Chapter 6 Cognition Piaget’s Theory and Infancy Piaget’s theory: described the child as actively constructing and building knowledge (schemes) through the processes of assimilation and accommodation to achieve equilibrium in understanding. Piaget believed that cognitive development results from maturational factors an d environmental experiences. four stages through which all children progress in an invariable order. The first stage, the sensorimotor stage (birth to two years), is characterized by the child’s actions on the environment. The child undergoes three major achievements during this initial stage (which contains six substages). The first accomplishment involves a progression from actions that are reflexive to more goal-directed actions called means-end behavior A second accomplishment involves the child’s gradual changing focus from the self to a greater orientation to the external world. Of importance is the attainment of the object concept, or object permanence. Possession of the object concept (or object permanence) is necessary for the development of deferred imitation, the ability to imitate a model who is no longer present, and marks the end of the sensorimotor stage and the beginning of the preoperational stage. Critics of Piaget’s theory suggest that children demonstrate object permanence much earlier than Piaget thought. Concepts The child’s use of concepts, or the way in which the child organizes information on the basis of some general or abstract principle, increases the efficiency of cognitive processing. Classification classes can be based on perceptual groupings (objects that look alike), thematic relations (objects that function together or complement one another), or taxonomic groupings (based on some abstract principle). Children’s earliest classifications (before about nine to twelve months) appear to be largely perceptually based. This reliance on shared perceptual features decreases with age, particularly as children begin to understand hierarchical relations among objects that are perceptually dissimilar. Initial groupings of objects tend to occur at a basic level: objects go together when they look alike or are used in similar ways. Numerical concepts-- newborns and very young infants may be able to detect differences between small numbers of objects. Memory types of memory. Recognition memory requires participants to indicate somehow that they have experienced a stimulus before. Recall memory participants must reproduce previously presented stimuli. Explicit memory refers to a recollection of a past event or experience. Explicit memory is a conscious process and can be demonstrated via either recognition or recall. Implicit Memory refers to non-conscious recollections of how to do something behaviorally. The lack of language presents difficulties in obtaining research evidence regarding infants’ memory. Researchers have utilized two techniques, habituation and operant conditioning to measure recognition memory. Fagan’s paired-comparison procedure using human faces demonstrates that infants retain information about previously viewed faces for surprisingly long periods of time. Rovee-Collier’s experiment with the model and ribbon tied to the infant’s leg relies on operant conditioning to demonstrate memory. The infant remembers the mobile, and how to make it spin by pulling the ribbon. Recall memory is measured through techniques such as deferred imitation, and elicited imitation, in which the subject repeats a sequence of actions demonstrated by the experimenter. As infants mature, they are able to separate memory from context, process information more rapidly, and remember a greater amount of information. Problem-Solving Piaget’s descriptions of the development of means-ends behavior during the sensorimotor stage of development suggest that infants show the beginnings of problem-solving. Infants are capable of solving problems by combining several subgoals to reach an interesting toy. Problem-solving can even stake on complex qualities. In some circumstances, very young children can discern the similarities across problems and transfer that knowledge from one problem to another. Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory of cognitive development Vygotsky, in contrast to Piaget, emphasized that development must be understood within the context of the culture in which a child is reared. The social activity surrounding formal and informal exchanges with others plays a significant role in development. Scaffolding Others provide a scaffolding for cognitive development—that is, temporary support by demonstrating cognitive skills and techniques in which the child is deficient and that the child eventually incorporates as part of her or his own thinking. The zone of proximal development stresses that the most effective help the child can receive from an expert is assistance just slightly beyond his or her capacities, thereby building on the child’s current level of competence. The role of skilled collaborators Research indicates that in general, when children work with a skilled collaborator (whether an adult or a peer), performance on cognitive tasks improves. An important component in effective collaboration is shared attention and communication, known as intersubjectivity. Infants show the early beginnings of participating in shared attention and communication in the first few months of life as demonstrated through simple routines, such as the game of peek -aboo. Language A baby’s contact with language is one-sided. Among the first tasks is to learn to identify the myriad sounds of the native language. These tasks involve distinguishing specific sounds in the spoken language, noting the regularities in how they are combined, recognizing which combinations constitute words, and eventually, after making the transaction from listener to speaker, forming the consonant-vowel combinations that build words and sentences. Phonology The first task of the newborn infant is to establish phonological skills in order to receive and produce messages. Very young infants can discriminate among different phonemes categorically and are sensitive to the prosody of the language, or its patterns of intonation, stress, and rhythm that communicate meaning. From an early age, infants can detect differences in language. By six to eight weeks, the child begins to produce cooing sounds, vowel-like utterances occasionally accompanied by consonants. At about three to six months of age, the child produces consonant-vowel combinations (babbling), and shortly thereafter vocalizations feature canonical babbling. The discovery of cultural differences in babbling and differences in the preverbal utterances of deaf and normal-hearing infants suggests that prelinguistic utterances are influenced by environmental as well as biological factors. First Words Typically, children say their first words at one year. The child’s comprehension and production of words also signal a new focus in the mastery of language: semantic development. Children begin to speak one word at a time at 12–20 months. Children’s first words are mostly labels for objects, people, or events. At about eighteen months, most children show a vocabulary spurt. Many of the child’s first words are bound to a specific context---child applies a word to a narrower class of objects than the word signifies. This type of error is called underextension. Another type of error, called overextension, occurs when the child applies a word to a broader category than the word signifies. Children’s comprehension of language, or receptive language, far exceeds their productive language. In general, children show common trends in the way they acquire language. But children vary in terms of the age of the first word uttered, whether or not a vocabulary spurt occurs, and whether their one-word speech displays a referential style (mostly object words) or an expressive style (words that direct the behaviors of others). Individual differences may result from differences in neurological structures that control language or from inborn differences in temperament. Another possibility is that parents influence the rate and form of children’s vocabulary development. Cultural differences can be found in how children speak, even during the one-word stage, bolstering the idea that what children hear others say influences what they themselves say. Children in Korea and China use more verbs, mothers from both Asian groups pepper their speech with many more verbs and action sequences than mothers in the US. Atypical development occurs when children deviate noticeably from developmental milestones. Approximately five percent of children under age three display developmental delays. Assessment tools include the Denver Developmental Screening Test, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, and an assortment of questionnaires asking parents to report their children’s behaviors. The Bayley Scales of Infant Development is widely used to predict later childhood competence. The test consists of two scales, Mental and Motor, as well as a Behavior Rating Scale to assess the infant’s interests, emotions, and general level of activity compared to the standardization sample. Intervention programs can be developed with a multidisciplinary team to help the child achieve the most positive outcomes. Language in the Context of Social Interactions Many researchers hold a central tenet that language is a social activity. They acknowledge that there may be innate predispositions to learn language but emphasize the role that experiences with more mature, expert speakers play in fostering linguistic skill. Parents have a unique way of talking to their young children. Parents present a version of the spoken language that contains simple, well-formed sentences and is punctuated by exaggerated intonation, high pitch, and clear pauses between segments. Caregivers describe concrete events taking place presently, and model turn taking. Caregivers also elaborate, expand and explain. Two general principles operate during caregiver-child interactions. Parents interpret infants’ behaviors as attempts to communicate. Second, children actively seek relationships among objects, events, and people in their world and the vocal behaviors of the caregivers. Parentese (motherese) may serve a number of functions in the child’s growing competence with language: assisting the child’s acquisition of word meaning, facilitating the acquisition of phonology and syntax, and providing lessons in conversational turn taking. The more mothers talk with their children, the more words the children acquire. It is not just how much talk takes place but the type of talk that also matters. Motherese is also not a universal phenomenon. Cultural differences exist both in the type of talk and in the amount of conversation directed at infants. Linguistic exchanges with other interaction partners may uniquely influence the child’s linguistic skill. When communicating with fathers, for instance, children are challenged to make adjustments to maintain the interaction. Children learn language by overhearing it on television, in conversations between mothers and older siblings or even between two strangers. Research Applied to Parenting: Reading to Children Speech tends to be particularly diverse, expressive, and socially interactive when mothers read to their children. A program of dialogic reading presents advice to parents on how to read more effectively to young children. The program suggests that parents ask questions that stimulate the child to speak, follow up with further questions, recast the child’s utterances, model answers, and provide praise and social support in a gamelike atmosphere. Chapter 6 PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT: THE BRAIN, BODY, MOTOR SKILLS, AND SEXUAL DEVELOPMENT AN OVERVIEW OF MATURATION AND DEVELOPMENT Changes in Height and Weight Rapid increase in height and weight over first two years Growth is more gradual during middle childhood Puberty (early adolescence) there is another rapid growth spurt Changes in Body Proportions Cephalocaudal – head downward growth Proximodistal – center outward growth Skeletal Development Bones lengthen, thicken, and harden with age completing most growth by 18 Skull has fontanelles Muscular Development Born with all muscle fibers adolescence Variations in Physical Development Structures develop at different rates Individuals develop at different rates Cultural variations also exist DEVELOPMENT OF THE BRAIN Neural Development and Plasticity Last three prenatal months and first two years of life = brain growth spurt Neurons – basic unit of brain/nervous system; most present at birth Form synapses (connective spaces) with other neurons Glia – nourish neurons and encase them in myelin; form throughout life Neural Development: Cell Differentiation and Synaptogenesis Neurons can serve any neural function Synaptogenesis – formation of synapses More formed than needed Roughly half die; some stand in reserve – injury or new skills Placticity – cells are responsive to experience (allows change in brain) Neural Plasticity: The Role of Experience Increased head circumference in children from high SES homes Brain Differentiation and Growth At birth, brain associated with biological functioning is most developed Cerebrum and cerebral cortex (higher brain centers) Primary motor areas; primary sensory areas mature first Myelinization At birth – pathways between sense organs and brain are myelinated Pathways between muscles and brain are next Reticular formation and frontal cortex Needed for long term concentration Cerebral Lateralization Cerebrum consists of two hemispheres connected by the corpus callosum Each covered by a cerebral cortex Left – right side of body; speech, hearing, positive emotions Right – left side of body; music, touch, negative emotions Lateralization increases with age MOTOR DEVELOPMENT Sequence of motor skills is the same for all infants Basic Trends in Locomotor Development Cephalocaudal – head-downward Proximodistal – center-outward The Maturational Viewpoint Motor development is a genetically programmed sequence of events The Experiential/Practice Hypothesis Maturation and practice are important Dynamical Systems Theory New skills are constructed as infants actively reorganize existing capabilities Desire is to achieve a goal Fine Motor Development Development of Voluntary Reaching Prereaching is replaced by voluntary reaching Development of Manipulatory Skills Claw-like ulnar grasp is replaced by the pincer grasp (near end of first year) Psychological Implications of Early Motor Development Motor skills allow for fun social interaction Provide evidence of normal development Enhances perceptual, cognitive, and social development Beyond Infancy: Motor Development in Childhood and Adolescence Each year, skills improve Young children tend to overestimate physical feats they can perform. Puberty – males increase in muscle development; females tend to become less active PUBERTY: THE PHYSICAL TRANSITION FROM CHILD TO ADULT The Adolescent Growth Spurt Increase in height and weight Bodies and faces appear more adultlike Sexual Maturation – Timed with growth spurt, predictable pattern Girls Onset of breast and pubic hair development Widening of hips, enlarging of uterus, and vagina Menarche (first menstruation) Completion of breast growth Boys Development of testes and scrotum Emergence of pubic hair Growth of penis, sperm production Appearance of facial hair Lowering of voice Large individual differences in timing Secular Trends: Are We Maturing Earlier? Reaching sexual maturity earlier in industrialized societies than in the past Also growing taller and heavier THE PSYCHOLOGICAL IMPACTS OF PUBERTY Girls Hope to be attractive, not too tall Increasingly negative body images Reactions to menarche are mixed Boys Better body images than girls Mixed reactions to becoming sexually mature Adolescent Body Image and Unhealthy Weight Control Strategies Positive body image: Body image dissatisfaction: Boys Either want to lose weight, or become more muscular If average weight, tend to be happy Girls – compelled to be thin Family factors influencing weight behaviors Chaotic home environment High conflict levels; low emotionality Intrapersonal factors Internalize stress Emotionally unexpressive Behavioral factors Participation in certain sports Dieting; perfectionism Social Impacts of Puberty Nonindustrialized societies Puberty is a sign of adulthood Other societies May be an increase in parent-child conflict, but culture and environment influence the experience of adolescence Does Timing of Puberty Matter? Possible Impacts on Boys Early maturing is beneficial Possible Impacts on Girls Early maturing is a disadvantage Adolescent Sexuality Hormonal changes increase sex drive Culture influences sexuality Education on sexual matters Preparation for role as a sexual being Sexual Explorations, Attitudes, Behaviors Sexuality and the Internet Sexual Attitudes Sexual Behavior Personal and Social Consequences of Sex Factors associated with early sexual involvement Failure to use contraception Sexually Transmitted Disease Teenage Pregnancy and Childbearing Consequences for adolescent mothers 50% drop out of school Consequences for babies of adolescents Intellectual deficits; emotional disturbances; poor peer relations More delinquent behaviors Dealing with the Problem of Teen Sexuality Begin at home with communication Teen Outreach program Formal sex ed Abstinence Information about contraceptives, Information about resisting pressure CAUSES AND CORRELATES OF PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT Biological Mechanisms Effects of Individual Genotypes Set limits for height and rate of maturation Hormonal Influences - Endocrinology of Growth Pituitary gland releases growth hormome, stimulates production of estrogen, testosterone, and androgen Environmental Influences Lack of Nutrition Marasmus – not enough calories or protein; small, impaired social and intellectual development Kwashiorkor – not enough protein; fluid collects in face, legs, and abdomen Vitamin and mineral deficiencies Zinc and iron Excessive Nutrition Obesity Illness If adequately nourished, most do not affect growth long term Emotional Stress and Lack of Affection Nonorganic failure to thrive – before 18 months of age Deprivation dwarfism – 2-15 years of age