36
37
Sample preparation for molecular analyses
38
Concentration and purity of RNA samples were assessed using NanoDrop (Thermo Scientific,
39
Wilmington, DE) and Qubit 2.0 Fluorometer (Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY) and RNA quality
40
was assessed using RNA Nano Chips on the Agilent 2100 Bioanalyzer (Agilent Technologies, Palo
41
Alto, CA). One µg of each sample was amplified using the Amino Allyl MessageAmp II aRNA
42
Amplification Kit (AM1753, Ambion, Austin, TX). Fifteen µg of aRNA was labeled with Cy3 or Cy5 (GE
43
Health Care, Pittsburgh, PA) and subsequently purified according to the Ambion Kit instructions. 1.5
44
µg of a Cy3 labeled sample were combined with 1.5 µg of a Cy5 labeled sample and fragmented
45
using RNA Fragmentation Reagents (Ambion AM8740, Austin, TX) according to the manufacturer’s
46
instructions. Samples were hybridized with mixing in a MAUI hybridization instrument overnight at
47
42 °C. Arrays were scanned using Axon GenePix 4000B.
48
49
Protocols for microarray analysis
50
Array data were analyzed using two statistical software packages for both experiments. We
51
first analyzed array data using SAS (Cary, NC). Any spots with an intensity of less than 300 (equal to
52
the mean background level on the arrays) were removed from the analyses, as were spots present
2
53
on less than 20 out of 24 arrays: this reduced the number of probes included into the analyses to
54
37,252 for Experiment 1 and 38,511 for Experiment 2. Expression data were log-transformed and
55
normalized using a mixed-model normalization (proc MIXED, SAS, Cary, NC) with the following
56
model:
57
Y = μ + dye + block + array + array*dye + array*block + є
58
where Y is expression, dye and block are fixed effects, and array, array*dye and array*block are
59
random effects. Transcripts with significant expression differences between groups were detected
60
by using a mixed-model ANOVA with the model:
61
Y = μ + treatment + spot + dye + array + є
62
where Y represents the residual from the previous model. Treatment, spot and dye are fixed effects
63
and array is a random effect. In Experiment 1, “treatment” corresponded to the behavioral state of
64
the worker ants (i.e. foraging vs. non-foraging workers) while in Experiment 2 “treatment” included
65
worker behavioral state (foraging vs. non-foraging), queen effect (presence vs. absence) and their
66
interaction. P-values were corrected for multiple testing using a false discovery rate or FDR
67
(Benjamini & Hochberg 1995) of < 0.05 (proc MULTTEST, SAS).
68
The same array data also were analyzed using R 2.11.1 (Team 2009). Background correction
69
(with parameter “normexp+offset=50”) and normalization (“NormalizedWithinArrays” with
70
parameter “loess” followed by “NormalizedBetweenArrays” method = “Aquantile”) were done in the
71
“limma” package for R (Smyth 2004). For both experiments, we used a direct two-colors design to
72
compare gene expression in the different treatment groups. We fit a linear model using the limma
73
function “lmFit” and we created a contrast matrix to test all the pairwise comparisons of interest.
74
For Experiment 1 we set only one pairwise comparison (foraging vs. non-foraging) since there were
75
only two treatment groups. For Experiment 2, the tested pairwise comparisons were the following:
76
QRin vs. QLin, QRout vs. QLout, QRin vs. QRout, QLin vs. QLout. We used the limma function
77
“eBayes” to estimate a “Bayesian moderate t-statistic” (Smyth 2004), setting the FDR at 0.05. To
3
78
correct for multiple testing across contrasts, we used the limma function “decideTests” with
79
method=“separate” and setting the p-value at 0.05.
80
For both experiments, analyses with R detected a higher number of significantly
81
differentially expressed transcripts compared with SAS: 1387 vs. 771 in Experiment 1 (a significant
82
proportion of the differentially expressed transcripts were common to both analyses; see Figure S4)
83
and 395 vs. 4 in Experiment 2. Therefore, we relied on the analyses with R to characterize genes that
84
were differentially expressed and to perform gene ontology analyses. We used analyses with SAS
85
instead to visualize global patterns of gene expression (hierarchical clustering and principal
86
component analyses) and to perform studies of directional overlap in queenless workers, since SAS
87
outputs were more suitable for these specific analyses. The array data were deposited on the
88
ArrayExpress website according to MIAME standards (ArrayExpress ID number will be provided prior
89
to publication).
90
91
Validation of differential expression of candidate genes using quantitative real-time PCR
92
We used total RNA extracted with RNeasy Plus kit (Qiagen, Valencia CA) from pools of 10
93
workers from the same colonies that were used for Experiment 2 and compared gene expression
94
between QRin, QRout and QLin using an ABI Prism 7900 instrument (Foster City, CA). cDNA was
95
synthesized using SuperScript III First-Strand Synthesis System for RT-PCR (Invitrogen-Life
96
Technologies, Carlsbad, CA, USA) and Random Hexamers according to the manufacturer's protocol.
97
The cDNA was then diluted 7 times with ultra-pure water. Amplifications were performed in a 10 μl
98
reaction mixture containing 5 μl of 2X SYBR Green Master Mix (Applied Biosystems-Life
99
Technologies, Carlsbad, CA, USA), 1 μl of each primer (10 μM) and 2 μl of cDNA at the following
100
conditions: 50 °C for 2 min, 95 °C for 10 min, 40 cycles of 95 °C for 15 sec and 60 °C for 1 min, a
101
dissociation step of 95 °C for 15 sec and 60 °C for 15 sec. We used 9 pools of workers for QLin, 8 for
102
QRin and 5 for QRout. Triplicate reactions were performed for each sample and averaged for use in
103
statistical analysis. Expression levels of candidate genes were normalized to the geometric mean of
4
104
two housekeeping genes, Rp-9 and Rp-37 (Wurm et al. 2011). Negative control (cDNA reaction
105
without RT enzyme) was also used to control for potential contamination by genomic DNA. Primer
106
sequences (Table S10) were developed in Primer3Plus (Untergasser et al. 2012) and primer
107
efficiency was first validated using standard curves. Statistical analysis was performed with
108
nonparametric Wilcoxon comparisons for each pair of treatments in JMP 10 (SAS, Cary, NC). The
109
data are shown normalized to the group QRin.
110
Expression patterns of all seven candidate genes were consistent with results from arrays,
111
although expression differences were significant for two genes only (Figure S6). We examined
112
expression of for, which is a major regulator of worker DOL in other social insects: for was slightly
113
upregulated in QRin but expression levels were not significantly different across groups. For immune
114
response, we examined Hym and spirit: both of these genes were downregulated in QRin and
115
expression levels of Hym were significantly different between QRin and QRout (P<0.05) while no
116
significant differences were detected for spirit. Finally, mf (chaeta development), mhc (locomotion),
117
oat (neurogenesis) and syt1 (neurotransmitter secretion) were all upregulated in QRin: of these
118
genes, only oat was significantly differentially expressed between QRin and QRout (P<0.01) and QLin
119
and QRout (P<0.05).
120
121
Comparative studies across species
122
Alaux et al. (2009) found 937 genes differentially expressed between young nurse and old
123
forager worker honey bees from brain tissue. Only 81 genes were in common with our study
124
(Representation factor = 0.7, p < 4.81e-06, significantly less than expected by chance) and 1 GO term:
125
generation of precursor metabolites and energy.
126
Ament et al. (2011) found 1719 genes differentially expressed between nurse and forager
127
worker honey bees from fat body tissue. 185 genes were in common with our study (Representation
128
factor = 0.8, p < 8.73e-06, significantly less than expected by chance) together with 14 GO terms:
129
generation of precursor metabolites and energy, oxidative phosphorylation, mitochondrial electron
5
130
transport, NADH to ubiquinone, energy derivation by oxidation of organic compounds, electron
131
transport chain, respiratory electron transport chain, monocarboxylic acid metabolic process,
132
response to chemical stimulus, ATP synthesis coupled electron transport, mitochondrial ATP
133
synthesis coupled, electron transport, cellular respiration, multi-organism process, response to other
134
organism, oxidation-reduction process.
135
Toth et al. (2010) found 219 genes associated with foraging and provisioning in the
136
primitively eusocial paper wasp Polistes metricus from brain tissue. In this case no GO terms were
137
shared with our study and only 26 genes were in common (Representation factor = 0.4, p < 2.02e -14,
138
significantly less than expected by chance).
139
140
Discussion of genes of interest
141
Genes associated with carbohydrate metabolism included: forkhead box, sub-group O (foxo),
142
a major player in the insulin-signaling pathway which is associated with caste determination in
143
honey bees and to the workers’ transition from nursing to foraging behavior (Wheeler et al. 2006);
144
phosphoglycerate kinase (Pgk) and Triose phosphate isomerase (Tpi), both players in the glycolytic
145
pathway (Celotto et al. 2006; Wang et al. 2004); Hexosaminidase 2 (Hexo2), which plays a role in
146
glycoprotein maturation within the insect secretory pathway (Léonard et al. 2006); and aconitase
147
(Acon), which catalyzes the conversion of citrate to isocitrate in the tricarboxylic acid (TCA) cycle
148
(Cheng et al. 2013). With the exception of Tpi, all these genes were upregulated in foraging workers.
149
Genes associated with lipid storage and metabolism were upregulated in both groups of
150
workers. The following genes were upregulated in non-foraging workers: Indy, which is involved in
151
the transport and storage of Krebs cycle intermediates in tissues and an important player in the
152
regulation of life span via an effect on metabolism (Wang et al. 2009); Lsp2, a storage protein
153
expressed in the fat body tissue of Drosophila (Beneš et al. 1990); Lipid storage droplet-1 and 2 (Lsd-
154
1 and Lsd-2), which respectively regulate lipolysis and a promote lipid storage (Beller et al. 2010);
155
and beta-coatomer protein (betaCop), an essential component of the trafficking machinery cycling
6
156
between the endoplasmic reticulum and Golgi (Beller et al. 2008). Interestingly, expression of Indy
157
was associated with aging and metabolic processes in another study comparing how different social
158
environments affect global gene expression of fire ant queens during colony founding (Manfredini et
159
al. 2013). In both studies, Indy was downregulated in the group where we expected a longer lifespan
160
(non-foraging workers and haplometrotic queens), which is in agreement with the pattern observed
161
in Drosophila. This result suggests that Indy might be an important regulator of aging in S. invicta as
162
well. By contrast, several other noteworthy genes associated with lipid metabolism were
163
upregulated in foraging workers. These included: juvenile hormone epoxide hydrolase 2 (Jheh2),
164
which is involved in juvenile hormone catabolic process; helix loop helix protein 106 (HLH106), a
165
major player in the metabolism of fatty acids (Kunte et al. 2006); and CTP:phosphocholine
166
cytidylyltransferase 1 (CctI), a member of the phospholipid signaling pathways that might affect life
167
span when overexpressed (Landis et al. 2003).
168
We found that S. invicta workers with foraging/non-foraging tasks significantly differed in
169
expression of genes associated with larval central nervous system remodeling and signal
170
transduction. Some of these genes were upregulated in non-foraging workers, including: for, which
171
encodes a cyclic guanosine monophosphate-dependent protein kinase and was previously
172
characterized as a fundamental regulator of foraging behavior across multiple insect species (Ingram
173
et al. 2011; Lucas et al. 2010; Lucas & Sokolowski 2009); syt1, a regulator of neurotransmitter
174
release (Lee et al. 2013); AkhR, a member of the G-protein coupled receptor 1 family (Hewes &
175
Taghert 2001); and finally oat, which is involved in the urea cycle and arginine biosynthesis
176
(Neumüller et al. 2011).
177
Only a handful of genes were differentially expressed between queenright and queenless
178
workers at FDR<0.1. Toy, which is expressed in both eyes and the brain of Drosophila and
179
responsible for axonal outgrowth and differentiation of mushroom bodies (Furukubo-Tokunaga et al.
180
2009) and cuticular proteins 49Aa, which may play a role in chemical recognition among nestmates,
181
were upregulated in queenless workers. Npc2a, which is involved in sterol homeostasis and steroid
7
182
biosynthesis and recently characterized as a player in the antibacterial immune response (Huang et
183
al. 2007; Shi et al. 2012), syt1 and oat (see above) were downregulated in queenless workers.
184
185
REFERENCES
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
Alaux C, Le Conte Y, Adams HA, et al. (2009) Regulation of brain gene expression in honey bees by
brood pheromone. Genes Brain and Behavior 8, 309-319.
Ament SA, Chan QW, Wheeler MM, et al. (2011) Mechanisms of stable lipid loss in a social insect. J.
Exp. Biol. 214, 3808-3821.
Beller M, Bulankina AV, Hsiao H-H, et al. (2010) PERILIPIN-Dependent Control of Lipid Droplet
Structure and Fat Storage in Drosophila. Cell Metabolism 12, 521-532.
Beller M, Sztalryd C, Southall N, et al. (2008) COPI complex is a regulator of lipid homeostasis. PLoS
biology 6, e292.
Beneš H, Edmondson RG, Fink P, et al. (1990) Adult expression of the Drosophila Lsp-2 gene.
Developmental biology 142, 138-146.
Celotto AM, Frank AC, Seigle JL, Palladino MJ (2006) Drosophila model of human inherited
triosephosphate isomerase deficiency glycolytic enzymopathy. Genetics 174, 1237-1246.
Cheng Z, Tsuda M, Kishita Y, Sato Y, Aigaki T (2013) Impaired energy metabolism in a Drosophila
model of mitochondrial aconitase deficiency. Biochemical and biophysical research
communications.
Furukubo-Tokunaga K, Adachi Y, Kurusu M, Walldorf U (2009) Brain patterning defects caused by
mutations of the twin of eyeless gene in Drosophila melanogaster. Fly 3, 263-269.
Hewes RS, Taghert PH (2001) Neuropeptides and neuropeptide receptors in the Drosophila
melanogaster genome. Genome Research 11, 1126-1142.
Huang X, Warren JT, Buchanan JA, Gilbert LI, Scott MP (2007) Drosophila Niemann-Pick Type C-2
genes control sterol homeostasis and steroid biosynthesis: a model of human
neurodegenerative disease. Development 134, 3733-3742.
Ingram KK, Kleeman L, Peteru S (2011) Differential regulation of the foraging gene associated with
task behaviors in harvester ants. BMC ecology 11, 19.
Klobuchar EA, Deslippe RJ (2002) A queen pheromone induces workers to kill sexual larvae in
colonies of the red imported fire ant (Solenopsis invicta). Naturwissenschaften 89, 302-304.
Kunte AS, Matthews KA, Rawson RB (2006) Fatty acid auxotrophy in Drosophila larvae lacking SREBP.
Cell Metabolism 3, 439-448.
Landis GN, Bhole D, Tower J (2003) A search for doxycycline-dependent mutations that increase
Drosophila melanogaster life span identifies the VhaSFD, Sugar baby, filamin, fwd and Cctl
genes. Genome Biol 4, R8.
Lee J, Guan Z, Akbergenova Y, Littleton JT (2013) Genetic Analysis of Synaptotagmin C2 Domain
Specificity in Regulating Spontaneous and Evoked Neurotransmitter Release. The Journal of
Neuroscience 33, 187-200.
Léonard R, Rendić D, Rabouille C, et al. (2006) The Drosophila fused lobes gene encodes an Nacetylglucosaminidase involved in N-glycan processing. Journal of Biological Chemistry 281,
4867-4875.
Lucas C, Hughson BN, Sokolowski MB (2010) Job switching in ants: Role of a kinase. Communicative
& integrative biology 3, 6-8.
Lucas C, Sokolowski MB (2009) Molecular basis for changes in behavioral state in ant social
behaviors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 6351-6356.
Manfredini F, Riba-Grognuz O, Wurm Y, et al. (2013) Sociogenomics of Cooperation and Conflict
during Colony Founding in the Fire Ant Solenopsis invicta. PLoS genetics 9, e1003633.
8
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
Mersch DP, Crespi A, Keller L (2013) Tracking Individuals Shows Spatial Fidelity Is a Key Regulator of
Ant Social Organization. Science 340, 1090-1093.
Mirenda JT, Vinson SB (1981) Division of labour and specification of castes in the red imported fire
ant Solenopsis invicta buren. Animal Behaviour 29, 410-420.
Neumüller RA, Richter C, Fischer A, et al. (2011) Genome-Wide Analysis of Self-Renewal in
Drosophila Neural Stem Cells by Transgenic RNAi. Cell stem cell 8, 580-593.
Shi XZ, Zhong X, Yu XQ (2012) Drosophila melanogaster NPC2 proteins bind bacterial cell wall
components and may function in immune signal pathways. Insect Biochemistry and
Molecular Biology 42, 545-556.
Smyth GK (2004) Linear models and empirical bayes methods for assessing differential expression in
microarray experiments. Statistical applications in genetics and molecular biology 3, 3.
Sorensen A, Busch T, Vinson S (1984) Behavioral flexibility of temporal subcastes in the fire ant,
Solenopsis invicta, in response to food. Psyche 91, 319-332.
Team RDC (2009) R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R foundation for
statistical computing, Vienna, Austria.
Toth AL, Varala K, Henshaw MT, et al. (2010) Brain transcriptomic analysis in paper wasps identifies
genes associated with behaviour across social insect lineages. Proceedings of the Royal
Society B-Biological Sciences 277, 2139-2148.
Untergasser A, Cutcutache I, Koressaar T, et al. (2012) Primer3—new capabilities and interfaces.
Nucleic acids research 40, e115-e115.
Vander Meer RK, Alonso LE (2002) Queen primer pheromone affects conspecific fire ant (Solenopsis
invicta) aggression. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 51, 122-130.
Wang P, Saraswati S, Guan Z, et al. (2004) A Drosophila temperature-sensitive seizure mutant in
phosphoglycerate kinase disrupts ATP generation and alters synaptic function. The Journal of
Neuroscience 24, 4518-4529.
Wang PY, Neretti N, Whitaker R, et al. (2009) Long-lived Indy and calorie restriction interact to
extend life span. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America 106, 9262-9267.
Wheeler DE, Buck N, Evans JD (2006) Expression of insulin pathway genes during the period of caste
determination in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Insect Molecular Biology 15, 597-602.
Wurm Y, Wang J, Riba-Grognuz O, et al. (2011) The genome of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 56795684.
9
Download