The Mechanisms Underlying Incubation in Problem Solving Ut Na Sio Doctor of Philosophy Department of Psychology Lancaster University March, 2010 i “the perfect uselessness of knowing the answer to the wrong question” (Ursula Kroeber Le Guin, The Left Hand of Darkness, p.57, 1969) ii ABSTRACT This thesis investigated the mechanisms underlying incubation. Three unconscious processes have been proposed to explain incubation effects: spreading activation, that is, distributing activation to relevant memories via associative networks; selective forgetting, that is, suppressing inappropriate strategies/concepts; and cue assimilation, that is, re-encoding a problem in a form that facilitates the assimilation of chance-encountered cues. The heterogeneity of settings and the insensitivity of measurements of the incubation studies have to date made it impossible to draw any conclusions on the role of incubation. This thesis addressed these methodological problems, initially by carrying out a meta-analysis to synthesize past findings, and in experimental studies by supplementing the traditional paradigm with an additional measurement (sensitivity to relevant and misleading items), to test between the three hypotheses. The meta-analysis identified that incubation is susceptible to numerous procedural moderators. Longer preparation periods gave a stronger incubation effect for visual problems. Low-load tasks yielded a stronger incubation effect than rest during an incubation period when solving linguistic problems. Experiments with linguistic problems revealed that cue-assimilation occurred when solving problems at an intermediate level of difficulty and when the incubation period was filled with low-load tasks. This supported the cue-assimilation hypothesis and confirmed the meta-analysis findings, and suggested an additional moderator: problem difficulty. Sensitivity to relevant and misleading items was not influenced by incubation. The spreading-activation and selective-forgetting hypotheses were not supported. An experiment using visual problems, examining the impact of preparation length and attentional allocation style (focused vs. diffuse), revealed a positive correlation between iii preparation length and incubation effect size. Controlling individuals’ attentional allocation styles reduced the variance in incubation effect sizes. Together, the meta-analysis and the experimental studies support a positive incubation effect, and identify procedural and cognitive moderators of the effect. The absence of knowledge-activation during the incubation period suggests that incubation effects may rely on unconscious meta-cognitive processes. iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to take this moment to thank a number of people. They have made my years in Lancaster University not merely a study program but an education. First, I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisor, Prof. Tom Ormerod. His invaluable advice and support have guided me to successfully complete this thesis. I am also grateful to my second supervisor, Dr. Linden Ball, for being so ready and willing to help on all occasions. I would like to thank the Psychology Department, Lancaster University, for the financial support. My thanks also go to my friends, spending time with them is the best incubation period for my PhD study. Without them, I would have been defeated by Britain’s gloomy weather and my self-doubts. At last, I must thank my family for loving me enough to let me go. v DECLARATION I declare that the thesis hereby submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at Lancaster University is my own work and has not been previously submitted in substantially the same form for the award of a higher degree elsewhere. vi PUBLISHED MANUSCRIPTS Sio, U. N. & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Mechanisms underlying incubation in problem-solving: Evidence for unconscious cue assimilation. In N.A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society. Sio, U. N. & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94-120. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT ..................................................................................................................... iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................................ v DECLARATION.............................................................................................................. vi PUBLISHED MANUSCRIPTS...................................................................................... vii CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................... 7 CHAPTER II: LITERATURE REVIEW ........................................................................ 11 An Information Processing Model of General Problem Solving ................................ 11 The Nature of Insight .................................................................................................. 15 Proposed Mechanisms of Incubation ...................................................................... 23 Procedural Moderators of the Incubation Effect ..................................................... 26 Attentional Resource Allocation Styles and Incubation ......................................... 32 Methodological Problems of Past Incubation Studies ............................................ 33 CHAPTER III: THE IMPACT OF PROCEDURAL MODERATORS ON INCUBATION EFFECTS: META-ANALYSIS............................................................. 36 Method .................................................................................................................... 37 Results ..................................................................................................................... 45 Discussion ............................................................................................................... 64 CHAPTER IV: EXPERIMENTS I, II, and III - LINGUISTIC INSIGHT PROBLEMS 73 EXPERIMENT I ......................................................................................................... 74 Method .................................................................................................................... 75 Results ..................................................................................................................... 80 Discussion ............................................................................................................... 89 EXPERIMENT II ........................................................................................................ 91 1 Method .................................................................................................................... 92 Results ..................................................................................................................... 94 Discussion ............................................................................................................. 109 EXPERIMENT III .................................................................................................... 116 Method .................................................................................................................. 117 Results ................................................................................................................... 121 Discussion ............................................................................................................. 133 DISCUSSION: EXPERIMENT I, II, and III ............................................................ 138 CHAPTER V: EXPERIMENT IV-VISUAL INSIGHT PROBLEMS .......................... 144 Method .................................................................................................................. 147 Results ................................................................................................................... 152 Discussion ............................................................................................................. 165 CHAPTER VI: GENERAL DISCUSSION .................................................................. 172 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................. 181 Appendix A ................................................................................................................... 195 Appendix B ................................................................................................................... 203 Appendix C ................................................................................................................... 227 Appendix D ................................................................................................................... 243 Appendix E ................................................................................................................... 246 Appendix F.................................................................................................................... 248 Appendix G ................................................................................................................... 249 Appendix H ................................................................................................................... 250 Appendix I .................................................................................................................... 253 2 LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES Table 3.1 Coding System Used In The Meta-Analysis .............................................. 40 Table 3.2 Stem-And-Leaf Display Of 117 Unbiased Effect Sizes ............................. 46 Table 3.3 The Random Variance Component, Weighted Mean, Standard Deviation, Standard Error, And 95% Confidence Interval of the Effect Size Estimate by Each Categorical Moderator ................................................................. 50 Table 3.4 Regression Model for Linguistic Problem Studies (N= 65) ...................... 51 Table 3.5 Regression Model for Misleading Cue Studies (N= 85) ............................ 52 Table 3.6 Regression Model on No Relevant Cue Studies (N = 82) ......................... 53 Table 3.7 Regression Model on High Cognitive-Load Incubation Task Studies (N= 75) ................................................................................................................... 54 Table 3.8 Regression Model on All Studies (N= 114) ............................................... 56 Table 3.9 Regression Model on All Studies (with Interactive Terms, N= 114) ......... 58 Table 3.10 Summary of Coefficient Differences of the Regression Model on All Studies (with Interactive Terms, N= 114) .................................................. 60 Table 3.11 Regression Model for Studies Excluding Creative Problem (N = 100) ..... 62 Table 3.12 The Summary of Coefficient Differences of the Regression Model Excluding Creative Problem (N= 100) ...................................................... 63 Table 4.1.1 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Latency at the First Attempt by Question Set and Condition .... 81 Table 4.1.2 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Two Attempts and Improvement Score by Condition ............................... 83 Table 4.1.3 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Cue and No-Cue Trials in Each Condition. ............................................... 84 3 Table 4.1.4 Means and Standard Deviations of Improvement Score on the Cue and No-Cue Trials, and the Score Differences by Condition ........................... 85 Table 4.1.5 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Condition and Cue on Improvement Scores. ................................................................................. 85 Table 4.1.6 Means and Standard Deviations of the Lexical Decision Times for Target Words and Neutral Words .......................................................................... 88 Table 4.1.7 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Condition and Word Type on Lexical Decision Time ............................................................................... 89 Table 4.2.1 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt in Experiment I and II ...................... 95 Table 4.2.2 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt by RAT Difficulty and Condition ..... 96 Table 4.2.3 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Two Attempts by Condition and RAT Difficulty Level ............................. 98 Table 4.2.4 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of the RAT Difficulty and Condition on Improvement Scores ............................................................ 99 Table 4.2.5 Means and Standard Deviations of Improvement Scores in the Presence and Absence Of Cue, and the Score Difference by Condition and RAT Difficulty. ................................................................................................. 101 Table 4.2.6 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of the RAT Difficulty, Condition, and Cue on the Number of Correct Responses ........................................ 103 Table 4.2.7 Means and Standard Deviations of the Natural and Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Times for Target Words and Neutral Words. ............... 107 Table 4.2.8 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of RAT Difficulty, Condition,and Word Type on Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time ........................ 108 4 Table 4.3.1 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt by Rebus Type and Condition ........ 122 Table 4.3.2 Mean and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Two Attempts by Condition and Rebus Type .......................................... 124 Table 4.3.3 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Rebus Type and Condition, on Improvement Scores ................................................................................ 125 Table 4.3.4 Mean and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Cue and No-Cue Trials in Each Condition. ............................................. 126 Table 4.3.5 Means and Standard Deviations of Improvement Score on the Cue and No-Cue Trials, and the Cueing Effect, by Condition and Rebus Type .... 127 Table 4.3.6 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Condition and Cue, and Rebus Type on Improvement Scores. ................................................................. 128 Table 4.3.7 Means and Standard Deviations of the Original and Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Times for Target Words and Neutral Words ................ 130 Table 4.3.8 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of RAT Difficulty, Condition, and Word Type on Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time ........................ 131 Table 4.3.9 Means and Standard Deviations of the Original and Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Times for Misleading Hints and Neutral Words. ......... 132 Table 4.3.10 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of the Condition and Word Type on Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time................................................ 133 Table 5.1 Ethnic and Gender Distribution, and Means and Standard Deviations of the GEFT Scores by Condition. ..................................................................... 153 Table 5.2 Solution Rate of the Visual Insight Problems (N = 32) ........................... 155 Table 5.3 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Insight Problem Solved In The Pre and Post-Incubation Period in the IS and IL Conditions, and in the Equivalent Time Period in the NI Condition. .................................... 156 5 Table 5.4 Summary of the Results of the Complete and the Backward Eliminated Model ....................................................................................................... 158 Table 5.5 Summary of Results of the Two Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses ................................................................................................................. 162 Table D1 Regression Coefficients in Original and Transformed Regression Models ................................................................................................................. 245 Table E1 Regression Coefficients in Original and Transformed Regression Models ................................................................................................................. 247 Figure 3.1. Funnel Plot of the Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis. ..................... 48 Figure 4.1.1 Task Presentation Sequence in Each Trial in Each Condition in Experiment I. ........................................................................................... 79 Figure 5.1 Task Presentation Order in Each Trial in Experiment IV. ...................... 151 6 CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION Anecdotal reports of the intellectual discovery processes of individuals hailed as geniuses (e.g., Ghiselin, 1985; Wallas, 1926; Woodworth & Schlosberg, 1954;) share a common theme: a flash of insight pops unexpectedly into the mind of the individual after he or she has put an unsolved problem aside for a period of time, having failed in initial attempts to solve it. This temporary shift away from an unsolved problem that allows a solution seemingly to emerge as if from no additional effort is termed an ‘incubation period’ (Wallas, 1926). Its importance in current thinking and practice is illustrated by a recent search in Google Scholar for the term incubation along with either creativity, insight, or problem which yielded 12,300 articles, with search restricted to the years 1997 to 2009 and the subject areas of Social Sciences, Arts and Humanities. An additional 3,300 articles were yielded when the subject areas Business Administration, and Economics were included. Yet there are many conflicting account of incubation, with some studies reporting a strong effect and other failing to find any effect at all. This thesis aims to help resolve the uncertainties surrounding the phenomenon by describing a statistical meta-analysis review of existing empirical studies of incubation and by reporting a series of incubation experiments using novel paradigms. One theoretical reason for studying incubation is because it is closely associated with insightful thinking. Indeed, Wallas (1926) proposed incubation as the second of five phases in problem-solving (the others being preparation, intimation, illumination, and verification). Insight may be characterized as a sudden, unpredictable and non-verbalizable solution discovery (e.g., Metcalfe & Weibe, 1987). Some researchers see the apparently unconscious nature of solution discovery as evidence that the processes required to achieve insight in problem-solving are qualitatively different from those used to tackle problems that do not require insight(e.g., Jung-Beeman & Bowden, 7 2000; Wertheimer, 1985). Incubation might serve a valuable role in arbitrating between theories of insight, in particular between special-process theories (e.g., Knoblich, Ohlsson, Haider, & Reinhus, 1999; Ohlsson, 1992) and theories of insight as a normal problem-solving processes (e.g., MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001). Understanding the role of incubation periods also allows us to make use of them effectively to promote creativity in areas such as individual problem-solving, classroom learning, and work environments. Educational researchers have tried to introduce incubation periods in classroom activity, and positive incubation effects in fostering students’ creativity have been reported (Lynch & Swink, 1967; Medd & Houtz, 2002; Rae, 1997; Webster, Campbell, & Jane, 2006). However, in the absence of a comprehensive theory or model that can explain how and why positive incubation effects might emerge and under what conditions they are best fostered, no general pedagogic recommendations can be made. Two streams of mechanisms have been proposed to account for the incubation effect. On the one hand, some researchers suggest that incubation periods provide additional time for individuals to work on the problem covertly (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Posner, 1973). In contrast to this conscious-work view is the proposal that performing irrelevant incubation tasks facilitates the occurrence of some helpful unconscious problem solving processes, such as spreading activation, selective forgetting, and cue assimilation (Bower, Regehr, Balthazard, & Parker, 1990; Simon, 1966; Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991; Seifert, Meyer, Davidson, Patalano, & Yaniv, 1995; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). However, the diversity of the findings makes it very difficult to draw any conclusions on the role of incubation. Some of the past studies have shown that introducing an incubation period can improve individuals’ performance on insight problem solving (Dominowski & Jenrick, 1972; Fulgosi & Guilford, 1968; Patrick, 1986; Smith & Blankenship, 1989, 1991). However, an incubation effect can disappear after a slight change in the 8 experimental setting (Fulgosi & Guildford, 1968; Patrick, 1986). Some researchers have even failed to replicate the results of past studies under the same experimental setting (Olton & Johnson, 1976). One possible reason for the conflicting findings is that there are many moderators affecting incubation, and past studies have failed to control or adjust them to an optimal level that fosters incubation. Another methodological problem of past incubation studies is the insensitivity of the measures used to assess incubation. The traditional method used in assessing incubation in past studies was perhaps too general, in that researchers only compared individuals’ problem solving performance between incubation and control conditions. Although a noticeable post-incubation performance improvement could support the positive impact of incubation periods, it cannot show whether the improvement is the result of the occurrence of a specific conscious or unconscious process. Because of these methodological shortcomings, the nature of incubation remains unknown. There were two objectives of this thesis. The first was to identify key moderators affecting incubation effects. The second was to test the occurrence of three proposed unconscious processes underlying incubation effects: spreading-activation, selective-forgetting, and cue-assimilation. To achieve these two objectives, a meta-analysis, which is a systematic quantitative review on past studies, was first carried out to identify the potential procedural moderators on incubation effects. Experiments I, II, III, and IV were then carried out to examine the impact of the potential moderators on the incubation effect and verify the proposed mechanisms of incubation. Experiments I, II, and III examined the occurrence of three proposed unconscious cognitive processes and the optimal experimental settings for the occurrence of these processes. In order to offer strong data to test the unconscious cognitive processes, incubation effects on both problem solving performance and sensitivity to relevant and irrelevant items were assessed in Experiment I, II, and III. Experiment IV shifted to investigate the impact of a 9 new cognitive moderator on incubation effects: attentional allocation style. This thesis includes six chapters and is organized as follows. Chapter II is devoted to the review of literature on insight problem-solving and incubation effects. Chapter III presents a meta-analysis for identifying the potential procedural moderators. Chapter IV presents the settings and findings of Experiments I, II, and III. Chapter V presents the details of Experiment IV and the results found. Chapter VI presents a general discussion of the findings of the meta-analysis and the experiments. 10 CHAPTER II: LITERATURE REVIEW This chapter first presents a brief discussion of an information-processing model of general problem solving. Following this, two major theories of insight problem solving are described. The last section reviews research relevant to incubation effects in insight problem solving, and discusses the potential moderators of the incubation effect as well as methodological problems of the past studies. These moderators and methodological issues provide the rationale for conducting the meta-analysis and the experiments reported in the remainder of this thesis. An Information Processing Model of General Problem Solving Newell and Simon (1972) suggested that, although problems differ in their degree of difficulty and contextual content, the mechanisms underlying different types of complex problem-solving experiences are qualitatively the same and can be decomposed into a series of basic cognitive processes. Newell and Simon put forward a general information-processing model to explain human problem-solving behaviors. To solve a problem, individuals have first to encode the information provided in the task environment and construct an internal representation of the problem. The basic elements of the internal representation are the initial and the goal states of the problem, the moves that can transform the initial state to the goal state, and the constraints under which moves can be applied. The problem state is the given situation of the problem, and the goal state is the final state to that problem, which problem solvers try to achieve. In some cases, the task environment may not provide enough information for knowing the initial state and the goal state of the problem. Individuals may then have to make use of their task-related knowledge in hypothesising these two problem states. Also, in order 11 increase the search efficiency, individuals usually will set up some constraints to filter out some seemingly irrelevant or illegal moves. Once the problem states are defined, solving a problem involves selecting and executing a sequence of moves that can transform the initial state into the goal state in accord with the problem constraints. The execution of each single move creates a new problem state (current state), which will be added into the representation. Therefore, the mental representation of a problem is dynamic rather than static during the problem-solving process. Individuals can estimate how close they are to the goal by calculating the difference between the current state and the goal state. There is usually more than one possible move, and individuals do not randomly select one to execute. Instead, individuals will apply heuristics, which are strategies that help an individual to evaluate the effectiveness of the available moves in the search space, to select the most effective move. The commonly-used heuristics include means-end analysis and hill-climbing, which aim to find the moves that can maximally reduce the difference between the current state and the goal state. This search process is halted once the goal state is reached or an impasse is reached, in which none of the available moves can apparently transform the current state to the goal state. In the latter case, the search process is resumed after the internal representation is reformulated into a better form (e.g., by releasing some unnecessary constraints or by re-encoding the initial and goal states), or after a switch in search strategy. In summary, there are two main stages in Newell and Simon’s (1972) model of general problem solving. The first stage is the formation of the problem representation, and the second stage is selecting and executing moves to make progress towards the goal. Individuals may perform differently on the same problem because of differences in their internal representation of the problem or because of the strategies they use in move selection. 12 The information included in a representation may differ across individuals, and depends on their task-related knowledge and experience. Individuals with no relevant experience in the domain are more likely to include information which is not relevant to the problem, and overlook subtle but relevant information in the task environment. They may even not know the critical moves to transform an initial state to the goal state. In other words, their representations may be mistaken or incomplete. Experienced individuals should be more capable of selecting the relevant information to encode. They may also generate some intermediate states as sub-goals to guide the move-selection process. Also, additional constraints, not only based on the superficial problem elements, but also on relevant problem solving experience, would be imposed to filter out further irrelevant moves and narrow down the search space. Individual differences also lie in the use of heuristics. To carry out a heuristic search, individuals need to have an evaluation function that scores each move according to how much it can reduce the distance between the current state and the goal/sub-goal state. The evaluation function, again, may differ depending on an individual’s past problem-solving experience. Individuals who have rich experience in solving similar problems may use a strong task-specific evaluation function. However, when encountering a new problem, individuals may only use weak task-general evaluation functions, which may not accurately assess the goodness of each move. Expertise research in a wide range of knowledge domains (e.g., physics, chemistry, and mathematics) has illustrated a significant effect of past problem-solving experience on problem representation and search strategy. Regarding problem representation, experts tend to represent a problem according to its underlying principles, whereas novices base their representation on superficial features of the problem (Adelson, 1984; Chi, Feltovich, & Glaser, 1981; Heyworth, 1989; Larkin, 1983). In terms of the use of a search strategy, novices usually work backward from the goal, in that they will apply 13 moves that can reduce the distance between goal state and the initial state. In contrast, experts work forwards from the initial state, in that they first apply the moves that can generate additional information to enrich the representation, even though those moves may not make immediate progress to the goal. In summary, experts’ move-selection process is knowledge-guided, while novices tend to use mean-end analysis, a task-general strategy, to select moves. (Bedard & Chi, 1992; Heyworth, 1989; Kavakli & Gero, 2002; Larkin, 1983; Lesgold et al., 1988). The core concept of Newell and Simon’s problem-solving model is that these aforementioned processes are explicit and occur at a conscious level, and can be implemented on any computational system. The conceptualization of problem solving as conscious search has become the dominant concept in human problem-solving and artificial intelligence research. Newell and Simon (1972)’s general problem solving model is supported by the analysis of individuals’ verbal protocols in solving different types of problems. SOAR, a general cognitive architecture, has been developed based on this theory to model a wide variety of intelligent behaviors, such as analogical transfer (Gorski & Laird, 2006; Xu, Wintermute, Wang, & Laird, 2009), and decision making (Wang & Lair, 2007). Characterizing problem solving as conscious search implies that individuals are consciously aware of the strategies they are using, and also of their progress towards the goal. This assumption is contradicted by a number of phenomenological findings from studies of a specific type of problem solving: insight problem-solving, which is characterized as sudden, unpredictable, and nonverbalizable (Metcalfe & Weibe, 1987). These phenomena raise a concern about whether Newell and Simon’s problem-solving theory can be applied to insight problem-solving, a topic to which we now turn. 14 The Nature of Insight Insight may be conceived as a sudden and unanticipated realization of the solution of a problem (Metcalfe & Weibe, 1987). Two different perspectives, the conscious and unconscious processing accounts, have been put forward that provide apparently mutually exclusive accounts of the emergence of insight. The conscious account takes the standpoint of Newell and Simon’s general problem-solving model and suggests that insight problem-solving can be attributed to conscious search processes, even though insight problem-solving has distinctive phenomenological features. Opposed to this is the unconscious account, proposing that cognitive mechanisms underlying insights are processes operating at a sub-conscious level. From the perspective of the conscious account, insight is not viewed as a distinct cognitive phenomenon from other kinds of problem solving. Kaplan and Simon (1990) suggested that insight problem-solving falls within the framework of Newell and Simon’s general problem solver model. The difficulty of insight problem-solving lies in having inappropriate search constraints and heuristics in the move selection process that lead to an impasse, and this impasse must be broken by a conscious switch of heuristics or imposing appropriate constraints to confine the search to a relevant search space. Based on this conceptualization, MacGregor, Ormerod, and Chronicle (2001) propose what has since become to known as the Criterion for Satisfactory Progress theory (CSPT), explaining how an inappropriate search strategy causes impasse in insight problem solving. MacGregor et al. suggest that when insight problems do not have a well-defined goal state, individuals will evaluate possible moves against locally rational criteria that indicate whether progress is being made toward the goal properties. For example, the criterion in solving the classic nine-dot problem is that each line must cancel a number of dots given by the ratio of dots remaining to lines available. However, 15 this move-selection criterion may drive individuals away from the moves that lie on the correct solution path. For example, when solving the nine-dot problem, individuals usually fail to make the necessary insight to draw lines beyond the array because of the many criterion-meeting moves available within the square shape. When individuals fail to find moves that meet the criterion for satisfactory progress, then they reach an impasse. As a consequence, they experience “criterion failure”, that is a recognition that their current representation of the problem is unsatisfactory. At this point, they will relax the requirement for maximizing progress, and will instead sample non-maximal moves (e.g., drawing lines beyond the square when solving the nine-dot problem), which may allow the discovery of subsequent moves that make more progress than previous attempts. The impasse is then resolved and insight emerges. The change of search strategy allows the exploration of the non-maximal moves that “suddenly” make available new solution paths. This may explain the distinctive phenomenological characteristics of insight, in which it appears to emerge suddenly and unexpectedly. CSPT has predicted the observed behavior of individuals in several insight problem-solving domains (Chronicle, MacGregor, & Ormerod, 2004; MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001; Ormerod, MacGregor, & Chronicle, 2002). CSPT also suggests that the change in the search strategy occurs only if the problem solver exhausts the initial search space and still fails to solve the problem, and this is determined by the size of the initial search space. If the search space is small, then individuals should realize the failure quickly, and in turn, have a stronger impulse to abandon the inappropriate strategy. The prediction that how fast an individual has the insight relies on the initial search space is evidenced by the findings that individuals’ problem solving performance in solving various visual insight problems is negatively correlated with the size of the initial search space (MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001; Ormerod, MacGregor, & Chronicle, 2002). 16 Unconscious processing accounts consider that the suddenness of the emergence of insight reflects a qualitative difference between insight and non-insight problem-solving processes. Ohlsson (1992) suggested that Newell and Simon’s problem solving model has to be modified in order to handle insight problem-solving. He proposed the Representational Change theory (RCT), suggesting that insight-specific cognitive processes should be added into Newell and Simon’s model. Ohlsson (1992) agrees that both insight and non-insight are approached at the same way initially, in that problem solvers build a representation consisting of initial and the goal state, and search and execute moves in transforming between the initial and the goal states. An impasse is a state for which problem solvers fail to find a move that can transform the current problem state to goal state. However, different from the perspective of the conscious account, RCT(Ohlsson, 1992) suggests that reaching an impasse is mainly due to having an incomplete or mistaken representation, rather than a wrong search strategy. Furthermore, the problem representation is restructured via three unconscious processes: Elaboration, Re-encoding, and Constraint relaxation. Elaboration is the process of enriching information encoded into the initially incomplete representation. Re-encoding is to abandon or reject the information encoded in the current representation in order to correct the mistaken representation. Constraint relaxation is releasing unnecessary constraints. When solving a problem, problem solvers tend to self-impose constraints on the goal state of the problem to confine the search space. However, the constraints sometimes are counterproductive and lead to search in an over-restricted space that does not include the crucial problem-solving moves, and releasing inappropriate constraints can expand the search space. These three representation-restructuring processes can be accomplished through the unconscious spread of activation to relevant but previously ignored memory items. Knoblich and colleagues (Knoblich, Ohlsson, Haider, & Rhenius, 1999; Knoblich, 17 Ohlsson, & Raney, 2001) have enriched RCT by proposing that chunk decomposition is another mechanism underlying restructuring. Chunking is the process of recoding several stimuli into fewer but more complex and meaningful units. Consolidating external information as different chunks can reduce working memory load during problem solving (Chase & Simon, 1973; Ericsson & Lehmann, 1996; Miller, 1956). Knoblich et al. (1999) examined the impact of constraints and chunks on the likelihood of insight in the domain of arithmetic problem-solving using matchstick algebra, where problems are presented to participants using matchsticks that form Roman numerals. Each matchstick problem consisted of a false arithmetic statement written in Roman numerals, an arithmetic operation, and an equals sign all constructed out of matchsticks (e.g., IV = III + III). Individuals had to transform the statement into a true statement by moving one stick from one position to another. In arithmetic problem-solving, individuals will hold different levels of move-selection constraints (from low to high), such as: numeral values cannot be changed except through operations, operator functions cannot be modified, and arithmetic functions have to be in the form x = F(y, z). They will also recode the task information into different chunks. For example, XVII will be perceived as two chunks (X and VII), instead of six single matchsticks \, /, I, I, \ and /. The chunk VII is a loose chunk relative to the chunk X, because VII can be decomposed into different meaningful components V, I, and I, while the components of X do not carrying any meaning. When solving arithmetic problems requiring insight into a new use of knowledge, individuals may have to relax some constraints and decompose some chunks. Low level constraints are local and specific, and the relaxation of them will only cause a small portion of change in the representation, and thus, they are easier to release. Loose chunks also have components that are individually meaningful, and thus they are more likely to be decomposed. Knoblich et al. suggest that insight problem-solving performance should be a function of 18 the level of the constraints that need to be relaxed and the tightness of the chunks that need to be decomposed. As predicted, individuals performed better in solving matchstick problems requiring the relaxation of local constraints and decomposing loose chunks (e.g., problem: V–III= IV, solution: VI–II= IV) than in solving problems biased by global constraints and tight chunks (e.g., problem: III = III + III, solution: III = III = III). Knoblich et al. suggest that Chunk-Decomposition and Constraint-Relaxation can be done by the suppression of activation of irrelevant constraints and chunks, and spread of activation throughout the semantic network to activate relevant but previously ignored memory items. In summary, the conscious account suggests that reaching an impasse is due to the use of wrong search strategy and it can be resolved by switching the search strategy, which is a conscious meta-cognitive process. Unconscious accounts suggest that having an incomplete/mistaken representation is the reason for failure to solve a problem, and the faulty representation can be restructured into a better form by changing the activation level of different memory items in the semantic network. Both the unconscious and conscious accounts have received support from empirical studies. However, it is difficult to draw a cross-study conclusion as to which account has better predictive power. Differences in the settings of insight problems used in these studies do not allow a direct comparison between these two theories. The insight problems used in verifying RCT are knowledge-rich, one-move matchstick problems where only one move is required to change the initial state to the goal state. They do not require the use of any heuristic search to monitor progress. Therefore, matchstick problems have not been used in testing CSPT. CPST receives support from other types of insight problem: knowledge-lean problems. Solving a knowledge-lean problem (e.g., the nine-dot problem) does not require the use of large amount of task-related knowledge. Therefore, 19 it is difficult to apply RCT to predicting the performance in solving knowledge-lean insight problems. In an attempt to fill this research gap, Jones (2003) used the “Car Park Game” as an insight problem to test whether successful insight problem solving is the outcome of relaxation of knowledge-based constraints or a switch in search strategy. The “Car Park Game” is a visual problem in which individuals are asked to maneuver a taxi out of a car park. The pathway from the taxi to the exit is obstructed by other cars. In the task, all the cars can be moved forward or backward. Solving the problem requires individuals to move cars away from the exit pathway so that the taxi car is able to leave the car park. Jones suggested that individuals would hold a self-imposed constraint that the taxi cannot be moved until the exit is clear, and therefore they would exclude the critical move, which is moving the taxi even when the pathway is not cleared yet to enable the movement of other cars. Participants would move the taxi only if that self-imposed constraint was relaxed. Jones predicted that, if RCT is correct and the self-imposed constraint causes impasse and post-impasse restructuring is the relaxation of the unnecessary constraint relaxation, then an impasse at the point prior to moving the taxi is expected. According to CSPT, how quick an individual reaches an impasse depends on the effectiveness of search within the problem space, for example, how far ahead an individual can look. Therefore, Jones suggested that if CSPT is correct, the number of moves an individual can look-ahead during problem solving should predict how quick he/she reaches an impasse. In addition to this, RCT suggests that restructuring relies on the activation of relevant knowledge and the de-activation of irrelevant knowledge. Thus, Jones predicted that if RCT is correct, solving a problem with a similar layout beforehand should facilitate performance on the Car Park Game problem because the practice problem 20 would activate relevant knowledge. Jones proposed that CSPT should predict no facilitation effect as successfully insight problem solving relies on a task-general search strategy, but not the activation of task-relevant knowledge. Jones analyzed participants’ eye-movements during problem solving as well as their performance data, and suggested that both theories account for part of the individuals’ problem solving behavior. The data indicated that participants reached an impasse before moving the taxi, and also, solving the relevant practice problem can enhance performance on the Car Park Game. These were in line with Jones’ predictions based on RCT. However, the data also support predictions based on the CSPT theory, showing that participants with a look-ahead of 3 performed far better than those with a look-ahead of 1 or 2 steps. These findings imply the possibility that these two theories may not be opposing each other. However, the validity of Jones’ study is seriously in question because of some major conceptual and methodological flaws in the study. First, the key difference between RCT and CSPT has not been addressed in Jones’ study. RCT suggests that the mechanisms that lead to restructuring are the unconscious processes operating within the semantic network, while CRPT views restructuring as a conscious switch in search strategy. However, Jones has not investigated whether restructuring occurs at a conscious or unconscious level. Second, it is uncertain whether individuals would really hold the constraint “the taxi cannot be moved until the exit way is clear” while solving the Car Park Game. Individuals usually learn from their past problem-solving experience and develop different relevant constraints to guide the problem solving processes. However, the “moving the car until the exit is clear” is not a necessary condition of real-life car parking, and therefore, participants in Jones’ study may not hold this constraint when solving the Car Park Game, implying that the Car Park Game may not be an insight 21 problem. Third, the Car-Park-Game is a visual problem that does not require the use of a huge amount of task-related knowledge. Hence, it is not an appropriate task to examine whether restructuring is the outcome of the occurrence of the processes within a semantic network. The Car Park Game is not an ideal task to test CRPT either. When solving the Car-Park-Game, there are only two types of moves: move that can make maximum immediate-progress (moving the car away from the pathway), or move that make no immediate-progress at all (not moving any car away from the pathway). Because of the narrow variation in the degree of progress among the available moves, the problem-solving behavior may not truly reflect the impact of inappropriate move-selective criteria on insight problem-solving. Fourth, while CSRT does not currently contain a learning component since the theory has not been developed with this in mind, it is a mistake to suggest that CSRT predicts null transfer effects from training. There is extensive empirical evidence from expert-novice research showing that past task-related experience can change the criterion an individual uses in evaluating the moves (see the review in Chi et al., 1981). Because of these concerns, the conclusion of Jones’ study is questionable. Studying the role of incubation in insight problem-solving may serve a valuable role in addressing the nature of restructuring, and testing between RCT and CSPT. Incubation is the period of time during which a problem solver shifts attention away from the unsolved problem to another area after a first unsuccessful attempt at solving the problem. It has been suggested that performing incubation tasks can divert attention away from the problem, and this would facilitate unconscious processing underlying restructuring (Bowers, Regehr, Balthazard, & Parker, 1990; Seifert et al., 1995; Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). Examining the incubation effects found with different types of insight problems may offer evidence for deciding 22 whether post-impasse restructuring is an unconscious process, and whether the processes underlying restructuring arise at a semantic-level, a meta-cognitive level, or both. Proposed Mechanisms of Incubation Based on geniuses’ introspective reports of their idea discovery processes, Wallas (1926) identified that insightful thinking process can be divided into five stages: Preparation, Incubation, Intimation, Illumination, and Verification. Preparation is the period of time in which the problem solver gathers information to formulate a problem representation, and makes an initial attempt to solve it but reaches an impasse at the end. Incubation is the period of time that the problem solver shifts attention away from the unsolved problem to another area after the first unsuccessful attempt at solving the problem. Intimation is the stage that the problem solver gets a feeling that the fully conscious flash of solution is coming. Illumination is the moment that the solution to the problem suddenly appears in the problem solver’s mind. Verification, the last stage of the creative thinking process, is the period of time that the problem solver evaluates and modifies the solution into a better form. Wallas’ (1926) is one of the earliest models describing the insight thinking process systematically, and it provides a direction for researchers to study the insight process systematically and empirically. Several hypotheses have been proposed to account for the role of an incubation period on insight problem solving, and similar to the debate on the nature of insight, they can also be divided into two main kinds: conscious-work and unconscious-work. The conscious-work hypothesis holds that incubation effects are due to issues such as reduction of mental fatigue (Posner, 1973) or additional covert problem solving during the incubation period (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Posner, 1973). Both sources implicate changes in consciously controlled problem-solving activities during an incubation period. In contrast, the unconscious-work hypothesis suggests that positive incubation effects 23 are the result of gradual and unconscious problem-solving processes that occur during an incubation period (Bower et al., 1990; Simon, 1966; Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991; Seifert et al., 1995; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). Three different unconscious processes have been proposed to account for incubation effects. The first involves eliciting new knowledge: over time, activation will spread towards previously-ignored but relevant memory items. Even if relevant items do not receive above-threshold activation, this process can still sensitize individuals to related concepts, and thus they will be more likely to make use of external cues to solve a problem. In addition, partially-activated concepts may combine with others to yield fortuitous insightful ideas (Bower et al., 1990; Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). The second hypothesis is selective forgetting: an incubation period will weaken the activation of inappropriate solution concepts or/and constraints that distract individuals during initial attempts, allowing a fresh view of the problem (Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991). The third hypothesis is opportunistic assimilation, in which an individual’s mental representation of a problem will be re-organized into a more appropriate and stable form after initial unsuccessful attempts. The individual is then able to capitalise upon relevant external information or to re-arrange problem information in a manner that allows a solution to be found more readily (Seifert et al., 1995). The first two hypotheses suggest that the incubation effect or the emergence of the insightful solution can be attributed to autonomous processes among semantic networks. The Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis does not impose any presumption on where the restructuring occurs; it may be a switch in move-selection strategy (MacGregor et al., 2001) or the spread of activation at a semantic level (Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). The conscious- and unconscious-work accounts generate different predictions concerning the effects of activities that individuals engage in during an incubation period. 24 According to the conscious-work account, individuals benefit most from an unfilled incubation period, as this gives them an opportunity either to relax, reduce fatigue, or to continue working on the problem. In contrast, unconscious work accounts suggest that unconscious problem-solving processes occur when individuals shift their attention away from the problem to other mental activities. Thus, a certain level of involvement in other tasks during an incubation period may facilitate post-incubation problem-solving. A number of experimental studies have examined the role of task type presented during an incubation period. The experimental paradigms of these incubation studies are fairly uniform: one group of participants is interrupted with an incubation period (having a break or performing other tasks) while solving a problem, whereas the other group works on the problem continuously. Performance differences between these two groups are then compared. The findings of the published studies do not give unconditional support to either the unconscious-work or the conscious-work accounts. Patrick (1986) found that participants who had a filled incubation period outperformed those who had an unfilled incubation period. However, Browne and Cruse (1988) reported the opposite pattern: participants who took a rest during an incubation period performed better than those who had to perform tasks during an incubation period. There are also studies that report the same level of performance by participants with filled and unfilled incubation periods (Olton & Johnson, 1976, Smith & Blankenship, 1989). However, these studies vary in terms of the length of incubation period, the target problems tackled, and the nature of the interpolated tasks during the incubation period. Because of the inconsistent findings concerning incubation, some researchers have doubted the existence of the effect, in particular rejecting the unconscious-work hypotheses (Brown & Cruse, 1988; Olton & Johnson, 1976; Perkins, 1995). One explanation for conflicting findings it that there are procedural moderators other than task type that influence the occurrence of problem-solving processes during an incubation 25 period, such as the length of the incubation period, or the nature of the problem. The following section presents the likely key moderators of incubation, which is achieved by reviewing the methods used in previous studies. A particular focus of this review is to identify moderators that might discriminate conscious-work and unconscious-work hypotheses and also the mechanisms (reduction of fatigue, additional work, activation of new information, forgetting, restructuring) that might underlie each hypothesis. Procedural Moderators of the Incubation Effect Besides the incubation task moderators, other experimental parameter may also have an impact on the incubation effect. By reviewing the methods used in previous studies, a number of procedural moderators are identified. The following section is a discussion on some potential moderators. Note that many moderators in past studies (e.g., number of trials, participant characteristics) might have been included, but we focus on those we believe fundamental to discriminating between conscious-work and unconscious-work hypotheses, and also those relevant to the mechanisms (reduction of fatigue, additional work; activation of new information, forgetting, restructuring) that might underlie each hypothesis. The interpolated task used during the incubation period Various types of interpolated task have been used in past studies, and they can be divided into tasks of high or low cognitive demand. Examples of high cognitive demand tasks include mental rotation, counting backwards, and visual memory tests, whereas reading is commonly adopted as a low cognitive demand task. High demand tasks should fully occupy the individual’s mind, and prevent further conscious work on the unsolved problem. Some studies report that undertaking a high cognitive demand task during an incubation period is beneficial to the problem solving process (Kaplan, 1990; Patrick, 26 1986; Segal, 2004). Nonetheless, studies using low cognitive demand tasks that do not require individuals to focus their conscious attention on task performance report similar benefits (Beck, 1979; Silveira, 1972; Smith & Blankenship, 1989). Length of the preparation period During the preparation period, problem solvers gather information to formulate a problem representation and make initial attempts to solve, which may lead to an impasse. Although a problem may not be solved during the preparation period, this does not mean that the effort the problem solver spends on the problem is fruitless. Schank (1982, 1999) and VanLehn (1988) both suggest that failure in problem solving is important in the human learning process. Studies by Palatano and Seifert(1994) and Seifert et al. (1995) have found evidence of a Zeigarnik effect in insight problem solving (Zeigarnik, 1927, 1938), where individuals remembered the problems on which they got “stuck” better than those solved immediately. Seifert et al. hypothesized that having a better memory for failed problems might help individuals return efficiently to the problem once relevant new information is encountered during an incubation period, thereby maximizing the chance of solving. Evidence concerning this prediction has been obtained in an empirical study carried by Silveira (1971), showing that problem solvers performed better with longer preparation and incubation periods Length of the incubation period Longer incubation periods may allow more additional problem-solving activity, or allow a greater degree of forgetting of misleading items or spreading of activation memory. Thus, problem solvers may show a larger performance improvement when they return to the problem after a long incubation period than after a short one. Some studies report evidence supporting this contention (Silveria, 1971, Smith & Blankenship, 1989, 27 Beck, 1979, Fulgosi & Guilford, 1968). However, it is difficult to draw cross-experiment conclusions, since there is no standard operationalization of what constitutes “long” and “short” incubation periods. In Smith and Blankenship’s study (1989), for example, a 15-min incubation period was defined as a long incubation period, and they reported that participants receiving this length of incubation period performed better than those receiving a 5-min incubation period. However, in Beck’s (1979) study, a 20-min incubation period was considered to be short, and participants’ performance in this group did not differ from the control group. Kaplan (1989) suggested that, to judge whether the incubation period is short or long, the length of time that problem solvers spend on initial attempts to solve (named the “preparation period” by Wallas, 1926) should also be taken into account. Kaplan found that a larger incubation effect was observed after increasing the ratio of the length of preparation period to incubation period. Thus, in addition to including incubation and preparation periods as separate moderators in the meta-analysis reported below, a secondary analysis was also undertaken using the ratio of preparation to incubation time as an alternative moderator. Nature of the problem Various different types of problem have been used in incubation studies. Some problems, which are termed ‘creative problems’ here, require individuals to produce multiple new ideas to meet a specific brief. For example, a verbal divergent-production task is the Consequences test(e.g., “What would be the result if everyone suddenly lost the ability to read and write”; Fulgosi & Guilford, 1967). Typically, there is no right or wrong answer to these kinds of problem and performance is assessed in terms of the numbers of solution ideas that are generated. Other problems require individuals to discover a specific target solution that is known in advance by the experimenter. Problems of this kind studied in the literature on 28 incubation are generally of a type described as ‘insight’ problems, in that they require the solver to reject initial solution ideas by achieving insight into an alternative strategy or knowledge domain. The insight problems used in the incubation studies can be divided into visual problems, which typically require the solver to consider a visuo-spatial array of the problem (e.g., the nine-dot problem; Scheerer, 1963), and linguistic problems, which typically require the solver to consider linguistic information related to the problem. The Remote Associates Task (RAT; Mednick, 1962) is one of the most commonly used linguistic problems in incubation studies. In each RAT, three stimulus words are presented to individuals, who then have to think of a fourth word that can form association with each of the three words. For example, if the three stimulus words of a RAT are “electric”, “wheel”, and “high”, the fourth word can be “chair”. Bowden & Jung-Beeman (2003) have developed a pool of remote associate problems, and collected the normative data regarding the resolution rate and response time for the problems. The classification of insight problems into visual- and linguistic-based is supported by research findings from Gilhooly and Murphy (2005) showing that solving visual and linguistic insight problems require different types of cognitive skills. In the remainder of this chapter, problem types are referred to as creative, visual, and linguistic. Descriptions of the types of problem used in incubation studies are illustrated in Appendix A. Problem type is likely to be an important determinant of incubation, since each type creates seems to create different task demands. For instance, the nine-dot problem appears to require the solver to restructure an initially faulty or incomplete problem representation in searching for a representation that allows solution, while the Consequences Task appears to require the activation of as wide a range of different concepts as possible. 29 The presence of solution-relevant cues Some unconscious processes proposed to explain incubation effects are purely internal and independent of the external environment, such as the inhibition of irrelevant memory (Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991) and the recombination of partially-activated concepts (Bower et al., 1990). Others stress interactions with the external environment, such as the proposal that spreading activation can partially activate previously-ignored relevant memory and therefore sensitize the problem solver to chance encounters with related stimuli (Seifert at al., 1995). A few studies have examined effects of the presence of cues during an incubation period (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Dodds, Smith, & Ward, 2002; Dorfman, 1990; Driestadt, 1969, Olton & Johnson, 1976). Most failed to find any positive effect of cues on the incubation effect. However, the failure reported in these studies may be due to other factors, such as the difficulty of the unsolved problems. In order to have a fair evaluation on the impact of this moderator, we first have to isolate the effect of other moderators on the incubation effect. Misleading cues Another factor that may influence the occurrence of incubation effects is the presence of misleading cues. Smith and Blankenship (1989) carried out a series of experiments to examine the effect of an incubation period on solving Remote Associates Tasks (RATs), in which participants had to find a word that might accompany each of three presented words. Smith and Blankenship presented cues (shown here in italics) comprising misleading associates and the target word next to each of the three stimulus words. An example of a misleading RAT is: SHIP ocean, OUTER inner, CRAWL floor. The target solution is “space”. Performance improvements after an incubation period were observed only when participants solved tasks containing misleading cues. They concluded that a problem solver who is fixated on misleading information benefits more 30 from an incubation period. The misleading cues data provide critical support for forgetting-based explanations of incubation. The presence of misleading cues is therefore one of the potential moderators examined in this meta-analysis. Summary The aforementioned factors are potential moderators of incubation effects. Understanding the link between the moderators and incubation not only helps us to find the optimal settings to demonstrate incubation effects, but also offers insight into the underlying mechanisms of an incubation period. There have been two reviews to date of relations between different procedural variables and the incubation effect, and both are qualitative in nature. Olton’s (1979) review of past incubation studies led him to question the existence of incubation effects, given that no experimental paradigm appeared to demonstrate incubation effect reliably. Yet, a limited number of studies were available at that time: only 10 incubation studies were included in his review. A recent review by Dodds, Ward and Smith (in press) with more studies included suggested that several variables may interact to influence the effectiveness of an incubation period. However, the qualitative nature of their review led them to conclude that findings of past studies are too divergent and that more studies are needed to assess the impact of each variable and to identify the optimum settings for an incubation effect. The wide variation in experimental parameters among studies makes it difficult to draw cross-experiment conclusions from a qualitative review. To overcome these problems, a systematic meta-analytic review is needed. Meta-analytic review allows a quantitative evaluation of research domains that describes the typical strength of the effect or phenomenon, and also the relation of each moderator to the size of the effect by using statistical analysis methods (Rosenthal, 1995). In this thesis, a meta-analysis, reported in the next chapter, was carried out to verify the existence of a positive 31 experimental incubation effect, and more importantly, to examine the impact of the procedural moderators on the incubation effect. Attentional Resource Allocation Styles and Incubation The wide divergence of procedural moderator parameters in past incubation studies may be one of the reasons for inconsistent findings of incubation. However, there are some conflicting findings that cannot simply be attributed to the differences in experimental settings. Several studies have reported failure to replicate positive incubation effects even under the same experimental settings (e.g., Olton & Johnson, 1976), implying that there must be some internal factors influencing incubation effects. Some studies have examined the relations between the magnitude of incubation effects and individual differences in high-level cognitive abilities, such as intelligence or general problem solving ability. The findings are, however, conflicting, in that both positive (Mednick, Mednick, & Mednick, 1964; Moss, 2002) and negative (Murry & Denny, 1969; Smith & Blankenship, 1991; Moss, 2002) correlations were found between incubation and ability. One possible explanation is that high-level cognitive ability is the composition of different basic cognitive and non-cognitive characteristics, such as attentional style, memory capacity, and level of motivation. Each of these factors may have a unique impact on incubation effects, and they may also interact with each other and thus influence incubation effects. Therefore, the relation between high-level cognitive ability and incubation effect size is a complex one. A more systematic approach to examining individual differences in incubation effects would be first to investigate the impact of each of the basic cognitive characteristic on the incubation effect, before examining the combined effect of them. Attentional allocation style is suggested to be one of the basic cognitive characteristics that affects the occurrence of incubation effects (Ansburg & Hill, 2003; 32 Finke, Ward, & Smith, 1992; Martindale, 1995). Individuals have different attentional allocation styles, some showing a tendency to allocate attention widely to different stimuli, others being more likely to focus on a single stimulus. For those with a focused attentional style, performing an incubation task would force them to allocate their cognitive resources in a more diffused manner. If this led to enhanced problem-solving performance, this result would suggest the occurrence of some unconscious problem solving processes. For individuals who habitually allocate their attention broadly, they are always in a mental state that facilitates the occurrence of unconscious problem solving process, and thus, performing incubation tasks may not be that helpful to them (Ansburg & Hill, 2003; Finke, Ward, & Smith, 1992; Martindale, 1995). This interpretation implies that incubation periods do not benefit all the individuals to the same degree, and the mixed findings on the past incubation studies may partly due to the lack of control for this cognitive characteristic. Methodological Problems of Past Incubation Studies The literature on incubation effects and individual differences in cognitive characteristics suggests that the likelihood of occurrence of proposed incubation mechanisms depends on various procedural and cognitive factors. To generate strong data to test this contention, the traditional paradigm of past incubation studies, which only assesses performance improvement rather than specific underlying mechanisms, has to be modified. The experimental paradigms of most past incubation studies are rather uniform: one group of participants has an incubation period while solving the problem, whereas the other group is asked to solve the problem continuously. Researchers have then compared performance differences in solving problems between these two groups of participants. The disadvantage of simply measuring performance change is that it cannot offer direct 33 evidence to reject or support the occurrence of proposed conscious or unconscious processes. Non-significant performance improvement does not necessarily imply the absence of any unconscious process during the incubation period. It is possible that unconscious processes occur during an experimental incubation period, yet, the effect that they generate is not strong enough to cause any influence on an individual’s problem solving performance within the current experimental conditions. However, even if a significant positive performance improvement is observed, it still cannot verify the occurrence of any specific unconscious processing. Performance improvement could be the result of selective-forgetting, spreading-activation, problem-restructuring, the combination of any of them, or none of them. Therefore, in order to offer more direct evidence for the occurrence of these problem solving processes, a more specific assessment of processes underlying incubation effects is needed. To test for the presence of spreading-activation and selective-forgetting, a more direct measurement is to assess individuals’ sensitivity to relevant and irrelevant memory items before and after an incubation period. Yaniv and Meyer (1987), in an attempt to confirm the occurrence of spreading-activation underlying incubation, asked participants to solve a list of rare-word-definition tasks, and then measured their lexical decision time for answers of the unsolved word definition tasks and matched control words. They found that participants made quicker lexical decisions about the answers of unsolved tasks than control words, and suggested that the residues of initial activation persist even when individuals are not currently solving the problem, and this would sensitize individuals to assimilate relevant external cues encountered subsequently. This finding has offered potential bases for the incubation effect. However, one problem of Yaniv and Meyer’s study (1987) is that the rare-word-definition task is a recall task, and it is uncertain if similar spreading-activation would also occur in insight problem solving. Sio and Rudowicz (2007) adapted the lexical decision tasks in assessing the role 34 of an incubation period when solving RATs. An increase in sensitivity towards relevant memory items after an incubation period was found when solving RATs embedded with misleading cues. However, they did not also investigate whether enhanced problem-solving performance arose as a result of incubation. If spreading activation is the source of incubation effects, then enhanced performance to RATs should be accompanied by decreased lexical decision latencies. The presentation of lexical decision tasks containing solution-relevant words could also serve as a relevant cue to problems, and this helps test the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis. According to the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, incubation periods help problem restructuring and increase the chance of assimilating external cues. It implies that individuals who receive relevant cues (e.g., relevant lexical decision tasks) during an incubation period would outperform those who receive cues in a non-incubation condition or those who do not receive any cues during an incubation period. Comparing individuals’ performance improvement among No-Incubation and incubation conditions can offer empirical evidence to test the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis. In summary, the preceding discussion suggests the need for a quantitative review of the past incubation studies, and provides a context for the present set of experiments. To this end, a meta-analysis was carried out for assessing the impact of each of these aforementioned procedural moderators, and interactions between them, on incubation effects. Four experiments were then carried out to examine further the meta-analysis findings, and specifically to investigate the three unconscious-processing hypotheses: spreading-activation, selective-forgetting, and cue-assimilation. In these experiments, besides measuring incubation effects on performance levels, lexical decision tasks were adapted for a more direct assessment of the occurrence of the three proposed unconscious processes. 35 CHAPTER III: THE IMPACT OF PROCEDURAL MODERATORS ON INCUBATION EFFECTS: META-ANALYSIS This meta-analysis addresses two questions: first, is there reliable evidence for incubation; and second, what are the most influential moderators? To address the first question, the effect size of incubation reported in each available study was computed. Given that the variability among effect sizes is likely to be greater than that resulting from subject-level sampling, a random effects model was adopted in this meta-analysis. A heterogeneity test was carried out to verify this assumption, and then the weighted mean under the assumption of a random effects model was computed and assessed to determine if it was significantly larger than zero. To address the second question, weighted least-squares linear regressions were carried out using the aforementioned moderators as predictor variables and the incubation effect size in each study weighted by the inverse of its variance as the criterion variable. The results of the regression report the independent contributions of each potential moderator to the incubation effect, while controlling for all other moderators. This approach allows us to summarize the past studies systematically even though they vary widely in numbers and type of experimental parameters. In addition, interactions between different moderators, such as the nature of the interpolation task during the incubation period and the nature of the problem, were examined. 36 Method Literature Search Publications that contained studies relevant to a meta-analysis of incubation were collected through a search of the ERIC, PsycInfo, PsycArticles, and MEDLINE databases using the keyword incubat*, intersected with one of fixation, creativ*, divergent*, insight*, or problem. Then, references given in all the obtained articles were systematically searched for additional relevant publications. There is a concern that studies with statistically significant results are more likely to get published than those without significant results, and this may lead to a biased retrieval of studies. To ameliorate this to some extent, similar literature searches were carried out in the ProQuest Digital Dissertations databases and using Google Scholar™ for retrieving PhD dissertations, unpublished papers, and conference papers concerning the incubation effect. In total, 37 relevant publications were identified and obtained. Studies meeting the following criteria were assimilated in the analysis. 1. The settings and difficulty of the problems were the same among all the experimental conditions; 2. The total length of time that participants could spend on solving the problem consciously was the same among all the conditions; 3. The study included a control (no incubation) group, and participants in that group worked on the problem continuously; 4. Participants’ problem solving performance in pre and post- incubation periods was measured; 5. The study reported information that allowed the computation of an effect size. The 1st and 2nd selection criteria ensured that tasks were presented in an identical way among different conditions, and any between-condition performance differences could be attributed to differences in settings of the incubation period. The inclusion of criterion 37 3, a control condition (no break between the first and the second attempts at the problem), is essential to provide a baseline against which performance in incubation conditions can be compared. Only publications that assessed the problem solving performance in both first and second attempts were included in the analysis (criterion 4). Therefore, some studies (e.g., Sio & Rudowicz, 2007; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987) that did not assess post –incubation problem solving performance were excluded. The information required for computing effect sizes is discussed in the section “Estimation of effect sizes”. Eight publications were excluded because the experimental studies contained within them failed to meet one or more of the aforementioned criteria. The specific reasons for excluding the publications are described in Appendix B, which also describes the settings of studies included in the meta-analysis. Of the remaining 29 publications, 20 were refereed journal articles, 8 were PhD dissertations, and 1 was a conference paper. The ratio of the refereed to other studies is 2.2:1, which is within the suggested range of between 128:1 and 1:1 for including unpublished studies in an effort to avoid publication bias (Thornton & Lee, 2000). Most publications included multiple experiments, thereby allowing a reasonable sample size of independent studies (N = 117) to be achieved. Coding Procedure Many of the experiments reported in the selected publications had two or more experimental conditions, such as incubation periods of different length or different types of task in the incubation period. For the sake of the meta-analysis, experiments with more than one incubation condition were broken down into independent studies with one incubation condition and one control condition. The same control group may be included in more than one independent study, and compared with more than one incubation condition. For example, in Goldman, Wolters, and Winogard’s (1992) 38 experiment, there were control, short-incubation-period, and long-incubation-period conditions. The experiment was decomposed into two studies, one consisting of the control and short-incubation-period conditions, and the other consisting of the control and long-incubation-period conditions. To avoid inflating the degrees of freedom available, the number of participants in the control condition was split across studies entered into the analysis, a method advocated by Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, and van Ijzendoorn (2007). There were also studies having more than one control condition. In such cases, the control condition that had the most similar setting to the incubation condition was chosen. For example, in Hansberry’s (1998) third experiment, participants had to solve a list of RATs under one of three conditions: two control and one incubation. In one control condition, the RATs were presented individually for 60 seconds. In the other control condition, as well as in the incubation condition, each RAT was presented in two separate 30-second blocks. Data from the latter control condition were therefore used in computing the effect size, because this control condition and the incubation condition had the closest settings in terms of RAT presentation. After separating the experiments into numbers of independent studies, a standard system was used to code each study. Background information on each independent study (author, publication year, and the number of participants in each condition), as well as potential moderator variables, were extracted. Table 3.1 presents the coding system used in this meta-analysis. Appendix C presents the information extracted from each independent study by using the coding system. 39 Table 3.1 Coding System Used in the Meta-Analysis Variables Coding Description Author Author (s) of the study Year Year the study was published Total Total number of participants Problem Type 0 = creative problem (e.g., consequence task) 1 = visual problem ( e.g., radiation problem) 2 = linguistic problem ( e.g., remote associates task, anagram, rebus) Misleading Cues 0 = no misleading cues 1 = misleading cues embedded in the problem Preparation Period Amount of time spent on each problem before the incubation period ( in minute) Incubation Period Length of the incubation period ( in minute) Incubation Task 0 = rest 1 = low cognitive load task ( e.g., drawing a picture, reading) 2 = high cognitive load task ( e.g., mental rotation task, memory test) Cues Presence of relevant cues during the incubation period 0 = no cue 1 = yes 40 Estimation of Effect Sizes The effect size, Cohen’s d, was computed for each study entered into the meta-analysis. Cohen’s d in this meta-analysis comprised the difference in mean problem-solving performance scores between the control and incubation conditions divided by their pooled standard deviation (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). In some cases, effect sizes had to be calculated from t and F values, frequencies or p values. If a p-less-than value was given instead of an exact p value, the p-less-than value was treated as an exact value, and an estimate of Cohen’s d was generated. For studies that did not include any of the above-mentioned information but only provided statements of non-significant differences between the control and the incubation groups, then Cohen’s d was assumed to be zero. Among the studies that included multiple incubation conditions, some provided a statement of non-significant performance differences among the incubation conditions, and only reported the overall performance difference between the control and the incubation conditions. In such cases, all incubation conditions were assumed to generate the same magnitude of incubation effect sizes. Of the 117 effect sizes, 88 were extracted directly from the means and standard deviations, t value, F value, frequencies, or p value; 8 were computed from a p less than value; and 21 were estimated from statements of significance. In some incubation studies, problem solving performance was assessed along more than one dimension. For example, in the study carried out by Vul and Pashler (2007), participants’ performance on RATs was measured in terms of the time spent on solving RATs and the number of correct solutions. In such cases, a single effect size was computed by averaging the effect size from each measure (cf. Durlak & Lipsey, 1991). Following Hedge and Olkin’s (1985) suggestion for removing bias caused by small sample studies, an unbiased effect size estimate was computed by multiplying the effect size of each single study by a factor 1-3/ (4 (total N-2)-1), where total N is the 41 total number of participants of that study. Any unbiased effect size larger than 2 standard deviations from the group mean was considered an outlier, and was recoded to the value of the effect size found at 2 standard deviations, following a procedure for reducing the bias caused by extreme effect sizes reported by Lipsey & Wilson (2001). Heterogeneity Analysis In this analysis, it was predicted that the variance in magnitude of the unbiased effect sizes among studies was not due simply to sampling error but instead to the difference in settings of each study (e.g., length of incubation period, nature of incubation task, presence of cues). Therefore, analyses of the effect sizes should be carried out under the assumption of a random effects model. To confirm the assumption of heterogeneous distribution of effect sizes, a heterogeneity test was carried out before running any analyses on the effect sizes. The standard measure of heterogeneity is the Cochran’s Q test. The Q statistic is the weighted1 sum of squared differences between the unbiased effect size estimate of each independent study and the weighted average unbiased effect size estimate across studies. Q is distributed as a chi-square statistic with k -1 degree of freedom, where k is the number of independent studies. If the Cochran’s Q test for heterogeneity is statistically significant(Q is larger than the chi-square value with k-1 degree of freedom), the assumption of the random effects model is supported. 1 The weighting was the inverse of the within-study variance of the effect estimate, and the formula for the within-study variance was [(2 x square root of total N) + (N of experimental x N of control x square root of the unbiased effect size)]/(2 x total N x N of experimental x N of control), where N is the number of participants in that condition (Cooper &Hedges, 1994). Three studies were excluded when computing the weighted average unbiased effect size estimate and the Cochran’s Q value because they had a within-subjects design, and thus all participants were involved in both control and incubation conditions. Thus, the weighting formula could not apply to them. 42 Publication Bias Prior to investigating the impact of potential moderators, a preliminary analysis was undertaken to assess if a publication bias existed in the selection of studies despite the inclusion of unpublished studies. A funnel plot of sample size against unbiased effect size estimates was created. In the absence of any publication bias, it is expected that the plot would be a funnel shape, such that the amount of scatter about the mean effect size deceases with increasing sample size. In addition to checking the presence of publication bias qualitatively, a weighted least-squares linear regression was carried out using the unbiased effect size estimates as the dependent variable, and sample sized weighted by the inverse of variance in a random effects model, which is the sum of the between-studies variance2 (random variance component) and within-study variance of the unbiased effect size. The regression slope (unstandardized coefficient of the predictive variable) would be expected to approach zero if there is no publication bias (Macaskill, Walter, & Irwing, 2001). The outcome of this analysis is reported below. Regression Model Testing Due to the wide variation in experiment settings among incubation studies, observed incubation effect size differences may reflect the combined impact of different moderators. Hence, weighted least-squares regression analyses were carried out to reveal the true impact of each moderator on incubation effects. The regression analyses were organized into two main sections. In the first section, the incubation studies were first classified into different groups, in terms of the types of problem used, the cognitive The between-studies variance was equal to [Q-(k-1)]/c, where Q is the Cochran’s Q value and k was the number of studies. The formula for c was ((the sum of the inverse of the within-study variance) – (the sum of the square of the inverse of the within-study variance)/(the sum of the inverse of the within-study variance)), as suggested by Cooper and Hedges (1994). 2 43 load of the incubation tasks, the presence of misleading cues, and the presence of relevant cue during an incubation period. Within each sub-group, the random variance component of the studies was computed. A larger-than-zero random variance component implies that the variability of effect sizes within these studies is not simply due to subject-level sampling error (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). A weighted least-squares regression analysis was carried out as a follow-up analysis to model the effect sizes. The unbiased effect size estimate weighted by the inverse of variance was the outcome variable of the regression analysis. The predictor variables included Problem Type, Misleading Cues, Cues, Incubation Task, Preparation Period, and Incubation Period3. The categorical variables were represented with the appropriate number of dummy coded vectors. The categories “Creative Problem” (Problem Type), “Rest” (Incubation Task), “No misleading cues” (Misleading Cues) and the “No Cue” (Cues) were used as reference groups in the analysis, and their coefficients were restricted to zero. The second section of the analyses investigated the general impact of the moderators on the incubation effect sizes. In this section, all the incubation studies were group together, and a weighted least-squares regression analysis was carried out to investigate the general impact of each moderator. Again, the weighted unbiased effect size was the outcome variable of the regression analysis. The predictor variables were Problem Type, Misleading Cues, Cues, Incubation Tasks, Preparation Period, and Incubation Period. Another weighted least-squares regression was carried out to examine the interaction between the categorical variables Problem Type and Incubation Task. The predictor variables were the appropriate number of dummy coded vectors and the multiplicative terms of these two categorical variables, as well as the variables Misleading Cues, Preparation Period, Incubation Period, and Cues. A more detailed The standard deviation of the weighted mean, also known as the “standard error”, was calculated as the square root of 1/wi, and the 95% Confidence Interval was calculated as the weighted mean +/- 1.96*standard deviation of the weighted mean (Hedges & Olkin,1985). 44 3 description of the selection of the dummy coded vectors and the multiplicative terms of “Problem Type” and “Incubation Task” is presented in Appendix D. Results One hundred and seventeen studies were included in this meta-analysis. The total number of participants was 3606, and the median number of participants per study was 25. An unbiased effect size estimate was computed for each independent study. Among these studies, 85 of them report positive effect sizes. The unbiased effect size estimates range from -.71 to 4.07, and the median was .26. The unweighted mean of the unbiased effect size estimate was .41, with a standard deviation of .71. The upper and the lower bound 95% confidence interval were .54 and .28. Unbiased effect sizes larger than 2 standard deviations from the mean were recoded to the value of the effect size found at 2 standard deviations. Table 3.2 gives the stem-and-leaf display showing the distribution of the unbiased effect sizes. The unweighted mean of the adjusted unbiased effect size estimate was .36, with standard deviation .51. The upper and the lower bound 95% confidence interval were .26 and .45. The confidence interval does not include zero, implying that the estimate of mean unbiased effect size is significantly larger than zero. 45 Table 3.2 Stem-and-Leaf Display of 117 Unbiased Effect Sizes Stem Leaf -.7 1 -.5 9, 8, 8 -.4 0 -.3 8, 5, 3 -.2 -.1 8, 7, 4, 4, 0 -.0 3 .0 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 3, 4, 5, 5, 6, 6, 7, 7, 9 .1 0, 0, 0, 0, 0,1, 1, 3, 4, 4, 4, 5, 7, 7 .2 1, 4, 5, 8, 8 .3 1, 4, 6, 6, 7 .4 0, 0, 1, 1, 2, 2, 4, 5, 7, 7, 8, 8, 9 .5 0, 2, 2, 6, 6, 6, 6, 6, 6, 7 .6 2, 2, 4, 5, 6, 6, 6 .7 0, 1, 2, 4, 4 .8 6 .9 9, 9 1.0 5, 5, 7, 7, 8, 9 1.1 7 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5 46 1.6 8 1.7 0 1.8 2, 2, 2, 2 Note. Outliers were recoded. The heterogeneity statistic, Cochran’s Q, was 173.99, which is significantly larger than the Chi-Square critical value, df = 113, p < .001. This supports the use of a random effects model. The variance of each unbiased effect size in random effects model was the sum of the between-studies variance and within-study variance of the unbiased effect size. The between-studies variance, also called the random variance component, among these incubation studies was .0834. The mean of the weighted unbiased effect size was .29, with .04 standard deviation3, and the 95% confidence interval was (.21, .39). The non-zero confidence interval implies that the weighted mean is significantly larger than zero. This answers our first question, showing the existence of a positive incubation effect. Figure 3.1 presents the funnel plot of sample size against estimated unbiased effect size of each study in the meta-analysis. A weighted least-squares regression using unbiased effect sizes weighted by the inverse of the variance as the dependent variable, and sample size as the predictive variable, was carried out. The regression coefficient of the predictive variable was not significantly different from zero, standardized coefficient (β) = -.08, p = .41, suggesting the absence of publication bias. Thus no correction has been made for publication bias. 47 Sample Size Unbiased Effect Size Figure 3.1.1 Funnel Plot of the Studies included in the Meta-Analysis. Table 3.3 presents the weighted mean, standard deviation, 95% confidence interval, and random variance component in each sub-group of each categorical moderator. Six of the sub-groups (linguistic problems, creative problems, absence of misleading cues, absence of relevant cues, high cognitive load task, unoccupied incubation period) had larger-than-zero random variance components. New weightings, under the assumption of a random effects model, were computed for each of the sub-groups. Weighted least-squares regression analyses were carried out to find the moderators that accounted for the effect size variability among these sub-groups. Small numbers of studies using creative tasks and studies having unoccupied incubation periods preclude the possibility of regression analyses with these moderators. An effect of applying a weighting to this regression analysis is to under-estimate the original standard error of each unstandardized coefficient. Thus, an adjusted standard error was computed by dividing the original standard error by the square root of the mean square 48 residual, a procedure suggested by Lipsey and Wilson (2001). The corrected standard error was used in the significance test (z-test) of each unstandardized coefficient (B). Tables 3.4, 3.5, 3.6, and 3.7 present a summary of the regression analysis results of each sub-group. 49 Table 3.3 The Random Variance Component, Weighted Mean, Standard Deviation, Standard Error, and 95% Confidence Interval of the Effect Size Estimate by Each Categorical Moderator Problem Type Misleading Cues Incubation Task Cues Linguistic Verbal Creativea Yes Noa High Load Low Load Resta Yes No Cuea 65 35 14 29 85 76 22 16 32 82 .00281 0 .37418 0 .12004 .06478 0 .30000 0 .10409 Mean .22 .26 .29 .35 .32 .24 .52 .46 .24 .34 Standard Deviation .05 .08 .14 .09 .06 .05 .10 .18 .09 .06 .13,.32 .10, .41 .13, .35 .32, .72 .11, .82 .07, .41 .22, .45 Number of studies Random Variance Component 95% Confidence Interval .02, .55 .17, .53 .20, .43 Mean comparison with the t(77) = -.43 t(47) = -.19 t(112) = .30 t(90) = 1.17 t(36) = .28 t(112) = .90 reference group Note. The lower confidence intervals of all the effects are above zero, suggesting that the mean is significant larger than zero, p < .05. a The reference groups in mean comparisons. 50 Table 3.4 Regression Model for Linguistic Problem Studies (N= 65) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 3.15* .01 ANOVA significance test Model 13.42 5 2.68 Residual 5.24 59 0.85 Total 63.66 64 Summary of the Regression Model Corrected β B z score SEβ Incubation Task High cognitive load task .06 .06 .21 0.27 Low cognitive load task .54* .43 .25 2.15 .15 .17 .12 1.24 .15 .12 .19 0.80 -.04 -.04 .13 -0.30 Misleading Cues Ration of the preparation period to the incubation period Relevant Cue Note. Random Variance Component = .00281, R2 = .21, Another regression with the same predicting variables, except replacing the variable “ration of length of the preparation to the incubation period” by the variables “Incubation Period” and “Preparation Period”, was carried out. The pattern of the results was comparable but it had lower explanatory power, and lower significant level, R2 = .20, F(6, 58) = 2.47, p = .03. Neither the variable “preparation period” nor “incubation period” was significant. ANOVA = analysis of variance. *p < .05. 51 Table 3.5 Regression Model for Misleading Cue Studies (N= 85) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 3.56* .002 ANOVA significance test Model 2.25 7 2.89 Residual 62.62 77 0.81 Total 82.87 84 Summary of the Regression Model Corrected β B z score SEβ Problem Type Visual problem -.59* -.51 .19 -3.02 Linguistic problem -.31 -.29 .18 -1.74 High cognitive load task -.18 -.14 .22 -0.80 Low cognitive load task .06 .04 .26 0.23 Length of the Incubation Period < .001 .055 < .001 0.45 Length of the Preparation Period .03* .36 .01 2.16 Relevant Cue -.03 -.02 .16 -0.20 Incubation Task Note. Random Variance Component = .12004, R2 = .24. ANOVA = analysis of variance. *p < .05. 52 Table 3.6 Regression Model on No Relevant Cue Studies (N = 82) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 7.35* < .001 ANOVA significance test Model 29.34 6 4.89 Residual 49.94 75 0.67 Total 79.28 81 Summary of the Regression Model Corrected β B z score SEβ Problem Type -.79** -.66 .19 -4.10 -.22 -.21 .18 1.24 High cognitive load task -.25 -.22 .21 -1.20 Low cognitive load task .16 .12 .25 0.64 < .001 .02 0 Visual problem Linguistic problem Incubation Task Length of the Incubation 0.13 Period Length of the Preparation 3.01 .05** .51 .02 .09 .07 .18 Period Misleading Cue 0.53 Note. Random Variance Component = .10409, R2 = .37. ANOVA = analysis of variance. *p < .05. **p < .001. 53 Table 3.7 Regression Model on High Cognitive-Load Incubation Task Studies (N= 75) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 1.53 .19 ANOVA significance test Model 7.36 6 1.47 Residual 66.19 69 0.96 Total 73.55 74 Summary of the Regression Model Corrected β B z score SEβ Problem Type Visual problem -.41* -.34 .20 -2.01 Linguistic problem -.28 -.30 .17 -1.58 Length of the Incubation -0.87 -.01 -.12 .01 Period Length of the 1.31 .02 .21 .02 Misleading cue .08 .06 .16 0.51 Relevant cue .09 .08 .14 0.62 Preparation Period Note. Random Variance Component = .06478, R2 = .10. ANOVA = analysis of variance. *p < .05. 54 With the sub-group of studies using linguistic problems, low cognitive load tasks generated larger incubation effects than rest alone, = .54, p < .05. Also, there was an interaction between Problem Type and Incubation Task with this sub-group, such that that low cognitive load tasks facilitated the incubation effect only when solving linguistic problems. With the “absence of misleading cue”, and “absence of relevant cue” sub-groups, regression analyses reveal that, in the absence of these cues, individuals solving visual problems had smaller incubation effects than those solving creative and linguistic problems. There was also a positive impact of longer Preparation Periods on the incubation effect sizes. The cognitive load of the incubation tasks did not have any impact on the magnitude of the incubation effects in these two sub-groups. Note, however, that the presence or absence of these effects with these specific sub-groups does not necessarily imply the converse effects found with other sub-groups, hence a shift to analysis of general impacts of each moderator. In the second stage of the analysis, a weighted least-squares regression analysis was carried out to look at the general impact of each moderator on the incubation effect sizes. A summary of the regression analysis results is presented in Table 3.8. The negative coefficients associated with “visual problem” and “linguistic problem” indicate that individuals solving these two types of insight problem showed a smaller incubation effect than individuals solving creative problems. A z-test was carried out to compare the coefficients of “visual problem” and “linguistic problem”. The result was not statistically significant, z score = -1.25, p > .05, suggesting the magnitude of the incubation effect for visual and linguistic insight problems was comparable. 55 Table 3.8 Regression Model on All Studies (N = 114) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 4.08 < .001 ANOVA significance test Model 25.56 8 3.20 Residual 82.22 105 0.78 Total 107.79 113 Summary of the Regression Model Corrected β B z score SEβ Problem Type Visual problem -.60** -.52 .15 -3.61 Linguistic problem -.31* -.31 .14 -1.96 .16 .13 .12 1.16 .14 .10 .18 0.72 -.16 -.15 .14 -1.02 .03* .35 .01 2.29 < .001 .04 < .001 < .001 -.01 -.01 .11 -0.09 Misleading cues Incubation Task Low cognitive-load task High cognitive-load task Length of the Preparation Period Length of the Incubation Period Cue presented Note. Random Variance Component = .0834, R2 = .24. ANOVA = analysis of variance. *p < .05. **p < .001. 56 The length of preparation period was found to have a significant impact on the magnitude of the incubation effect, β = .03, p < .05. Three bivariate correlations were carried out to check for positive relationships between the length of preparation period and the magnitude of the weighted incubation effect when solving the three types of problem. There was a statistically significant positive correlation between the weighted incubation effect size and the length of preparation period with visual problems, r (35) = .40, p = .02, and creative problems, r (14) = .60, p = .03, but not with linguistic problems, r (65) = -.04, p = .75. Another weighted least-squares regression analysis was carried out to examine the interaction between Problem Type and Incubation Task, using Misleading Cues, Preparation Period, Incubation Period, Cues, and an appropriate number of dummy vectors and the multiplicative terms of the variables Problem Type and Incubation Task as predictor variables. Table 3.9 presents the results of this regression analysis. To examine the interaction effects, the coefficient differences between their multiplicative terms were examined to see if they were significantly larger than zero by using z-tests. The details of equations for computing the coefficient difference can be found in Table D1 in Appendix D. Table 3.10 presents the coefficient differences between multiplicative terms. 57 Table 3.9 Regression Model on All Studies (with interactive terms, N= 114) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 4.15 < .001 ANOVA significance test Model 35.60 12 2.97 Residual 72.18 101 .72 Total 107.78 113 Summary of the Regression Model Problem Type x Incubation Corrected β B Task z score SEβ Visual problem x Low 0.17 0.08 .35 0.50 0.90* 0.61 .35 2.60 0.72 0.43 .38 1.93 1.00* 0.97 .38 2.59 Visual problem -1.29** -1.10 .32 -4.00 Linguistic problem -1.24** -1.21 .40 -3.14 High cognitive load task -0.91** -0.81 .30 -3.00 Low cognitive load task -0.12 -0.09 .33 -0.38 0.24 0.19 .15 1.54 < .001 .018 < .001 < 0.001 cognitive load task Visual problem x High cognitive load task Linguistic problem x Low cognitive load task Linguistic problem x High cognitive load task Incubation Task Misleading Cues Length of the Incubation 58 Period Length of the preparation .03* .01 .01 1.97 .03 .03 .13 .26 period Answer was presented Note. Random Variance Component = .08340, R2 = .33. ANOVA = analysis of variance. *p < .05. **p < .001. 59 Table 3.10 Summary of Coefficient Differences of the Regression Model on All Studies (with interactive terms, N= 114) The coefficient The multiplicative terms Corrected SE z score difference Lij Hj – Lij Rj 0.08 .49 0.17 LijL j- LijRj 0.60 .50 1.21 LijL j- LijHj 0.52 .70 0.74 InjHj - InjRj -0.01 .46 -0.03 InjL j- InjRj 0.05 .48 0.10 InjL j- InjHj 0.06 .66 0.09 CjHj - CjRj -0.91* .30 -3.00 CjL j- CjRj -0.12 .33 -0.38 CjL j- CjH 0.79+ .45 1.77 InjHj- CjHj -0.39 .47 -0.82 LijHj - CjHj -0.25 .55 -0.45 LijHj- InjHj 0.14 .72 0.19 InjL j- CjLj -1.11* .47 -2.36 LijL j- CjLj -0.52 .55 -0.95 LijL j- InjL j 0.60 .72 0.82 Inj Rj - Cj Rj -1.29** .32 -4.00 Lij Rj - Cj Rj -1.24** .40 -3.14 Lij Rj - Inj Rj 0.04 .51 0.08 Note. C = creative problem; Li = linguistics problem; V = visual problem; R = rest; L = low cognitive demand task; H = high cognitive demand task; j= study j. + p = .08. *p < .05. **p < .001. 60 With creative problems, undertaking high cognitive load tasks was associated with smaller incubation effects than with low cognitive load tasks or rest during the incubation period, CjHj -CjRj = -.91, p < .05, and CjHj. - CjLj = -.79 , p = .08 . When solving linguistic and visual problems, no differences were found for incubation periods filled with low or high cognitive load tasks or rest. However, this regression model has a problem in exploring the interaction between Problem Type and Incubation Task. Among studies examining the role of an incubation period with creative problems, 12 out of 14 studies employed high cognitive load tasks and the remaining 2 studies employed rest during the incubation period. This unbalanced distribution may cause bias when examining the interaction between Incubation task and Problem Type. Hence, another regression analysis was carried out that excluded studies using creative problems. This third regression model included the variables Misleading Cues, Preparation Period, Cues, the dummy variables of Problem Type (excluding Creative problem) and Incubation Task, and their multiplicative variables. Appendix E presents a detailed description of the selection of the dummy coded vectors and the multiplicative terms, and the equations for computing the coefficient difference between the multiplicative terms. The results of this analysis is presented in Table 3.11, and the coefficient differences between the multiplicative terms are presented in Table 3.12 61 Table 3.11 Regression Model for Studies Excluding Creative Problem (N = 100) Variable SS df Mean Square F p 2.04* .05 ANOVA significance test Model 15.04 8 1.88 Residual 83.92 91 0.92 Total 98.96 99 Summary of the Regression Model Corrected β B z score SEβ Problem Type x Incubation Task Visual problem x Low -.21 -.12 .28 -0.77 .09 .08 .24 0.38 -.15 -.15 .21 -0.68 High cognitive load task -.01 -.01 .18 -0.05 Low cognitive load task .45* .41 .20 2.27 .13 .13 .11 1.10 .08 .20 .05 1.56 cognitive load task Visual problem x High cognitive load task Problem Type Visual problem Incubation Task Misleading Cues Length of the preparation period/incubation 62 Answer was presented .01 .01 .11 0.13 Note. Random Variance Component = .00031, R2 = .15, Another regression with the same predicting variables, except dividing the variable “ration of length of the preparation to the incubation period” to the variables “Incubation Period” and “Preparation Period”, was carried out. But the regression model was not significant, F(9, 90) = 1.27, p = .10. Neither the variable “preparation period” nor “incubation period” was significant. ANOVA= analysis of variance. *p < .05. Table 3.12 The Summary of Coefficient Differences of the Regression Model Excluding Creative Problem (N= 100) The multiplicative The coefficient Corrected SE z score terms difference LijHj – LijRj -.01 .17 -0.05 LijL j- LijRj .45* .20 2.27 Lij Lj - LijHj .46 .26 1.76 VjHj - VjRj .08 .30 0.28 VjL j- VjRj .24 .34 0.69 VjLj- VjHj .15 .45 0.34 VjHj - LiHj -.06 .32 -0.17 VjL j- LijLj -.36 .35 -1.03 Vj Rj - LijRj -.15 .21 -0.68 Note. Li = linguistics problem; V = visual problem; R = rest; L = low cognitive demand task; H = high cognitive demand task; j= study j. *p < .05. 63 The regression results indicate an interaction between Problem Type and Incubation Task. When solving linguistic problems, a low cognitive-load task generated significantly larger incubation effects than rest, LijLj -LijRj = .45, p = .05. The difference between low and high cognitive-loads was in the same direction but did not reach significance, and there was no significant difference between the rest condition and high cognitive load condition. When solving visual problems, the effect sizes among the three incubation conditions were comparable. This pattern of findings is consistent with the previous regression analysis results. In addition, the exclusion of creative problem studies makes the positive impact of a low cognitive-load incubation period on linguistic problems more significant. The model using the variables “Preparation Period” and “Incubation Period” was found to be not significant, and the variable “Preparation Period” was not significant in the analysis. This may be due to the decrease in the numbers of studies included in the current regression model. Moreover, as mentioned above, a positive association between the length of preparation period and incubation effect size was found when solving visual and creative problems. Thus, the exclusion of creative problem studies appears to decrease the significance of the variable “Preparation Period”, and the significance level of the regression model using the “Preparation Period” as one of the predictive variables. Discussion The meta-analysis supports the existence of incubation effects, and also identifies some potential moderators, including the problem type, length of preparation period, and the incubation task. Individuals solving creative problems were more likely to benefit from an incubation period than individuals solving linguistic and visual 64 problems. Longer preparation periods gave rise to larger incubation effects. When solving linguistic problems, a low cognitive-load task gave the strongest incubation effects. It is also suggested that the positive incubation effects found with creative problems are a direct reflection of their multi-solution nature. When solving a creative problem, individuals benefit from performing a wide search of their knowledge to identify as many relevant connections as possible with the presented stimuli. Each time an individual re-approaches the problem, performance is improved by extending search to previously unexplored areas of their knowledge network. Incubation appears to facilitate the widening of search of a knowledge network in this fashion. Linguistic and visual problems typically have only one possible solution. In order to solve them, individuals have to explore their memory or environment to look for specific relevant knowledge or to adapt a specific strategy. Widening search to new items of knowledge may not be facilitative if the solution to a problem lies within already activated knowledge that is currently represented inappropriately. Under this account, incubation supports knowledge activation, but it does not support restructuring. Another finding of the meta-analysis was the beneficial effect of an incubation period filled with low demand tasks on solving linguistic problems. A positive effect of a filled incubation period on problem-solving compared with rest during the incubation period undermines the conscious-work hypothesis that incubation effects are due to the mental fatigue reduction (Posner, 1973). There remains a possibility, of course, that a sufficiently light load might allow additional covert problem solving compared with a heavier task load (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Posner, 1973), but this does not explain why a light load should be more facilitative than rest alone. 65 The positive effect of a light cognitive load may indicate competition between controlled and automatic processes in solving linguistic problems. It has been suggested with remote associates task performance that only strong (and in this context, incorrect) associates are accessed when individuals focus their attention on seeking solutions, whereas remote associates are more likely to be accessed when an individual’s cognitive resources are allocated in a diffuse manner (Ansburg & Hill, 2001; Finke, Ward, & Smith, 1992; Martindale, 1995). During an incubation period, low-demand tasks may occupy part of the problem solvers’ attention, preventing the focused concentration that yields strong associates. Resting during an incubation period may allow individuals to continue consciously working on the problem, while performing high-demand tasks may shift attention entirely to that interpolated task, leading to a narrow rather than diffused attentional focus. The impact of performing high-demand tasks is analogous to the ‘verbal overshadowing’ effect reported by Schooler and colleagues (Schooler, Ohlsson, & Brookes, 1993) in which the act of verbalizing can impair performance by focusing individuals’ attention inappropriately on verbalisable components of a task. The suggested role of the light-load incubation tasks receives indirect support from recent findings which show that that visual search can be more efficient when performed concurrently with an unrelated task than when performed alone (Smilek, Enns, Eastwood, & Merikle, 2006). They suggest that the dual-task condition prevents a narrow attentional focus in searching stimuli. With visual problems, the magnitude of the incubation effects was independent on the setting of an incubation period (filled or unfilled). Differences between visual and linguistic insight problems may arise through a greater reliance on strategic search rather than knowledge activation in the former than the latter. MacGregor, Ormerod, and Chronicle (2001) proposed that in solving the nine-dot problem, 66 individuals select and execute moves that maximally reduce the distance between current and goal states, essentially drawing lines that connect as many dots as possible. While there remain moves available that satisfy a criterion of satisfactory progress (in this case, the ratio of dots cancelled to lines available), individuals will persevere with an initial representation of the problem that, in the case of the nine-dot problem, does not include consideration of space outside the dot array. According to MacGregor et al.’s account, individuals must experience a failure to find moves that meet a criterion of satisfactory progress before they change the initial representation of the problem, thereby including space outside the dot array in their attempts. An incubation period would be helpful only if the problem solvers became aware of the necessity of a strategy shift, but according to MacGregor et al. they are unlikely to do so unless they encounter criterion failure as a result of reaching impasse. Seifert, Mayer, Davidson, Palatino, and Yaniv (1995) offer an alternative account that also points to the criticality of experiencing failure and impasse for eventual success in insight problem-solving. If the hypothesis that visual problems require impasse for a strategy switch to occur is correct, a long preparation period (i.e., pre-incubation problem-solving) should be more likely to yield benefits from subsequent incubation with visual problems because it allows individuals to reach impasse prior to incubation. The results of the regression analysis and the follow-up bivariate correlations are consistent with this prediction, showing a statistically significant positive correlation between the incubation effect size and the length of preparation period with visual problems. A positive correlation between length of preparation period and incubation effect size was also found with creative problems. A long preparation period may 67 allow individuals to exhaust search in one domain, making it more likely for them to explore a new domain in the second phase of solving. A positive correlation was not found when solving linguistic problems, though this may simply reflect the small variability in length of preparation period among studies using linguistic problems (the preparation period of 82% of these studies ranged from 0.5 to 1 minute). The meta-analysis reveals that embedding misleading cues to the problems was not a significant predictor overall. This result is in contrast with previous reports (e.g., Smith et al., 1989), which suggest that incubation effects arise through forgetting of inappropriate information. The true effect of misleading cues is underestimated in our regression analyses, as we only examined the overall effect of misleading cues on problem solving in general. Twenty nine independent studies included in this meta-analysis examined the impact of misleading cues; 25 of them were examining the impact on linguistic problems, the rest on visual problems. The weighted mean of the effect size estimates of these studies for each problem type by the presence of misleading cues were: Linguistic with Misleading Cue: M = .36, SD = .09; Linguistic without Misleading Cue: M = .17, SD = .06; Visual without Misleading Cue: M = .26, SD = .08; Visual with Misleading Cue: M = .18, SD = .39. For studies employing linguistic problems, the presence of misleading cues induced a larger incubation effect, but an opposite pattern of results was found in studies employing visual problems, suggesting that the impact of misleading cues may be modality-specific. In order to test whether the presence of misleading cues affects incubation with linguistic problems alone, a weighted one-way analysis of variance was run to compare the incubation effect sizes of studies using linguistic problems that included misleading cues (25 studies) against studies using linguistic problems that did not include misleading cues (40 studies), F(1, 65) = 3.04, MSE = 3.00, p = .08. Thus, 68 where problem materials involve linguistic stimuli, getting rid of misleading concepts may be the key to task solution, in contrast with visual problems in which the key may be to restructure the knowledge that is currently active. The effect of misleading cues also offers some support for the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis, but this effect may be task-specific. In contrast with previous reports (e.g., Dominowski, 1972; Dreitadt, 1969; Mednick, Mednick, & Mednick, 1964), the moderator Presence of Cues was not found to be a significant predictor of incubation effects. It has been hypothesized that, during an incubation period, unconscious processes such as spreading activation sensitize individuals to solution concepts and make them more likely to utilise externally presented cues. To examine this hypothesis, researchers have presented the answers of unsolved problems during an incubation period, and compared post-incubation performance with participants not receiving any cues during the incubation period (e.g., Dodds, Smith, & Ward, 2002; Dominowski & Jenrick, 1972). Findings of these studies are equivocal, but it does appear that problem solvers do not always make use of solution relevant cues, even when the cue includes the solution itself. For example, Chronicle, Ormerod, and MacGregor (2001) found that presenting the nine-dot problem with a shaded background in the shape of the solution did not lead to significant levels of facilitation, even when the relevance of the shading was drawn to participants’ attention. However, because of the small number of studies that present solution relevant cues (three with linguistic, seven with visual problems, none with creative problems) and the wide variation in experimental parameters among these studies, it is impossible to carry out further statistical analysis. Overall, the meta-analysis results support the existence of incubation effects, though there appears to be a range of effects specific to particular tasks and 69 performance conditions. When attempting creative problems that require a wide search of knowledge, individuals benefit from an incubation period. Problems that involve reaching some kind of insight into a unique solution do not always benefit from incubation under all conditions. In the case of linguistic problems, such as the RAT, there is a modest incubation effect but only where the incubation period is filled with a low cognitive demand task. One possible explanation is that performing low cognitive load incubation tasks allows the occurrence of some unconscious problem solving processes, such as spreading-activation and selective forgetting. In the case of visual problems, incubation effects arise only where there has been a sufficiently long preparation period prior to incubation for the problem-solver to have entered a state of impasse. Only under these conditions can an incubation period contribute to the strategic shift needed to restructure a problem representation. Thus, the theoretical positions of spreading activation, selective forgetting and restructuring each receive support. However, evidence for each appears to be specific to particular problem types. The finding of a positive impact of an incubation period on solving creative thinking problems supports the contention that incubation periods help the elicitation of new ideas. Incubation is a concept central to many methodologies for encouraging creative decision-making, especially among management science and business communities (e.g., Rickards, 1991), and this result may be taken as supporting the inclusion of an incubation phase in such methodologies. It should be noted that, despite efforts to include studies from sources other than peer-reviewed journals, the meta-analysis may be influenced by a bias in favour of reporting significant effects at the expense of null effects. Thus, incubation effects may be to some extent overstated in this meta-analysis, a problem common to all 70 meta-analyses. Nonetheless, the reasonably large effect sizes found with creative problems indicate that, with this class of problem at any rate, incubation is a potentially valuable mechanism for fostering creative thought. Clearly, the empirical data on incubation are not straightforward. As a consequence, the traditional narrative review approach is not amenable to drawing strong cross-study conclusions. It is perhaps disappointing that relatively few published studies met the necessary criteria for inclusion in the meta-analysis, since a failure either to measure post-incubation performance or to provide effect size information limits the conclusions that can be drawn from them. Nonetheless, sufficient studies remain for the meta-analysis to be undertaken and to reveal some intriguing results. One remaining problem is the relatively narrow range of problem types that have been explored. For instance, the majority of studies that explore incubation effects with linguistic problems, which is the majority of studies overall, use the RAT. It is unclear that whether the RAT can be considered an insight problem or a linguistic completion task, suggesting it may not be representative of all linguistic problem-solving tasks. Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) have found that participants sometimes claimed that they solved RATs with insight, while sometimes reported that they solved them without insight. Further studies should aim to explore task-specific experimental settings for maximizing the incubation effect with a wider range of tasks. On the whole, the results of this meta-analysis support the existence of multiple types of problem-specific incubation effect, and suggest that the concept of incubation can only be understood through a close examination of the problems to which it is applied and the conditions under which it is elicited. Therefore, two sets of experiments 71 were carried out to examine the incubation effect on linguistic and visual insight problems separately. The following section presents the findings of the first set of experiments (Experiments I, II, and III) investigating the incubation effect on linguistic insight problem. In these three experiments, the impact was examined of the presence of misleading cues and the loading of incubation tasks on the occurrence of the three proposed mechanisms: spreading-activation, selective-forgetting, and cue-assimilation. To have a more specific measure of the incubation effect, both performance improvement and the change of sensitivity to relevant and irrelevant memory items were assessed. As a response to the problem of the narrow range of problem types in the past studies, two types of linguistic insight problem, RATs (Experiment I & II) and Rebuses (Experiment III), were adopted. 72 CHAPTER IV: EXPERIMENTS I, II, and III - LINGUISTIC INSIGHT PROBLEMS Three experiments examining linguistic insight problems were carried out in an attempt to discriminate between three hypotheses of unconscious incubation, and to examine the impact of incubation tasks and the presence of misleading cues on incubation effects in more detail. The traditional paradigm in incubation research, which only measures the incubation effect on solution performance, offers weak data to test between the three unconscious hypotheses. Therefore, in these three experiments, besides measuring performance change, lexical decision tasks (LDT) were also adopted to assess participants’ sensitivity towards relevant items and misleading cues embedded in the problems, before and after an incubation period. Decreased lexical decision latency to relevant items would support the spreading-activation hypothesis, while an increase in lexical decision latency to misleading cues would support the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis. Also, if spreading activation and selective forgetting are the sources of incubation effects, then enhanced performance on problems should be accompanied by a change of sensitivity to relevant items and misleading cues. The presentation of lexical decision tasks containing solution-relevant words could also serve as a facilitatory cue to the problem. The Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis suggests that incubation periods help problem restructuring and increase the chance of assimilating external cues. If this is the case, then facilitation by the presentation of cues should be greater in the incubation condition than in the No-Incubation condition. To examine the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, 73 participants’ performance on RATs in the cue-plus-incubation, the incubation-alone, the cue-alone conditions was compared. EXPERIMENT I Experiment I investigated whether an incubation period can enhance performance in solving Remote Associates Tasks (RATs), and examined two proposed unconscious hypotheses: Opportunistic Assimilation and Spreading Activation. To verify the Spreading-Activation hypothesis, a Lexical Decision Task (LDT) was adapted as a supplementary measure to assess if there was any change in participants’ sensitivity to relevant memory concepts after an incubation period. The presentation of the LDTs could also serve as an implicit cue to the problem, and comparing the post-incubation performance improvement in the presence and absence of cues enabled an examination of the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis. The co-examination of these two proposed mechanisms in one experiment also allows us to investigate how incubation can enhance cue assimilation. The Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis suggests that during the incubation period, the problem representation will be re-structured into a more appropriate form, which will increase the likelihood of assimilating the chance-encountered cue afterwards. However, the hypothesis does not predict the nature of the restructuring. Yaniv and Meyer (1987) have suggested that spreading activation occurs during the incubation period to sensitize individuals to the relevant memory items, and this will subsequently facilitate the cue-assimilation. Yet, no studies have previously been conducted to examine this proposal. The data of Experiment I should determine if there is any association between cue-assimilation and spreading-activation. 74 Method Participants Forty-eight (37 females, 11 males) first-year Psychology students from Lancaster University participated to gain course credits. The mean age was 19.33 with a standard deviation of 1.48. All were native English speakers. Materials Remote Associates Tasks (RATs; Mednick, 1962) were used as the problem-solving task in this study. In each RAT, three apparently unrelated words are presented, and participants have to think of a fourth word that forms an association with each of the three words. For example, if the three stimulus words of a RAT are “blue”, “cake” and “cottage”, the fourth word can be “cheese”. Sixteen RATs were selected from a pool developed by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003). The RATs were randomly divided into two sets, Set A and B. The reported solution rates ranged between 10%-31%, with a mean of 17.75% (SD = 6.32) for Set A, and 22.63% (SD = 8.18) for Set B. The mean reported solution rate of these two sets of RATs was then compared, and the difference was not significant, t(15) = 1.33, p = .20, Coden’s d = .69. The purpose of preparing two different sets of RATs with a similar level of difficulty is to check that measured effects are not item-specific (cf. the language as fixed effects fallacy: Clark, 1973). Appendix F presents the RATs given in Set A and B. Lexical Decision Tasks (LDTs) were adapted to examine participants’ level of sensitivity to relevant solution concepts. A set of LDTs consisted of five items including neutral words, pseudo-words, and the answer of the previously-presented 75 RAT (target words). All presented words were high frequency words (an occurrence of 90 or more in a million). Mental Rotation Tasks (MRTs) and Arithmetic Tasks (ATs) were used as incubation tasks. Each MRT consisted of a pair of objects. Participants had to judge if the objects in each pair were identical. Each of the MRTs created for the purpose of this study consisted of a pair of three-dimensional objects. Each object consisted of 11 solid cubes attached face-to-face to form a rigid arm-like structure with exactly three right-angled “elbows”. The object placed on the left hand side was the standard stimulus and the object on the right hand side was the comparison stimulus. In half the MRTs, the comparison stimulus was identical to the standard stimulus, whereas in the other half the comparison stimulus was the mirror image of the standard stimulus. The comparison stimulus was either rotated clockwise or anti-clockwise around the x-axis, y-axis, or z-axis with an angle. The rotation angle was 60, 80, 100, 120, 140, 160, or 180 degrees relative to the orientation of the standard stimulus. The ATs comprised three-digit additions and subtractions without regrouping. Design This experiment employed a 2 x 3 mixed factorial design. The within-subjects factor was whether LDTs were presented before or after the second attempt to the RAT. The between-subjects factor was the incubation condition with three levels: No Incubation (NI), Incubation filled with MRTs and ATs (IN),and Rest/Unfilled Incubation (RE) conditions. In the NI condition, after the completion of the first attempt at a RAT, participants were immediately prompted to do an LDT set followed by a second attempt to the same RAT(or vice versa), depending on whether it was an LDT-before (1st, 2nd, 5th, and 8th) or LDT-after (3rd, 4th,6th, and 7th) trial. In the IN 76 condition, participants were prompted to perform MRTs and ATs for two minutes before proceeding to complete the LDT set or the second attempt to the same RAT. In the RE condition, participants were prompted to sit quietly and listen to soft music for two minutes before proceeding to complete the LDT set or re-approach the same RAT. The inclusion of the RE condition served the purpose of assessing if any measured changes in RAT performance and lexical latency were due to performing incubation tasks or simply due to having a two-minute rest between the two attempts. Procedure All the task instructions and stimuli were presented on a computer using bespoke software. Before the start of the experiment, participants in the NI and RE conditions were informed that the main experiment involved solving RATs and LDTs. Participants were then given instructions on how to complete both tasks and were reminded to finish the tasks as quickly and accurately as possible. After this, they were prompted to solve 1 RAT and 2 LDTs for practice. Participants in the IN condition were informed that the main experiment involved solving RATs, LDTs, MRTs, and ATs. They were then given the task instructions and were reminded to finish all the tasks as quickly and accurately as possible. They were then prompted to solve 1 RAT, 1 MRT, 1 AT, and 2 LDTs for practice. After the practice phase, the main experiment started. During the main experiment, eight trials were presented in the same sequence for each participant. In each trial, the three stimulus words of a RAT were presented horizontally across the centre of the screen. Participants could respond at any time within the 30-second presentation time by first pressing the c key, and then speaking out their answer to a microphone connected to a tape-recorder. All the responses were 77 recorded for scoring of correct responses afterwards. Response latency was also measured as the duration from the onset of problem presented until the participant pressed c or until the presentation period ended. Once the participants spoke out the answer or the 30-second period had passed, participants had to press the c key again to proceed to the next task. Participants in the NI condition were then prompted to do an LDT set followed by a second attempt to the same RAT(or vice versa depending on whether it was a LDT-before or LDT-after trial). The five items in each LDT set were presented at the centre of the screen, one at a time. Before presenting each stimulus, a fixation cross was presented at the centre of the screen for 0.5 second to draw participants’ attention. Participants were prompted to press the z key if the item presented was a word or the m key if it was a pseudo-word. The first presented item was always a neutral word or pseudo-word, which served as a warm-up stimulus. The target word, along with pseudo-words and neutral words, was counter-balanced across the remaining four positions. In the RE Condition, participants were prompted to sit quietly and listen to soft music for two minutes before proceeding to complete the LDT set or the same RAT. In the Incubation condition, participants were prompted to complete MRTs for one minute and ATs for another minute (incubation period) before proceeding to complete the LDT set or the same RAT. In each MRT, two images were presented, one at the left and one at the right-hand side of the screen. Participants had to press the z key if they think the stimuli were identical, or press the m key if not. After participants responded, another MRT would be presented. The responses and the response latencies by participants were recorded by the computer. In each AT, participants had to write down their 78 answers on a sheet of paper, and then press the c key to proceed to next AT. In both tasks, the response time was measured as the duration from the onset of stimulus to the point that the participants press the c key. The number of MRTs and ATs presented during the incubation period varied among participants, depending on how quick the participant responded. Figure 4.1.1 presents the task presentation sequence in each trial in the three conditions. LDT No Incubation RAT RAT RAT Rest Condition: LDT LDT RAT RAT LDT (1st, 2nd, 5th, & 8th trials) MUSIC Condition: RAT (1st, 2nd, 5th, & 8th trials) (3rd, 4th, 6th, & 7th trials) Condition: Incubation RAT LDT RAT RAT LDT (3rd, 4th, 6th, &7th trials) (1st, 2nd, 5th, & 8th trials) MRT +AT (3rd, 4th, 6th, & 7th trials) Figure 4.1.12 Task Presentation Sequence in Each Trial in Each Condition in Experiment I. 79 Results Analyses of participants’ RAT performance and lexical decision latencies were carried out to examine the three predictions. The first prediction concerns the positive role of incubation effect on performance level. It was predicted that participants in the IN condition should demonstrate a larger performance improvement from the first attempt at the RAT problem to the second attempt, compared with participants in the other two conditions. The second prediction is based on the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, that an incubation period can facilitate problem restructuring and in turn increase the likelihood of cue assimilation. It was predicted that participants in the IN condition should show a larger performance improvement in the Cue Trials compared to the No-Cue trials, and this cueing effect should be less noticeable in the NI and RE conditions. Shifting from the incubation effect on performance to a qualitative change in cognitive state, the third prediction pertains to the occurrence of spreading activation during the incubation period. According to the Spreading-Activation hypothesis, participants in the IN condition should have significantly shorter lexical decision latencies to target words relative to the neutral words, and again, such differences should be less noticeable in the other two conditions. Background Statistics Before running the main analyses, participants’ performance in solving the RATs in Set A and B at the first attempt was compared to check the assumption that these two question sets were at the same difficulty level. Table 4.1.1 presents the means and the standard deviations of the number of correct responses and the response latency to the RATs at the first attempt by Question Set and Condition. 80 Table 4.1.113 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt by Question Set and Condition Number of Correct Responses Response Time Set A Set B Set A Set B M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) NI 0.89 (1.26) 1.44 (1.51) 27.87 (2.64) 27.15 (1.53) RE 0.57 (0.79) 1.25 (1.83) 28.99 (1.48) 27.42 (3.46) IN 1.00 (1.15) 0.50 (0.53) 28.05 (2.42) 29.04 (1.25) Condition Note. Incorrect responses or unanswered RATs were scored as taking the maximum permissible time (30 s). Two 2 (Question Set) x 3 (Condition) ANOVAs, using number of correct responses and response time as the dependent variables respectively, were carried out to compare participants’ performance in solving RATs in set A and B. Correct Responses: None of the effects was significant: Condition: F(2, 42) = .44, p = .51, p2 = .01; Question Set: F(1, 42) = .45, p = .64, p2 = .02; Condition x Question Set: F(2, 42) = .99, p = .38, p2 = .05. Response Time: Again, none of the effects was significant: Condition: F(2, 42) = .38, p = .54, p2 = .01; Question Set: F(1, 42) = .77, p = .47, p2 = .04; Condition x Question Set: F(2, 42) = 1.07, p = .35, p2 = .05. The non-significant ANOVA results confirm the presumption that the RATs in Set A and Set B were in similar level of difficulty. Hence, the data from both question sets were merged in all the subsequent analyses. Also, the non-significant Condition effect implies that there was no general problem-solving performance difference among participants in these three conditions. This ensures that any performance difference 81 found in the subsequent analysis could not be simply attributed to differences in the general problem solving ability. Incubation Effects on Problem Solving Performance Performance Improvement: If an incubation period can enhance problem-solving performance, then participants in the IN condition should show a larger performance improvement over the first attempt at the RATs, compared with the other two conditions. To assess performance improvement, an improvement score was computed for each participant, which was the proportion of RATs not solved at the first attempt that were solved at the second attempt. In order to reveal the net effect of an incubation period on performance improvement in the absence of any external cue, only data in the LDT-after (3rd, 4th, 6th, & 7th) trials were included in the analysis. One participant was excluded because of solving all the RATs presented in the LDT-after trials at the first attempt. Table 4.1.2 presents the means and the standard deviations of the number of correct responses in the first and the second attempts, and the improvement score, in each condition. 82 Table 4.1.214 Mean and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Two Attempts and Improvement Score by Condition First Attempt Second Attempt Improvement Score M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) 0.63 (0.91) 0.92 (1.03) .11 (.23) NI 0.61 (0.92) 1.00 (1.32) .14 (.26) RE 0.73 (1.22) 0.93 (1.33) .11 (.27) IN 0.53 (0.52) 0.80 (0.56) .07 (.13) Condition Overall Note. The number of correct responses in the second attempt is the sum of the number of RATs solved in the first attempt and the number of newly solved RATs in the second attempt. The overall improvement score, M = .11, SD = .23, was significantly larger than zero, t(47) = 3.20, p = .002, Cohen’s d = 0.93. This is expected because extra problem solving-time should enhance performance. The core question is whether there was any significant difference in the improvement scores among the three conditions. A one-way ANOVA, using Condition as the independent variable and improvement score as the dependent variable, was carried out. The Condition effect was not significant, F(2, 44) = .39, p = .68, ηp2 = .017, implying that the degree of improvement was comparable among the three conditions. This does not support the prediction that an incubation period can enhance problem-solving performance. The non-significant incubation effect on performance may be due to the fact that the post-incubation performance improvement, not only relies on the occurrence of relevant internal cognitive processes during an incubation period, but also on the external environment such as the presence of relevant external cues. 83 Opportunistic Assimilation: The Opportunistic-Assimilation account suggests that incubation periods provide time for the problem solver to restructure the mental representation of the unsolved problem into a better form that could increase the chance of assimilating newly encounter-external cues into the problem. In other words, the magnitude of the incubation effect depends on whether problem solvers encounter relevant external cues during the incubation period. To verify this hypothesis, the degree of RAT performance improvement in the presence and absence of external cues (LDTs) in the three conditions was examined. The Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis predicts that participants in the IN condition should show a significant performance improvement in the Cue Trials compared to the No-Cue trials, and the facilitation of the presentation of the cue should be less noticeable in the NI and RE conditions. The mean number of RATs solved at the first and second attempts in the Cue (LDT-before:1st, 2nd, 5th , & 8th) and No-Cue (LDT-after:3rd, 4th, 6th, & 7th) trials is presented in Table 4.1.3. Table 4.1.315 Mean and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Cue and No-Cue trials in Each Condition. First Attempt Second Attempt Cue No Cue Cue No Cue M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) NI .61 (0.92) .56 (.70) 1.00 (1.08) 1.00 (1.08) RE .73 (1.22) .20 (.41) 0.93 (1.33) 1.00 (1.13) IN .53 (0.52) .20 (.53) 0.80 (0.56) 0.46 (0.74) Condition 84 For each participant, the improvement score in Cue and No-Cue trials, and the differences between these two scores (Cue – No-Cue), were calculated. One participant was excluded because he/she solved all the RATs presented in the LDT-after trials at the first attempt. Table 4.1.4 presents the means and the standard deviations of the improvement scores in the presence and the absence of the cues, and the differences between them, in each condition. Table 4.1.416 Means and Standard Deviations of Improvement Score on the Cue and No-Cue trials, and the Score Differences by Condition Cue No Cue Cueing Effect (Improvement Score on Cue – No Cue) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) NI 0.15 (0.27) 0.14 (0.26) < .01 (.34) RE 0.16 (0.21) 0.11 (0.27) .05 (.30) IN 0.07 (0.15) 0.07 0(.13) -.01 (.12) Condition It was predicted that the cueing effect, indicated by the improvement score difference between the Cue and the No-Cue trials, should be most noticeable in the IN condition. This prediction was tested by carrying out a 2 (Cue) x 3 (Condition) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Cue (No-Cue vs. Cue) and using improvement score as DV. Table 4.1.5 presents the summary of the ANOVA results. 85 Table 4.1.517 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Condition and Cue on Improvement Scores. Source df MS F p ηp2 .84 .44 .04 Between-Subjects Condition 2 .05 Error ( between) 44 .06 Within-Subjects Cue 1 .007 .19 .67 < .01 Cue x Condition 2 .007 .19 .83 .01 Error (within) 44 .04 Neither the main effects nor the interaction effect was significant, Cue: F(1, 44) = .19, p = .67, p2 = .04; Condition: F(1,44) = .84, p = .44, p2 = .04; Cue x Condition: F(2, 44) = .19, p = .82, p2 = .09. This result indicates that, in all conditions, the mean proportion of improvement in the Cue trials and No-Cue trials did not differ significantly. Participants in the IN condition were not more likely to assimilate the cues to solve the problems. The Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis is therefore not supported here. The analyses of performance improvement failed to confirm that an incubation period can enhance problem solving performance, either directly or indirectly via cue assimilation. However, the null effect on performance change in this experiment does not necessarily imply the absence of any unconscious problem-solving processes during the incubation period. It may be that spreading-activation occurred during the incubation period in this experiment to increase the activation of the relevant memory, but the effect was too small to have any impact on the performance level. To address 86 this possibility of the occurrence of spreading-activation during the incubation period, participants’ sensitivity to relevant items among the three conditions was compared. Incubation Effects on Knowledge Activation Spreading Activation: If spreading activation occurred during the incubation period, then participants in the IN condition should be more sensitive to target words relative to the neutral words, reflected in a quicker lexical decision times; and this difference should be smaller in the other conditions. To test this prediction, participants’ lexical decision times for target and neutral words in the LDT-before (1st, 2nd, 5th, and 8th) trials in the three conditions were examined. Accuracy on the LDT was 93% or higher in all conditions. Incorrect lexical decisions and correct decisions with extreme lexical decision times (< 50 ms or > 2500ms) were discarded (4.5% of the data). After that, the mean and standard deviation of the lexical latencies were calculated for each participant. Within each participant, lexical decision times longer than 2SDs from the mean were re-coded to the value found at 2SD. The re-coded lexical latencies for all conditions approximated a normal distribution (no skew values exceeded +/- 1.5), therefore, natural time data were used in the analysis. Table 4.1.6 presents the means and standard deviations of the lexical decision times for target words (answers of the unsolved RATs) and the neutral words in the LDT-before trials by Condition. 87 Table 4.1.618 Means and Standard Deviations of the Lexical Decision Times for Target Words and Neutral Words Lexical Decision Time Target Word Neutral Word M (SD) M (SD) NI 772.25 (261.06) 751.68 (200.18) RE 688.28 (160.13) 762.23 (218.56) IN 754.66 (208.13) 772.47 (188.50) Condition A2 (Word Type) x 2 (Question Set) x 3 (Condition) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Word Type (Neutral vs. Target), using lexical decision time as the dependent variable, was carried out to compare participants’ lexical decision latency to target words and neutral words among the three conditions. The summary of the results is presented in Table 4.1.7. 88 Table 4.1.719 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Condition and Word Type on Lexical Decision Time Source df MS F p ηp2 .19 .82 .01 Between-Subjects Condition 2 14476.29 Error ( between) 45 74600.27 Within-Subjects Word Type 1 13411.67 .98 .33 .02 Word Type x Condition 2 18326.10 1.34 .27 .06 Error (within) 45 23570.06 Neither the main effects nor the interaction effects was significant, Word Type: F(1,45) = .98, p = .33, p2 = .02; Condition: F(2, 45) = .19, p = .82,p2 = .01; Word Type x Condition: F(2, 45) = 1.34, p = .27,p2 = .06. This indicates that, in all the conditions, participants did not respond significantly faster to the target words than they did to the neutral words. Participants in the IN condition were not more sensitized to the target words, and this is not in line with the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. Discussion Experiment I failed to demonstrate any incubation effects, and cannot offer any supporting evidence to the Opportunistic-Assimilation and the Spreading-Activation hypotheses. However, the absence of an incubation effect at both behavioral (performance) and cognitive (knowledge activation) levels may be related to the 89 difficulty of the RATs used in this experiment. The RATs used in this experiment may have been too difficult for the participants to benefit from an incubation period, reflected in the low number of correct responses (see Table 4.1.1). An incubation period may not be very helpful if the problems are so difficult that the individuals do not have the required knowledge to solve them. Also, consistent failures in solving RATs at the first attempt may lower participants’ motivation, and in turn, affect performance in the second attempt. Participants may even consider some of the RATs as unsolvable after repeated failures in the first attempts, and this may suppress any further problem-solving processes. Therefore, to have a more appropriate evaluation of the role of an incubation period, Experiment II was conducted to replicate Experiment I but using easier RATs. In addition to this, Experiment II also further examined the meta-analysis findings concerning the moderating effect of incubation-task loading on the incubation effect. 90 EXPERIMENT II The paradigm of Experiment II was similar to that of Experiment I, except for five modifications. First, there was an adjustment to the difficulty level of the RATs. In Experiment II, RATs were again selected from a pool created by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003), but easier RATs were chosen this time. Two sets of RATs were prepared, one consisting of intermediate RATs and the other set consisting of easy RATs. Second, there was no Rest condition, but instead three Incubation conditions with incubation periods filled by high, intermediate or low cognitive load tasks. The No-Incubation condition remained and served as a control condition. The rationale for including these three incubation conditions is to examine the impact of the loading of incubation tasks on incubation effects. The third modification was to the lexical decision tasks. Although word length and frequency were controlled in Experiment I, it is still possible that there were some uncontrolled differences between the target words and the neutral words. To equate this ancillary lexical factor, the same items served as both target words and neutral words across participants in this experiment. The target words (answers for the easy RATs) presented to participants solving easy RATs were used as neutral words for those solving intermediate RATs, and vice versa. The fourth modification was the counterbalancing of the presentation order of the LDT-before and LDT-after trials with each participant. In Experiment II, participants were randomly assigned to solve the LDT before the second attempt to the RAT in the 1st, 2nd, 5th, and 8th trials or in the 3rd, 4th, 6th, and 7th trials. This counterbalancing ensured that any measured effect was neither order-specific nor item-specific. The last modification was the control of the number of incubation tasks presented during the incubation period. In Experiment I, the number of MRTs and ATs presented during the 91 incubation period varied among participants, and this may affect the incubation effect size on each participant. To control for this potential confounding factor, in Experiment II, it was fixed that 5 MRTs (8 second each) and 8 ATs (10 second each) were presented during the incubation period. Method Participants 173 (Female: 124, Male: 49) students from Lancaster University participated in this experiment for course credits or payment, and the mean age was 20.33 (SD = 2.91). All were native English speakers. Materials Two sets of RATs (Easy and Intermediate) were prepared. As in Experiment I, the RATs were selected from a pool created by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003). The mean reported solution rate of the RATs in the Intermediate set was 36.16% (SD = 8.57) with a range of 25%-45%. The mean solution rate of the RATs in the easy set was 73.33% (SD = 5.65) with a range of 60%-80%. Appendix G presents the RATs in the Easy and the Intermediate sets. Three sets of MRTs and ATs (Low-, Intermediate-, and High-load) were prepared and used as the incubation tasks. The MRTs used in Experiment I were divided into two sets based on participants’ response time and accuracy. MRTs with below-average accuracy rate and shorter-than-average response time were included in the intermediate-load set. MRTs with above-average accuracy rate and longer-than-average response time were included in the low-load sets. The mean 92 response time and accuracy rate of the selected MRTs was 5.70 (SD = .90), and 52.87% (SD = 9.59) in the intermediate-load set, and 3.17 (SD = .55) and 77.52 % (SD = 10.33) in low-load set. The differences in response time and accuracy rate between these two sets were significant, response time: F(1, 53)= 158.50, p < .001; accuracy rate: F(1, 53) = 83.94, p < .001. The MRTs included in the high-load set were created by adding one right-angled 4-cube arm to each MRT stimulus in the intermediate set. This means that the MRT stimuli in the high-load set were 3-D objects made up of 15 solid cubes with four right-angled arms, while the MRT stimuli in the intermediate and low set were objects made up 11 cubes and with three right-angled arms. The increase in the figural complexity should raise the difficulty level of the MRTs. The ATs in the High-, Intermediate-, and Low-Load sets, were 3-digit multiplications and divisions, 3-digit summation and subtractions with regrouping, and 3-digit summation and subtraction without regrouping, respectively. Lexical Decision Tasks (LDTs) were again used to examine participants’ level of sensitivity to relevant solution concepts. Each set of LDTs consisted of five items including neutral words, pseudo-words, and the answer to the previously-presented RAT (target words). Neutral words included words unrelated to the stimuli and the answers of the presented RATs in another set. Design This experiment employed a 2 x 2 x 4 mixed factorial design, with the presentation of LDTs either before or after the second attempt to the RATs as the within-subjects factor. The between-subjects factors were the RAT difficulty (easy vs. intermediate) and the cognitive loading of the incubation tasks (no incubation, low-, 93 intermediate-, and high-load). Procedure Participants were randomly assigned to solve either the easy or intermediate RATs in one of four incubation conditions. The procedure and the presentation of the tasks in Experiment II were identical to those used in Experiment I, except that when solving MRTs and ATs, participants could response only after the presentation time was up ( MRT: 8 seconds, AT: 10 seconds). Results Similar to Experiment I, the analyses consisted of three main parts. The first part examined the post-incubation performance improvement, the second part investigated the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, and the last part focused on the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. Before conducting the main analysis, participants’ RAT performance was examined to check that, first, the RATs in Experiment II were indeed easier than those in Experiment I, and second, the easy RATs were easier than the intermediate RATs. Participants’ RAT performance in the first attempt, in terms of accuracy and response latency, in Experiment I and II was compared. Table 4.2.1 presents the means and the standard deviations of the number of correct responses and response time at the first attempt in Experiment I and II. 94 Table 4.2.120 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt in Experiment I and II RAT Performance Correct Response Response Time M (SD) M (SD) Experiment I 0.96 (1.25) 28.04 (2.42) Experiment II 3.56 (1.99) 22.19 (7.80) The mean number of correct responses in Experiment II was significantly higher than the mean in Experiment I, t(187) = 8.52, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.25. Participant in Experiment II also took shorter time to solve the RATs, t(187) = 7.19, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.05. These results converge to support the view that the RATs in Experiment II were easier than those in Experiment I. Table 4.2.2 presents the means and the standard deviations of the response time and accuracy, for easy and intermediate RATs, in each of the incubation conditions in Experiment II. 95 Table 4.2.221 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt by RAT Difficulty and Condition Number of Correct Responses Response Time Easy RAT Intermediate RAT Easy RAT Intermediate RAT M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) No Incubation 4.41 (1.97) 2.78 (1.31) 18.59 (6.53) 23.19 (3.61) Low Load 4.56 (1.75) 1.73 (1.80) 17.99 (4.58) 27.29 (2.50) Intermediate Load 5.11 (1.57) 2.11 (1.24) 16.53 (5.16) 26.20 (3.08) High Load 5.00 (1.56) 2.61 (1.69) 19.19 (5.51) 25.15 (4.14) Condition 96 To determine if the easy RATs were easier than the intermediate RATs, two 2 (RAT Difficulty) x 4 (Condition) ANOVAs were carried out using number of correct responses and response time as the dependent variables respectively. Correct Response: The easy RATs were solved more often than the intermediate RATs, indicated by the significant RAT Difficulty effect, F(1, 133) = 87.25, p < .001, p2 = .40. Non-significant effects of Condition, F(3, 134) = 1.05, p = .37, p2 = .02, and Condition x RAT Difficulty, F(3, 133) = 1.34, p = .26, p2 = .03, suggest that in both type of RATs, the RAT performance was comparable among the four experimental conditions. Response Time: Consistent with the ANOVA results for correct responses, the main effect of RAT Difficulty was significant, F(1, 133) = 90.39, p < .001, p2 = .41, while the effect of Condition and the interaction between Condition and RAT Difficulty were not significant, Condition: F(3, 133) = .98, p = .40, p2 = .02; Condition x RAT Difficulty: F(3,133) = 2.58, p = .06, p2 = .06. These ANOVA results support the assumption that the intermediate RATs were more difficult than the easy RATs. The following sections present the results of the main analyses conducted to investigate incubation effects on problem solving performance and sensitivity to relevant concepts, as well as the moderating effect of the cognitive load of incubation tasks. Incubation Effects on Problem-Solving Performance Performance Improvement: To determine whether an incubation period can enhance problem solving performance, the improvement scores in the LDT-after trials among the four conditions were compared. Twenty participants were excluded in the 97 analysis because they solved all the RATs presented in the LDT-after trials in the first attempt. Similar to Experiment I, the improvement score was first computed for each participant. Table 4.2.3 presents the means and the standard deviations of the number of correct responses in the first and the second attempt, and the improvement scores by Condition and RAT Difficulty. Table 4.2.322 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Two Attempts by Condition and RAT Difficulty First Attempt Second Attempt Improvement Score M (SD) M (SD) Condition Overall M (SD) Easy RAT 2.22 (0.81) 2.58 (0.80) 0.19 (0.32) No Incubation 2.06 (1.06) 2.37 (1.02) 0.13 (0.28) Low Load 2.21 (0.92) 2.74 (0.87) 0.29 (0.24) Intermediate Load 2.44 (0.62) 2.78 (0.73) 0.24 (0.39) High Load 2.17 (0.65) 2.43 (0.59) 0.12 (0.20) Intermediate RAT Overall 1.18 (0.93) 1.34 (1.05) 0.07 (0.21) No Incubation 1.38 (0.86) 1.53 (0.93) 0.08 (0.23) Low Load 0.65 (0.61) 0.82 (0.81) 0.05 (0.17) Intermediate Load 1.05 (0.83) 1.20 (1.01) 0.07 (0.17) High Load 1.58 (1.12) 1.74 (1.24) 1.00 (0.26) 98 The overall improvement scores on solving easy and intermediate RATs were both significantly larger than zero, Easy RAT: M = .19, SD = .31, t(75) = 5.25, p = < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.21; Intermediate RAT: M = .07, SD = .21, t(76) = 3.13, p = .002, Cohen’s d = .72. A 2 (RAT Difficulty) x 4 (Condition) ANOVA, using improvement score as the dependent variable, was carried out to check if the degree of improvement was a function of incubation condition. The summary of the ANOVA results is presented in Table 4.2.4. Table 4.2.423 The ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of RAT Difficulty and Condition on Improvement Scores Source df MS F p ηp2 Between-Subjects RAT Difficulty 1 .54 7.62 .01 .05 Condition 3 .042 0.59 .62 .01 RAT x Condition 3 .094 1.32 .27 .027 Error ( between) 145 .071 The Condition effect was not significant, F(3, 133) = .59, p = .62, p2 = .01. This indicates that the degree of improvement was comparable among the four conditions. This non-significant difference was found on both easy and intermediate RATs, as the RAT x Condition effect was also not statistically significant, F(3, 133) = 1.32, p= .27, p2 = .027. This pattern of the results does not support the positive effect of incubation period on problem solving performance. The RAT Difficulty effect was significant, F(1, 133)= 7.62, p = .01, p2 = .05, revealing the improvement scores of 99 participants solving easy RATs were significantly higher than those solving intermediate RATs. This may be due to the fact that easy RATs were easier to solve, and therefore, participants solving easy RATs were more likely to have improvement during the second attempt. Opportunistic Assimilation: To examine if an incubation period can facilitate cue-assimilation and in turn enhance the subsequent problem solving process, participants’ RAT improvement scores in the presence (LDT-before trials) and absence of the cue (LDT-after trials) were examined. Thirty-one participants were dropped because they solved all the RATs presented in the first attempt. Table 4.2.5 presents the means and the standard deviations of the improvement scores in the Cue and No-Cue trials, and the differences between them, in by Condition and RAT Difficulty. 100 Table 4.2.524 Means and Standard Deviations of Improvement Scores in the Presence and Absence of Cue, and the Score Difference by Condition and RAT Difficulty. Cue No-Cue Cueing effect (Improvement Score Cue – No-Cue) M (SD) M (SD) Condition M (SD) Easy RAT No Incubation .44 (.40) .14 (.28) .31 (.42)* Low Load .19 (.39) .24 (.34) -.05 (.58) Intermediate Load .13 (.27) .25 (.40) -.12 (.49) High Load .38 (.41) .14 (.22) .23 (.40)* Intermediate RAT No Incubation .30 (.34) .08 (.23) .23 (.29)* Low Load .09 (.17) .05 (.17) .03 (.26) Intermediate Load .30 (.28) .20 (.17) .23 (.36)* High Load .12 (.22) .10 (.26) .02 (.24) *Significantly difference from zero, p < .05 In the No-Incubation condition, a positive cueing effect was found when solving easy RATs and intermediate RATs (Easy RAT: .31, Intermediate RAT: .23). However, in the Low-Load condition, the presence of cue did not facilitate the problem solving performance (Easy RAT: -.05, Intermediate RAT: .03). In the Intermediate-Load condition, only participants solving Intermediate RATs benefited from the presence of 101 cue (Easy RAT: -.12, Intermediate RAT: .23). Interestingly, a reverse pattern was observed in the High-Load condition that the presence of cue could enhance the performance only when solving easy RATs, but not intermediate RATs (Easy RAT: .23, Intermediate RAT: .02). Apparently, the presentation of cues could have a negative, null, or positive effect on RAT problem-solving processes, depending on the cognitive loading of the incubation tasks and the difficulty level of the RATs. To examine how these two procedural factors moderate the magnitude of the cueing effect, a 2 (RAT Difficulty) x 2 (Cue) x 4 (Condition) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Cue (Cue vs. No-Cue), using improvement score as the dependent variable, was carried out. The ANOVA results are presented in Table 4.2.6. 102 Table 4.2.625 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of RAT Difficulty, Condition, and Cue on Number of Correct Responses Source df MS F p ηp2 Between-Subjects RAT Difficulty (R) 1 .766 8.17* .01 .057 Condition (C) 1 .105 1.12 .34 .025 RxC 3 .076 0.82 .49 .018 134 .088 Error (between) Within-Subjects Cue (Cu) 1 .860 11.16* .001 .077 Cu x R 1 .023 0.30 .58 .002 Cu x C 3 .242 3.14* .03 .066 Cu x R x C 3 .260 3.37* .02 .070 134 .077 Error (within) *p < .05. There was a significant Cue effect, F(1, 134) = 11.16, p = .001, ηp2 = .077, indicating that performing LDTs containing solution-relevant words improved performance on the RATs more than a second attempt at the same RATs. There was also a significant interaction between Cue and Condition, F(3, 134) = 3.14, p = .03, ηp2 = .066. Subsequent pairwise comparisons within each condition revealed that, in the No-Incubation condition, the mean improvement score in the Cue trials was significantly larger than the mean score in the No-Cue trials, t(35) = 4.47, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 6.78. However, a Cue effect was absent in the other three conditions; 103 Low-Load: t(34) = -.13, p = .90, Cohen’s d = .01; Intermediate-Load: t(36) = .96, p = .34, Cohen’s d = .31, High-Load: t(33) = 2.02, p = .052, Cohen’s d = 1.41. The null findings may be due to the fact that the presence of a cue had an opposite impact in solving easy and intermediate RATs in the three incubation conditions, and they may cancel out each other when looking at the overall cueing effect in solving RATs. This explanation is supported by the significant Cue x RAT x Condition effect, F(3, 111) = 3.12, p = .03, ηp2 = .08. To investigate this interaction effect further, data were split into four groups, in terms of the experimental conditions. In each group, a 2 (RAT Difficulty) x 2 (Cue) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Cue, using improvement score as the dependent variable, was carried out. In the No-Incubation condition, there was a significant effect of Cue, F(1, 34) = 20.13, p < .001,ηp2 = .372, but the Cue x RAT Difficulty effect was not significant, F(1, 34) = .03, p = .51,ηp2 = .013. This implies that participants solving the easy and the intermediate RATs benefited from the cue to the same degree. In the Low-Load Condition, neither the main effect of Cue nor the interaction effect between Cue and RAT Difficulty was significant, Cue: F(1, 33) = .01, p = .92, ηp2 < .001; Cue x RAT Difficulty: F(1, 33) = .31, p = .58, ,ηp2 < .001, suggesting that the exposure to relevant cue had no impact on the subsequent problem-solving processes. In the Intermediate-Load Condition, the Cue effect was not significant, F(1, 33) = .68, p = .42, ηp2 < .001. A significant Cue x RAT Difficulty effect was found in the Intermediate-Load Condition, F(1, 33) = 6.22, p = .02, ηp2 < .151, implying that participants solving intermediate and easy RATs benefit from the presence of cue in different degrees. The cueing effect in intermediate RATs was significantly larger than 104 zero, M = .23, SD = .36, t(19) = 2.87, p = .01, Cohen’s d = 1.32, however, the cueing effect in solving easy RAT was not significantly different from zero, M = -.12, SD = .49, t(16) = -.99, p = .34, Cohen’s d = .50. This suggests that the only participants solving the intermediate RATs could make use of the cues presented to them to solve the RATs. In the High-Load Condition, a significant Cue effect, F(1, 32) = 5.32, p = .03, ηp2 = .142 and a marginally significant Cue x RAT Difficulty effect were found, F(1, 32) = 3.63, p = .07, ηp2 = .102. Different from the findings in the Intermediate-Load Condition, the interaction effect was in the opposite direction, indicating that the presence of cue is helpful only when solving easy RATs M = .23, SD = .40, t(14) = 2.24, p = .04, Cohen’s d = 1.32, but not intermediate RATs, M = .02, SD = .24, t(18) = .41, p = .69, Cohen’s d = .19. This pattern of interaction suggests that an incubation period can help us utilize the cue, but the occurrence of the cue-assimilation process is moderated by the loading of the incubation tasks and the problem difficulty. One of the follow-up questions is about the mechanism underlying the cue-assimilation process. It has been hypothesized that spreading-activation occurs during the incubation period and this would sensitize individuals to cues encountered subsequently by chance (Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). If the occurrence of spreading activation during an incubation period is the basis for cue-assimilation, then a significant interaction between RAT Difficulty and Condition on the lexical decision latency to target words is expected. Incubation Effects on Knowledge Activation Spreading Activation: To determine whether spreading activation occurred during the incubation period, participants’ lexical decision times were compared for 105 the target words and the neutral words (answers for the RATs in the other sets) of the RATs in the LDT-before trials among the four conditions were compared. Similar to Experiment I, incorrect lexical decisions and correct decisions with extreme lexical decision time (<50 ms or >2500ms) were discarded (1.6% of the data). Within each participant, any lexical decision time longer than 2 SDs from the mean was recoded to the value found at 2 SD. In this experiment, the skew value of the data in some conditions exceed 1.5, therefore, the lexical decision times were logarithmically transformed to diminish skew. Table 4.2.7 presents the means and standard deviations of the natural and log-transformed lexical decision times for neutral words and target words by Condition and RAT Difficulty. 106 Table 4.2.726 Means and Standard Deviations of the Natural and Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Times for Target Words and Neutral Words. Easy RAT Target Word Neutral Word Target Word Neutral Word M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Condition No Incubation Low Load Intermediate Load High Load Intermediate RAT Original Lexical Decision Time 691.26 711.28 630.90 687.31 (183.30) (137.25) (129.10) (129.46) 670.86 663.16 676.69 712.83 (292.23) (127.20) (164.21) (111.06) 697.23 713.71 714.50 745.80 (160.86) (139.80) (165.02) (119.40) 645.55 724.10 637.70 730.33 (114.38) (133.25) (129.10) (116.47) Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time No Incubation 2.82 (.11) 2.84 (.08) 2.79 (.08) 2.83 (.08) Low Load 2.80 (.13) 2.82 (.08) 2.82 (.10) 2.85 (.07) Intermediate Load 2.83 (.10) 2.84 (.08) 2.84 (.10) 2.87 (.07) High Load 2.80 (.08) 2.85 (.08) 2.79 (.10) 2.86 (.07) There was no significant difference in the log-transformed lexical decision times to the answers of the easy and the intermediate RATs when they were presented as neutral items in the LDTs, t(172) = .12, p = .73, Cohen’s d= .02. This ensures that 107 there is no priori difference in the baseline lexical decision time for target words and neutral words. To investigate if spreading-activation occurred during the incubation periods to further sensitize participants to relevant memory items, the log-transformed data were analyzed using a 2 (Word Type) x 2 (RAT Difficulty) x 4 (Condition) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Word Type (Target Word vs. Neutral Word). Table 4.2.8 presents the summary of the ANOVA results. Table 4.2.827 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of the RAT Difficulty, Condition, and Word Type on Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time df MS F p ηp2 RAT Difficulty (R) 1 < .001 .001 .40 .006 Condition (C) 3 .011 .81 .11 .047 RxC 3 .008 .58 .98 .002 123 .013 Word Type (W) 1 .075 26.79 < .001 .180 WxR 1 .002 0.72 .40 .010 WxC 3 .006 2.03 .11 .050 WxRxC 3 < .001 0.06 .98 < .010 123 .003 Source Between-Subjects Error ( between) Within-Subjects Error (within) In general, participants made quicker lexical decisions to the target words than the neutral words, indicated by the significant Word Type effect, F(1, 123) = 26.79, p 108 < .001, ηp2 = .18. None of the interaction effects between Cue and other IVs. approached significance, revealing that the lexical decision time difference between the target words and the neutral words were comparable in all the conditions and in both types of RATs. This pattern of results suggests that participants were, in general, sensitized to the answer of the unsolved RATs after the initial approach to the RATs. However, performing incubation tasks did not further sensitize participants to the answers of the unsolved RATs. This is inconsistent with the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. The absence of a significant Word Type x Condition x RAT Difficulty effect also implies that the cue-assimilation process could not be attributed to the occurrence of spread of activation within the semantic network during the incubation period. Discussion Experiment II, using easier RATs as insight problems, had more fruitful results than Experiment I. Experiment II reveals that although the incubation period did not directly enhance performance on RATs, it facilitated participants to incorporate the external cue into problem solving and in turn improve performance. Furthermore, Experiment II reaffirms the meta-analysis by showing that the loading of the incubation tasks had an impact on the post-incubation RAT performance. Experiment II also identifies a new moderator on incubation effects: Problem Difficulty, and reveals a significant interaction between loading and problem difficulty moderators on the cue-assimilation process. However, Experiment II still fails to offer any evidence in support of the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. In the No-Incubation condition, participants solving both easy and intermediate RATs benefited from the presentation of cues (target words given as LDTs), and the 109 magnitude of the cueing effects was comparable to the effects found in the incubation conditions. This is different from the prediction that participants in the No-Incubation condition should be less likely to assimilate the external cue because the problem representation was not restructured into a better form yet. In the No-Incubation condition, cues were presented while participants were actively solving the RAT, and so it was very likely for the participants to aware of the association between the LDTs and the RATs, and incorporate the cue into problem solving immediately once it was resumed. It means that, even if participants were not having an appropriate initial representation of the RAT, they could still utilize the cue embedded in LDTs and solve the RAT. However, this conscious-awareness explanation cannot account for the finding in the incubation conditions that a positive cueing effect arose only when solving intermediate RATs in the Intermediate-Load Condition and when solving easy RATs in the High-Load Condition. The Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, suggesting that incubation period facilitates problem restructuring and in turn increases the likelihood of cue-assimilation, seems to offer a better explanation for this pattern of findings. In the Intermediate-Load condition, performing the incubation tasks occupies part of the participants’ attentional resource, and this would then create a diffused attentional state. This attentional state can facilitate the problem restructuring process, and in turn increase the likelihood of assimilation of the external cue subsequently encountered by chance. This restructuring process would be essential especially when solving intermediate RATs because participants’ initial problem representations may be faulty. When solving easy RATs, the initial problem representation is likely to be already in an appropriate form. Therefore, it may not require any restructuring, and 110 performing incubation tasks and a shift from a focused to a diffuse attentional state would not be helpful. Indeed, the diffused attentional state may even cause the occurrence of some unneeded unconscious processes, which may induce unnecessary changes on the problem representation, and this may then inhibit the cue-assimilation process. This unconscious cue-assimilation explanation can also account for the opposite findings in the High-Load condition and the null findings in the Low-Load condition. In the High-Load condition, the incubation tasks were very cognitively demanding, so that participants had to focus fully on them. This focused attentional state would not foster the occurrence of any unconscious restructuring process. Therefore, participants in the intermediate RAT group did not benefit from the presence of cues. For participants solving easy RATs, a focused attentional state could inhibit the occurrence of any redundant unconscious cognitive processes. The problem representation, which was already at an appropriate form, would then remain unchanged. Hence, participants could still able to incorporate the cues into problem solving after the incubation period, and benefit from the presentation of the cues. The null cueing effect in solving both types of RATs in the Low-Load condition may be due to the fact the low-load incubation tasks were too easy. In Experiment II, participants had 8 seconds to solve each MRT. According to the data from Experiment I, participants took an average of about 3 seconds to solve an easy MRT. In the remaining 5 seconds, participants may shift attention to think about other things, or to think about the previously-presented but unsolved RATs. According to the unconscious cue-assimilation explanation, shifting attention to other tasks should increase the likelihood of cue-assimilation in solving intermediate RATs, but it should not be helpful at all when solving easy RATs. Conversely, resuming work on the 111 unsolved RAT would facilitate the cue-assimilation process only when solving easy RATs. Thus, it implies that whether the presence of cue has an effect on the solution rate depended on what participants have chosen to do during the remaining time of the incubation period. There should be large individual differences because the experiment instructions did not give any specific guidelines as to whether participants should think about the unsolved problem or not if they finished the MRTs and ATs early. Thus, due to uncontrolled individual differences, it is not unexpected that the overall cueing effects in the Low-Load condition did not approach significance. Some may question the reliability of the findings regarding the effect of cues because the meta-analysis reported that the presence of a cue was not a significant moderator of the incubation effect in past incubation studies. There are several explanations for this inconsistency. First, Experiment II reveals that the cueing effect is moderated by the loading of the incubation tasks and the difficulty of the insight problems. However, due to the small number of the available studies, the meta-analysis only examined the overall effect of cue, and did not examine the interaction between the presence of cue, loading of the incubation tasks, and problem difficulty, on the incubation effect size. Second, there are numbers of ways to present the cues, and the presentation of the cue in some of the past studies may not be effective, which would certainly lessen the overall cueing effect. In Experiment II, when solving intermediate RATs, the magnitude of the cueing effect in the No-Incubation condition was comparable to the effect found in the Intermediate-Load incubation condition. Some may then challenge the importance of having an incubation period. However, in real-life problem solving situations, the opportunity for encountering additional external cues would be low if there is no change in the external environment. Shifting to perform other irrelevant incubation 112 tasks will cause a change in the environment, and this will increase the chance of encountering external cues. The findings of Experiment II suggest that performing intermediate-load incubation tasks generates the optimal attentional state for assimilating any chance-encountered cues. The results of Experiments II offer empirical evidence for the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, in that incubation periods facilitate the occurrence of unconscious processes (e.g., problem restructuring) which would increase the likelihood of assimilating cues subsequently encountered by chance. The findings of Experiment II enrich this hypothesis by identifying two procedural moderators of this cue-dependent incubation effect: loading of the incubation task and problem difficulty. One further question concerns the nature of the unconscious processes occurring during the incubation period. It has been suggested that a cue-assimilation effect can be attributed to the occurrence of spreading activation during the incubation period (Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). However, the findings reported here regarding LDT performance challenge this view. Experiment II reveals that participants were more sensitive to the answer even if they could not solve it at the initial attempt. This is strong evidence for the occurrence of spreading activation during the initial attempt. However, the incubation period did not further sensitize participants to these relevant concepts, reflected in the non-significant difference in LDT performance between the four conditions. This does not support the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. The non-significant difference also implies that the cue-assimilation process cannot be attributed to the occurrence of spreading activation during an incubation period. On the whole, the findings of Experiment II support the unconscious role of incubation, and offer empirical evidence for the Opportunistic-Assimilation 113 hypothesis, but offer no supporting evidence for the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. The cue-dependent incubation effect is moderated by two factors: loading of incubation tasks and problem difficulty. Although spreading activation did not occur during the incubation period in solving the RATs in Experiment II, it is still too early to jump to the conclusion that spreading activation is not a mechanism underlying an incubation period. There is a possibility that spreading activation occurs only if an individual’s mind is fixated on some misleading concepts, and the role of spreading activation is to re-distribute the activation (Sio & Rudowicz, 2008). To verify this possibility, Experiment II was replicated but using misleading insight problems. The use of misleading insight problems also allows the examination of the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis by comparing the lexical decision latency to misleading concepts in incubation and no-incubation conditions. If selective-forgetting occurs during an incubation period to desensitize individuals to misleading items, then an increase in LDT latency to misleading items in the Incubation condition is expected. Another reason for replicating the experiment on other insight problems is to examine the generalizability of the findings of Experiment II. It is unclear whether the RAT can be considered as an insight problem or a linguistic completion task, suggesting it may not be representative of all insight problem-solving tasks. Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003a) found that participants claimed that they solved RATs sometimes with insight and sometimes without insight. This suggests the need to replicate Experiment II with another type of linguistic insight problem. Experiment III was carried out to address the aforementioned concerns. Experiment III examined the incubation effect on another type of linguistic insight problem: Rebus. A Rebus combines verbal and visual clues to a common phrase (e.g., 114 “you just me” = “just between you and me”; “peazzzce” = rest in peace). A correct interpretation of the visual and verbal cues is essential in order to solve a rebus. The validity of rebuses as insight problems was supported by a significant positive correlation between individuals’ performance in solving rebus and self-rated insight, and RAT performance (MacGregor & Cunningham, 2008). In Experiment III, both neutral and misleading Rebuses were used. Experiment III also improved the methodology of Experiment II by matching the amount of work assigned to participants among different experimental conditions. In Experiment II, participants in the No-Incubation condition were not required to perform the MRTs and ATs. However, in Experiment III, participants in the No-Incubation condition were also asked to perform the MRTs and ATs at the end of each trial. 115 EXPERIMENT III Experiment III had two objectives. The first one was to replicate the cue-dependent incubation effect found in Experiment II on another insight problem: Rebus. The second objective was to investigate if spreading activation and selective forgetting occur during an incubation period when solving insight problems embedded with misleading hints. The findings of Experiment II suggest that performing intermediate-load incubation tasks can generate the largest incubation effect in solving intermediate difficulty problems. Therefore, in Experiment III, only Rebuses at an intermediate level of difficulty were used because Experiment II reported a null incubation effect when solving problems at an easy level. Also, there was only one incubation condition: the Immediate-Load condition. The No-Incubation condition remained and served as the control condition. Neutral and misleading Rebuses were used in Experiment III to examine the Spreading-Activation and Selection-Forgetting hypotheses. If spreading activation impacts only when individuals are fixated on misleading concepts, then an increased sensitivity to relevant memory items after an incubation period is expected only for participants solving misleading Rebuses. If selective forgetting is one of the mechanisms underlying incubation, then a decrease in sensitivity to misleading concepts is expected after the incubation period, which will in turn resolve fixation on the misleading items. Also, it was expected that a significant cueing effect in solving Rebus would be revealed in the Intermediate-Load condition. This prediction arises because, according to the findings of Experiment II, performing incubation tasks can facilitate 116 unconscious problem restructuring, and in turn, increase the likeliness of assimilating external cues. Method Participants 69 (51 females, 18 males) students from Lancaster University participated for course credits or payment. The mean age was 20.99 years with a standard deviation of 4.82. All participants were native English speakers. Materials Two sets of Rebus, neutral and misleading, were prepared and used as the problem-solving tasks in this study. Eight Rebuses selected from the pool developed by MacGregor and Cunningham (2008), with a solution rate range from 25-50%, were included in the neutral set. Each Rebus was presented inside a rectangular border. These 8 Rebuses were also used as the stimuli in the misleading set. In the misleading set, a misleading hint was presented below the rectangular border of each of these eight Rebuses. The misleading hint is a word that can form an association with one of the clues of the presented Rebus but is not part of the answer. It was expected that the misleading hints would direct the participants to interpret the clue in an inappropriate way and search in the wrong direction. To avoid participants realizing that the presented hints were always irrelevant, two filler Rebuses, with a relevant hint(a word which is part of the answer) presented below the rectangular border, were added to the misleading set. The relevant hints in the filler Rebus should also lure the participants to attempt to use the misleading hints to solve the misleading Rebuses. To match the 117 number of stimuli in the neutral and misleading sets, these two filler Rebuses were also included in the neutral set, but the hints below the rectangular box were removed. In total, there were 10 Rebuses in each set, but the filler Rebuses were not included in the analyses. The Rebuses presented in the neutral and misleading sets are presented in Appendix H. Lexical Decision Tasks (LDTs) were again adapted to examine participants’ level of sensitivity to the relevant solution concepts and the misleading hints. A set of LDTs consisted of five items including neutral words, pseudo-words, and target words (a word which is part of the answer). If the previously-presented Rebus was a misleading one, the misleading hint was also included in the LDTs. All presented words were high frequency words (an occurrence of 90 or more in a million). To ensure that there was no baseline lexical decision time difference between the neutral, target, and misleading words, 15 participants were recruited from the Lancaster University to perform a set of lexical decision tasks including the presentation of these three types of words and the pseudo-words used in Experiment III. The presentation of the lexical decision tasks was the same as the one in Experiment II. The log-transformed LDT data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA with repeated-measures on Word Type: neutral, relevant, and misleading hint. The main effect of Word Type was not significant, F(2, 28) = .24, p = .82, ηp2 = .031, implying that there is no prior difference in the lexical decision latencies for these three types of words. Hence, any difference in lexical decision latencies in the Experiment III cannot be attributed to this factor. Mental Rotation Tasks (MRTs) and Arithmetic Tasks (ATs) presented in Experiment III were the intermediate-load MRTs and ATs in Experiment II. 118 Design This experiment employed a 2 x 2 x 2 mixed factorial design. The within-subjects factor was whether the LDTs were presented before or after the second attempt at the Rebus. The two between-subjects factors were the Incubation conditions: No Incubation (NI) and Incubation (IN), and the types of the Rebuses: Neutral and Misleading. Similar to Experiment II, the presentation order of the LDT-before and LDT-after trials was counterbalanced across participants. Participants were randomly assigned to receive the LDT-before trials either in the 1st, 2nd, 4th, 7th, and 9th trials or in the 3rd,5th, 6th, 8th, and 10th trials. The two filler Rebuses were always presented at the first and fifth trials. The main experiment consisted of 10 trials. In every trial, regardless of condition, participants were asked to solve 1 Rebus (either neutral or misleading, depending on the type of Rebus they were assigned to solve), 5 LDTs, 5 MRTs, and 8 ATs. The presentation order of these four types of tasks varied among participants, depending on which incubation condition they were assigned. Each trial in the non-incubation condition began with the presentation of a Rebus, followed by the presentation of a set of LDTs and then the re-presentation of the same Rebus (or vice versa, depending on whether it was a LDT-before or LDT-after trial). After that, 5 MRTs and 8 ATs were presented. In the incubation condition, after the first presentation of the Rebus, 5 MRTs and 8 ATs were presented; then the LDTs were presented followed by the presentation of the same Rebus (or vice versa, depending on whether it was a LDT-before or LDT-after trial). 119 Procedure Participants were randomly assigned to solve the neutral or the misleading Rebus set, in either the No-Incubation or Incubation condition. All the task instructions and stimuli were presented on a computer using bespoke software. Participants were first informed that the main experiment involved solving Rebuses, MRTs, ATs, and LDTs. They were then given instructions on how to complete the tasks and were reminded to finish the tasks as quickly and accurately as possible. After this, they were prompted to solve one Rebus, one MRT, one AT, and two LDTs for practice. For participants assigned to solve the misleading set, the practice Rebus presented to them was a Rebus in which a relevant hint appearing below the rectangular frame. Prior to the main experiment, participants were reminded that the hints presented below the rectangular frame could be relevant or misleading. Regardless of condition, at the beginning of each experimental trial, a Rebus was presented for 30 seconds. After the time was up or the participants answered the Rebus, participants in the No-Incubation condition were then prompted to do an LDT set followed by a second attempt to the same Rebus (or vice versa depending on whether it was a LDT-before or LDT-after trial). After that, participants were told to solve MRTs and ATs for two minutes. Participants in the Incubation condition were first prompted to solve the MRTs and ATs, before performing the LDTs and re-approaching the problems. If participants solved the Rebus at the first attempt, they were instructed to give the same answer again in the second attempt to the same Rebus. Each Rebus was presented in the middle of the screen. Participants were told that they could respond at any time within the presentation time (30 seconds) by first 120 pressing the C key to pause the timer, and then reporting their answer verbally to the experimenter for immediate judgment. If their answer was correct, then they were prompted to do the next task; otherwise, they had to continue to solve the Rebus until the time was up or until a solution was reached. The number of correctly solved problems and the response latencies were recorded. The response latency was measured as the duration from the onset of problem presented until the participant got the right answer or until the presentation period ended. The Rebuses in the misleading set were presented in the same way except that the hint for each Rebus was presented for 1 second prior to the presentation of the Rebus, and the hint also appeared below the rectangular frame during the presentation of the Rebus. The presentation of the LDTs, MRTs and ATs were the same as in Experiment II. Results Before running the main analyses to verify the predictions, response time and accuracy for the neutral and misleading Rebuses at the first attempt were compared to verify that the misleading hints did indeed mislead the participants and impair their performance. Table 4.3.1 presents the means and the standard deviations of number of correct responses and response time to the neutral and misleading Rebuses at the first attempt in each incubation condition. 121 Table 4.3.128 Means and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses and Response Time at the First Attempt by Rebus Type and Condition Number of Correct Responses Response Time Neutral Misleading Neutral Misleading Rebus Rebus Rebus Rebus M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) No Incubation 3.82 (1.23) 2.82 (2.10) 20.24 (4.15) 23.00 (5.87) Incubation 4.67 (1.33) 2.44 (1.46) 16.91 (3.85) 25.19 (4.05) Condition Note. The two filler rebuses were excluded, and only 8 rebuses were included in the analysis. Two 2 (Rebus Type) x 2 (Condition) between-subjects ANOVAs was carried out, using number of correct responses and response time as the dependent variables. The factor Rebus Type had a significant effect on both number of correct responses, F(1, 66) = 18.56, p < .001,ηp2 = .220, and response time, F(1, 66) = 25.90, p < .001,ηp2 = .282. This supports the assumption that the misleading hints would induce a negative effect on participants’ performance with Rebus problems. The effect of Condition was not significant: number of correct responses: F(1, 66) = .28, p = .60, ηp2 = .004, response time; F(1, 66) = .39, p = .54,ηp2 = .006, suggesting that there were no performance differences between participants in these conditions. There was a significant Rebus Type x Condition interaction in response latencies, F(1, 66) = 6.49, p = .01,ηp2 = .09. Two independent t-tests were carried out as follow-up analyses to compare the response time to neutral and misleading Rebuses in each condition. Participants in the Incubation condition took significantly longer to 122 solve the misleading Rebuses than the neutral Rebuses, p < .001. Participants in the No-Incubation condition also took longer to solve the misleading RATs, but the difference did not approach significance. The null effect in the No-Incubation condition may be due to the fact that response time was a less sensitive and reliable performance indicator than number of correct responses. However, the overall results support the view that the presence of the misleading hints could reduce participants’ performance on Rebuses. Similar to the Experiment I and II, the number of correct responses at the first and the second attempt was used to examine the incubation effect on problem solving performance, and the data on lexical decision tasks were analyzed to assess if there were any changes in sensitivity to misleading and relevant items. Four separate analyses were carried out to address four questions: 1) if performing intermediate-load incubation tasks could enhance performance on Rebuses, 2) if an incubation period filled with intermediate-load tasks could facilitate the cue-assimilation process, 3) if spreading activation occurred only when solving misleading Rebuses, and 4) if selective forgetting occurred during the incubation period to suppress the activation of the misleading concepts. Incubation Effect on Problem-Solving Performance Performance Improvement: To assess the incubation effect on performance, an improvement score, which was the proportion of Rebuses not solved at the first attempt that were solved at the second attempt, was calculated for each participant. These numbers were then averaged across participants solving the same type of Rebuses in the same condition. Similar to Experiments I and II, only data on Rebuses presented in the LDT after trials were examined. The data on the filler Rebuses were 123 not included in the analysis. Table 4.3.2 presents the means and standard deviations of the number of correct responses in each attempt, and the improvement score, in the LDT-after trials, by Condition and Rebus type. Table 4.3.229 Mean and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Two Attempts by Condition and Rebus Type First Attempt Second Attempt Improvement Score M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Condition Overall Neutral Rebus 2.14 (0.71) 2.41 (.82) .14 (.28) No Incubation 2.06 (0.75) 2.35 (.86) .14 (.28) Incubation 2.24 (0.75) 2.47 (.80) .14 (.28) Misleading Rebus Overall 1.26 (1.47) 1.61 (.95) .11 (.18) No Incubation 1.43 (1.03) 1.81 (.98) .13 (.20) Incubation 1.11 (1.02) 1.44 (.92) .09 (.16) The overall improvement score was significantly larger than zero, Neutral Rebuses: t(33) = 2.86, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.00; Misleading Rebuses: t(33) = 3.55, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.24. If the incubation period can improve the performance on Rebuses, then the improvement scores of participants in the Incubation condition should be larger than the scores of those in the No-Incubation condition. A 2 (Condition) x 2 (Rebus Type) ANOVA on improvement scores was conducted to 124 verify this prediction. The summary of the results is presented in Table 4.3.3. Table 4.3.330 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Rebus Type and Condition on Improvement Scores Source df MS F p ηp2 Between-Subjects Rebus Type 1 .014 .24 .62 .004 Condition 1 .004 .08 .78 .001 Rebus Type x Condition 1 .004 .08 .78 .001 Error ( between) 64 .056 Neither the main effects nor the interaction effects were significant, implying that participants in the Incubation condition did not demonstrate a larger performance improvement in solving neutral and misleading Rebuses than participants in the No-Incubation condition. This does not support the hypothesis that an incubation period can enhance problem-solving performance. Opportunistic Assimilation: Another objective of Experiment III was to replicate the findings of Experiment II concerning the impact of an incubation period on cue assimilation. In Experiment II, performing irrelevant intermediate-load incubation tasks did not inhibit the cue-assimilation process, but instead facilitated the unconscious cue-assimilation process. If this cue-dependent incubation effect is not item-specific, then participants in the Incubation condition in this experiment should be able to assimilate the external cues to solve the Rebuses in the second attempt. In other words, it was predicted that, in the Incubation condition, the degree of 125 performance improvement in the LDT-before trial should be significantly larger than in the LDT-before trials than in the LDT-after trials. A similar pattern of findings was expected in the No-Incubation condition, but the mechanism underlying this cueing-effect would be conscious, rather than unconscious. To verify this prediction, the difference between the improvement score in LDT-before (Cue) and LDT-after (No-Cue) trials in each condition was compared. The means and the standard deviations of the number of correct responses to Rebuses in the Cue and No-Cue Trials by Condition and Rebus Type are presented in Table 4.3.4. Table 4.3.5 presents the mean improvement score and its standard deviation in Cue and No-Cue trials by Rebus Type and Condition. Table 4.3.431 Mean and Standard Deviations of the Number of Correct Responses in the Cue and No-Cue Trials in Each Condition. First Attempt Second Attempt Cue No Cue Cue No Cue M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Condition Neutral Rebus No Incubation 1.76 (0 .97) 2.06 (0.75) 2.18 (0.95) 2.35 (0.86) Incubation 2.33 (1.14) 2.33 (0.84) 2.72 (1.13) 2.56 (0.86) Misleading Rebus No Incubation 1.24 (1.25) 1.59 (1.17) 1.94 (1.20) 1.94 (1.09) Incubation 1.33 (1.08) 1.11 (1.02) 1.67 (1.03) 1.44 (0.92) 126 Table 4.3.532 Means and Standard Deviations of Improvement Score on the Cue and No-Cue trials, and the Cueing Effect, by Condition and Rebus Type Cue No-Cue Cueing Effect (Improvement Score Cue – No-Cue) Condition M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Neutral Rebus No Incubation .19 (.32) .14 (.28) .07 (.47) Incubation .27 (.37) .14 (.29) .11 (.51) Misleading Rebus No Incubation .24 (.25) .13 (.20) .14 (.31) Incubation .11 (.16) .09 (.16) .01 (.20) A 2 (Rebus Type) x 2 (Cue) x 2 (Condition) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Cue (Cue vs. No-Cue) was carried out on improvement scores to compare the degree of improvement in different incubation condition and in solving different types of Rebuses. The ANOVA results are presented in Table 4.3.6. 127 Table 4.3.633 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of Condition and Cue and Rebus Type on Improvement Scores. Source df MS F p ηp2 Between-Subjects Condition (Co) 1 .02 0.34 .56 .006 Rebus Type (R) 1 .02 0.38 .54 .006 Co x R 1 .11 1.86 .18 .030 Error ( between) 59 .06 Within-Subjects Cue (Cu) 1 .224 2.97 .09 .050 Cu x Co 1 .018 0.24 .63 .004 Cu x R 1 .001 0.01 .91 < .001 Cu x Co x R 1 .053 0.70 .41 < .012 Error (within) 59 .075 Neither the main effects nor the interaction effects approached significance, implying that, in both conditions and both types of Rebuses, the rate of improvement in the presence and the absence of a cue did not significantly differ. Experiment III fails to reveal any the facilitatory effect of incubation period on cue assimilation. However, the null finding in this experiment does not necessarily refute the existence of a positive role for an incubation period on cue assimilation. The absence of any cueing effect in this experiment may be due to the ineffectiveness of the cue used, indicated by the non-significant main effect of Cue (p = .09) on the improvement score. A more detail discussion on this view is presented in the next section. 128 Incubation Effects on Knowledge Activation Data from the lexical decision tasks were analysed to determine whether spreading activation and selective forgetting occur during the incubation period. Prior to these analyses, the data were first screened to remove outliers and incorrect responses. Accuracy on the lexical decision tasks was 93% or higher in all groups. Incorrect lexical decisions and correct decisions with extreme lexical decision time (< 50 ms or >2500ms) were discarded (1.2% of the data). After that, the mean and standard deviation of the lexical decision latencies were calculated for each participant. Within each participant, lexical decision times longer than 2SDs from the mean were recoded to the value found at 2 SDs. The recoded lexical latencies in some conditions had skew values exceeding 1.5. To diminish skew, the lexical decision times were logarithmically transformed. Spreading Activation: Participants’ lexical decision time to target words and neutral words in the LDT-before trials were examined to test the hypothesis that being misled is the criterion for the occurrence of spreading activation during an incubation period. The means and standard deviations of the natural and log-transformed lexical decision time for neutral words and target words in the LDT-before trials by Conditions and Rebus Type are presented in Table 4.3.7. 129 Table 4.3.734 Means and Standard Deviations of the Original and Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Times for Target Words and Neutral Words Neutral Rebus Target Word M (SD) M (SD) Condition No Incubation Neutral Word Misleading Rebus Target Word Neutral Word M (SD) M (SD) Original Lexical Decision Time 544.39 (83.04) Intermediate Load 546.42 (123.84) 625.27 (66.76) 561.58 (95.65) 634.64 (92.72) 645.48 (92.52) 567.81 (103.54) 643.34 (82.04) Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time No Incubation 2.73 (.07) 2.79 (.05) 2.74 (.07) 2.80 (.06) Intermediate Load 2.73 (.09) 2.81 (.06) 2.74 (.07) 2.81 (.06) It was predicted that participants who were assigned to solve misleading Rebuses in the Incubation condition would make quicker lexical decisions to target words relative to the control words. This LDT difference should be less noticeable with participants solving neutral Rebuses in the same condition and on those solving the misleading Rebuses in the No-Incubation condition. The log-transformed data were analyzed using a 2 (Word Type) x 2 (Rebus Type) x 2 (Condition) ANOVA with repeated-measures on Word Type (Target Word vs. Neutral Word). Table 4.3.8 presents the summary of the ANOVA results. 130 Table 4.3.835 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of RAT Difficulty, Condition, and Word Type on Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time df MS F p ηp2 Rebus Type (R) 1 .003 .52 .47 .009 Condition (C) 1 < .001 < .01 .96 < .001 RxC 1 < .001 .05 .82 .001 Error ( between) 61 .006 Word Type (W) 1 .054 34.80 < .001 .363 WxR 1 < .001 0.20 .66 .003 WxC 1 < .001 0.09 .77 < .001 WxRxC 1 < .001 0.11 .74 .002 Error (within) 61 .002 Source Between-Subjects Within-Subjects Participants generally made quicker lexical decisions to the target words than the neutral words, indicated by the significant Word Type effect, F(1, 61) = 34.80, p < .001,ηp2 = .363. The three way interaction between Condition x Rebus Type x Word was not significant, F(1, 61) = .04, p = .84, ηp2 = .001, implying that participants who solved misleading Rebuses in the Incubation condition were not more sensitized to relevant concepts, as compared with other participants. This does not support the suggestion that spreading activation occurs during an incubation period only when the problem solver is misdirected. 131 Selective Forgetting: Lexical decision times for misleading hints and neutral words were analysed to test the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis that an incubation period helps desensitize individuals to misleading but over-activated memory items. Only data from participants who were assigned to solve the misleading Rebuses were included in the analysis (N = 35). The means and standard deviations of the natural and log-transformed recoded lexical decision time for neutral words and misleading hints in the LDT-before trials by Conditions are presented in Table 4.3.9. Table 4.3.936 Means and Standard Deviations of the Original and Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Times for Misleading Hints and Neutral Words. Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Natural Lexical Decision Time Time Misleading Hints Neutral Words Condition Misleading Hints Neutral Words M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) No Incubation 588.27 (111.70) 604.38 (88.89) 2.76 (.07) 2.77 (.06) Incubation 595.17 (148.72) 612.10 (92.52) 2.77 (.09) 2.78 (.05) As a corollary to the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis, it was predicted that participants in the Incubation condition should have longer lexical decision times to misleading hints relative to neutral words, as compared with participants in the No-Incubation condition. A 2 (Word Type) x 2 (Condition) mixed-subjects ANOVAs with repeated-measures on Word Type (Misleading Hint vs. Neutral Word), using lexical decision time as the dependent variable, was carried out to verify the prediction. The summary of the ANOVA results is in Table 4.3.10. 132 Table 4.3.10371 ANOVA Summary Table for the Effect of the Condition and Word Type on Log-Transformed Lexical Decision Time df MS F p ηp2 Condition (C) 1 < .001 0.05 .83 .001 Error ( between) 32 .007 Word Type (W) 1 .005 2.12 .16 .060 WxC 1 < .001 0.02 .83 .001 Error (within) 32 .002 Source Between-Subjects Within-Subjects The main effect of Word Type was not significant, F(1, 32) = 2.12. p = .16, ηp2 = .06, indicating that participants’ sensitivity to neutral words and misleading hint was comparable. The non-significant interaction between Word Type and Condition implies that a similar non-significant difference was found in both Incubation and No-Incubation conditions. This is not in line with the prediction that participants in the Incubation condition would be less sensitive to misleading hints. Discussion Experiment III failed to replicate the findings of Experiment II on cue assimilation. Experiment III reported a non-significant cueing effect in both the Incubation and No-Incubation conditions. Similar to Experiment II, Experiment III reported a non-significant post-incubation change in participants’ sensitivity to target words. This does not support the possibility that spreading activation occurs during 133 incubation period only when problem solvers are misled. Experiment III also examined the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis, and the findings did not support the view that a selective-forgetting mechanism occurs during an incubation period to suppress further the activation level of the misleading concepts. As in Experiment II, the cues for Rebuses were presented as the items in LDTs. However, a significant cueing effect was not found in Experiment III. This raises the question about the validity and the generalizability of the findings of Experiment II. Yet, the failure in replicating the significant cueing effect in Experiment III may simply due to the ineffectiveness of the cues used in Experiment III. Unlike the cues presented in Experiment II, which were the answer of the unsolved RAT, the cues in Experiment III were just part of the answer, e.g., the word “high” was used as the cue to the Rebus with the answer “Search high and low”. Also, solving a Rebus requires the correct interpretation of both verbal AND visual clues (e.g., “you just me” = “just between you and me”). Therefore, simply presenting one single cue may have limited effect on the solution rate. This view is in line with Kershaw and Ohlsson’s (2004) account that difficulty of many insight problems is determined by multiple factors, and presenting one single cue can only remove a single factor and this is not effectively enough to help the individual solve the problem. The LDT performance data in Experiment III and II were more consistent with each other. Experiment III and II both revealed that the reaction times for target words were significantly shorter than the neutral words after the first approach to the unsolved problems, suggesting that activation spreads towards the answers of the problem during the initial attempt. However, this spreading-activation process did not continue during the incubation period further to sensitize participants to the answers, reflected in the null Condition effect on lexical decision latencies to target words in 134 both experiments. Also, the non-significant Condition effect on participants solving misleading Rebuses eliminates the possibility that spreading activation occurs during an incubation period only if an individual is misled. This pattern of findings does not support the Spreading-Activation hypothesis. Another key finding of Experiment III is the absence of a selective-forgetting mechanism during the incubation period. Experiment III revealed that participants in the Incubation condition were not less sensitive to the misleading hints, as compared with participants in the No-Incubation group. Also, in both conditions, the sensitivity to misleading hints and neutral words was comparable. There are two possible explanations for the non-significant differences. One explanation is that participants were not paying any attention to the misleading hints, and therefore, the misleading hints could not induce a misleading effect. This would explain the absence of a selective-forgetting mechanism during the incubation period. According to this account, participants’ Rebus performance should not be impaired by the presence of misleading hints. However, the number of correct responses to misleading Rebuses was significantly lower than the number of correct responses to neutral Rebuses. Also, each misleading hint was presented alone for one second prior to the presentation of the corresponding Rebus and during the presentation of that Rebus, and it would be very difficult for participants to ignore the misleading hint. Furthermore, if participants did not process the misleading hints at all, then participants’ sensitivity to the misleading hints should be the same, independent of whether the Rebuses were solved or not solved after the first attempt. However, the results of a repeated-measures ANOVA on log-transformed lexical decision latencies, using Word Type (Neutral Words, Misleading Hints of the solved Rebuses, and Misleading Hint of the unsolved Rebuses) as the within-subjects factor contradict this 135 prediction. The ANOVA reported a significant Word Type effect was found, F(1, 48) = 5.85, p = .02,ηp2 = .196. Subsequent pairwise comparisons indicated that the lexical decision time to misleading hints of the solved Rebuses was significantly shorter that the lexical decision time to the misleading hints of the unsolved Rebuses, p = .044 and to the neutral words, p < .001, and there was no lexical decision latency difference between neutral words and misleading hints of the unsolved Rebuses. Therefore, the possibility that participants did not process the misleading hints is rejected. Although participants in this experiment were misled by the misleading hints, they were able to suppress the activation of the misleading hints, indicated by the findings that participants were not sensitized to the misleading hints of the unsolved Rebuses. However, the finding that participants were sensitized to misleading hints of the solved Rebuses suggests that the suppression inhibition may not persist until the Rebus is solved. It may be that participants restructure the initial problem representation when they realize the irrelevance of the misleading hints and the inappropriateness of the initial problem representation. Once the representation is successfully restructured into a better form, the suppression mechanism may be terminated and the misleading hint would then be treated as part of the information presented in the task environment. For those who did not manage to restructure the problem, the suppression mechanism would persist. This may be the reason of the findings that participants’ sensitivity to the misleading hint of the Rebus at the end of the first attempt was the function of whether the Rebus was solved or unsolved. However, the introduction of the incubation period did not further strengthen the suppression mechanism, reflected in the null Condition x Word Type effect on the lexical decision times. This pattern of findings suggested that selective forgetting 136 occurs during the initial approach but not during the incubation period. This does not support the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis. The findings of Experiment III suggest that selective forgetting is not the mechanism underlying incubation period to overcome the fixation effect. 137 DISCUSSION: EXPERIMENT I, II, and III Experiment I, II, and III adapted a multi-dependent variable approach to examine the three unconscious-work hypotheses concerning incubation. The combined findings of these three experiments provides most support for the Opportunistic-Assimilation hypothesis, in which performing incubation tasks can facilitate the assimilation of chance-encountered cues, and that this cue-dependent incubation effect is moderated by the loading of the incubation tasks. This is consistent with the findings of the meta-analysis. The present experiments also further extend the meta-analysis findings by showing that problem difficulty is another potential moderator of the incubation effect. Experiment I, II, and III, however, do not offer evidence to support the Spreading-Activation and Selective-Forgetting hypotheses. The present experiments reveal that cue assimilation was more likely to occur when solving problems at an intermediate level of difficulty and when the incubation period was filled with intermediate-load tasks. Performing intermediate-load incubation tasks should occupy a part of participants’ cognitive resource and help generate a diffused attentional state. This is in line with the meta-analysis findings that incubation effects are most significant when the incubation periods are filled with irrelevant incubation tasks that can occupy part but not all of an individual’s cognitive resource and prevent a focused attentional state. These consistent findings support the contention that the processes underlying the incubation period are unconscious in nature. The significant interaction between problem difficulty and loading of the incubation task implies that problem difficulty is another moderator of the incubation effect. The non-significant changes in lexical decision latency suggest that the 138 incubation effects found in the experiments may not be the outcome of the occurrence of knowledge-activation processes during the incubation period. The findings of Experiment I, II, and III revealed that participants were sensitized to target words and they could also inhibit the activation of the misleading hints even if they did not solve the problems. However, introducing the incubation period did not further increase the activation level of the target words or strengthen the inhibition mechanism. The absence of spreading activation during the incubation period in Experiment III also undermines the possibility that spreading activation occurs during the incubation period when individuals are fixated on misleading concepts. Some may challenge the present findings as they are not fully consistent with the findings of some previous studies that also used LDTs to assess the incubation effect. Sio and Rudowicz (2008) created a list of misleading RATs where the stimuli of these RATs activate irrelevant knowledge in the domain of GO, and these RATs misled GO experts to believe that the misleading RATs were GO-relevant. They asked the Chinese GO experts to solve these misleading RATs and neutral RATs, either continuously (control group), with filled incubation period, or with two minutes rest. They have found that the GO experts in the filled-incubation condition made quicker lexical decisions to unsolved misleading RATs, as compared with those in the control condition. The disparity between Sio and Rudowicz ’s (2008) and Experiment III’s findings may be due to the difference in the nature of the misleading effect, as well as the cognitive characteristics of the participants in these two studies. In Sio and Rudowicz’s (2008) study, GO experts were asked to solve a list of RATs where the stimuli activate irrelevant GO-related knowledge. The misleading effect was the result of the automatic activation of the domain-specific knowledge. However, in 139 Experiment III, the misleading effect was induced by presenting an external object prior and during the presentation of Rebus. This misleading effect may not be as strong as the internally-generated fixation effect in Sio and Rudowicz’s (2008) study. Moreover, unlike the participants in Experiment III, the participants in Sio and Rudowicz’s study were GO experts and their GO-related knowledge was well connected and easily activated. When an individual is solving an insight problem containing a stimulus in his/her domain of expertise, domain-specific knowledge will be activated automatically, implying that the activation in a semantic network will be unevenly distributed. Having a well-structured task-specific knowledge network and being strongly fixated on irrelevant task-specific knowledge may be the necessary conditions for the occurrence of spreading activation. The study of Zhong, Dijksterhuis, and Galinsky (2008) also reported a decreased lexical decision times for answers of the unsolved RATs after performing irrelevant incubation tasks. However, a direct comparison between the findings of the present experiments and their study is inappropriate because of methodological differences between them. In Experiment I, II, and III, a set of LDTs was presented after the initial attempt to one RAT. In Zhong et al.’s (2008) study, participants had to solve a list of RATs and then a list of LDTs containing the answers of the previously-presented RATs. This means that participants in their study were having more than one unsolved RAT in the mind while performing the LDTs. Having many unsolved problems may magnify the feeling of reaching an impasse, and increase the perceived need for restructuring, and this may facilitate the occurrence of spreading-activation. Whether these kinds of meta-cognitive effect arise at a conscious or unconscious level remains an open question. The findings of Experiment III offer evidence to reject the Selective-Forgetting 140 hypothesis. Experiment III revealed that selective forgetting occurred during the initial approach to suppress the activation level of the misleading hints, yet this inhibition mechanism was not further strengthened during the incubation period. This does not support the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis. One may argue that the misleading effect generated in Experiment III was a relatively weak one and participants could overcome it easily. An incubation period would help suppress the activation of the misleading memory items only if individuals are strongly fixated on them. However, the study of Sio and Rudowicz (2008), using RATs that can generate a strong fixation effect also reported the absence of any activation-suppression mechanism during the incubation period. If selective forgetting is not the mechanism underlying an incubation period, then an alternative explanation is needed to account for the findings of the study of Smith and Blankenship (1989), which have been considered as strong evidence of the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis. Smith and Blankenship (1889) found that participants in the incubation condition were less able to recall the misleading cues and demonstrated a larger degree of performance improvement on the insight problems, as compared with those who had to solve the insight problems continuously. Smith and Blankenship (1989) concluded that forgetting of misleading cues was the mechanism underlying performance improvement. However, forgetting of misleading cues may simply reflect the natural decay of participants’ memory on the problem-related information after the incubation period. The incubation periods in Smith and Blankenship study were 5 and 15 minutes long, which are much longer that the incubation period in Experiment III and in the study of Sio and Rudowicz (2008). Therefore, it is likely that participants in Smith and Blankenship’s (1989) study had forgotten part of the problem-related information after the incubation period. Also, 141 they did not compare participants’ performance on recalling other problem-related information. Therefore, it is difficult to conclude whether the forgetting of the misleading cues is the result of selective or general forgetting. There is a possibility that the performance improvement was the outcome of the occurrence of other mechanisms during the incubation period. The co-occurrence of forgetting the misleading cue and performance improvement created an illusion that selective forgetting occurred during the incubation period to overcome a fixation effect. The absence of evidence for spreading activation and selective forgetting in the present experiments suggests that any restructuring processes that occur during the incubation period are not the knowledge-activation processes proposed by the unconscious account. It may be the case the incubation effects on cue assimilation reported in the present experiments are the outcome of the occurrence of meta-cognitive processes (e.g., search strategic shift). In order to verify this possibility, incubation experiments should be carried out on knowledge-lean visual insight problems, where their solutions require changes in search strategy. .A significant incubation effect in solving visual insight problem would be an evidence of the occurrence of meta-cognitive processes during an incubation period. The meta-analysis presented in the previous chapter has also suggested that incubation effects, and by implication the mechanisms by which individual attempts to solve problems, differed according to whether were linguistic or visual. Therefore, in order to fully understand the incubation effect, incubation experiments using visual insight problems are needed. Apart from examining the incubation effect on different classes of problems, there is another research gap that needs to be filled in order fully to understand the nature of incubation. The meta-analysis and the Experiments I, II, and III have 142 identified some key procedural factors moderating the incubation effect, and this offers a potential explanation to the mixed findings of past studies. However, not all the inconsistencies of past findings can be attributed to the heterogeneous experimental settings. For example, Olton and Johnson (1976) failed to obtain evidence of incubation, even following the experimental settings of previous incubation studies that reported positive incubation effect(e.g., Dreistadt, 1969) . This suggests the possibility of the existence of additional cognitive moderators of the incubation effect. One of the potential cognitive moderators may be attentional allocation styles. The findings of Experiment II and the meta-analysis results suggest that incubation tasks help create a diffuse attentional state that facilitates the occurrence of some relevant unconscious processes. According to this, individuals who typically have a focused attentional state should benefit more from the incubation period than those who have already have a tendency to allocate their attention broadly. Experiment IV, an incubation study of visual insight problems, was carried out to address these questions. 143 CHAPTER V: EXPERIMENT IV-VISUAL INSIGHT PROBLEMS The meta-analysis supports the existence of multiple types of problem-specific incubation effects. Experiment I, II, III investigated incubation effects on linguistic insight problems. Experiment IV was carried out to examine incubation effects on visual insight problems. Experiment IV also investigated the impact of the length of preparation period and attentional allocation styles on incubation in solving visual insight problems. The regression models presented in Chapter II have significant but limited predictive power, in that they can only account for 33% of the variance in the incubation effect sizes. This implies the some key moderators are missing from the model. The findings of Experiments I, II, and III have suggested that problem difficulty is one of these missing moderators. However, the moderators of incubation are not necessarily procedural in nature. The inconsistency of the findings between studies sharing the same or similar paradigm suggests the existence of non-procedural moderators. One of the potential non-procedural moderators is individuals’ attentional allocation styles. The findings of Experiments I, II, and III suggest that performing incubation tasks helps divert individuals’ attention away from the problem, and this facilitates relevant unconscious processes. According to this hypothesis, individuals who have a focused attentional state should benefit more from an incubation period than those who always allocate their attention broadly. As a consequence, even under the same procedural settings, individuals may still benefit differentially from an incubation period. However, most of the pervious incubation studies have overlooked the impact of this cognitive moderator. The lack of control of this cognitive characteristic may be 144 one of the reasons for the diversity of past findings. To fill this research gap, Experiment IV examined the link between attentional allocation styles and incubation effects. Examining the impact of attentional allocation styles on incubation effect also offers additional data to determine the moderating role of preparation periods on incubation. It has been suggested that a long preparation period allows individuals to explore the entire initial problem space to reach an impasse, which is a necessity for the occurrence of processes underlying restructuring, and these processes can be further facilitated by a post-impasse incubation period (Seifert et al., 1995). In line with this, the meta-analysis presented in Chapter III revealed that when solving visual insight problem, long preparation periods gave rise to larger incubation effects. If exhausting the problem space and reaching an impasse is crucial for benefiting from an incubation period, then individuals’ attentional allocation styles, which affect how efficient they are in executing focused search, should influence the incubation effect size. Also, the impact should be more profound when the preparation period is limited. When the preparation period is short, individuals who tend to allocate their attention broadly may not have enough time to exhaust the search space. Thus, they may be less likely to benefit from the incubation period. However, when the preparation period is long, most of the individuals (either with focused or diffuse attentional allocation style) would have enough time to reach an impasse, and therefore, have the same opportunity to benefit from the incubation period. Hence, if the proposed reach-of-impasse explanation is correct, then an interaction between preparation period length and individuals’ attentional allocation styles on the incubation effect size is predicted. The core difficulty in conducting an experiment to examine the impact of 145 attentional allocation style on incubation is the recruitment of participants with similar background (e.g., age, gender, intelligence) but significantly different attentional styles (focused vs. diffuse). The findings of studies carried out by Nisbett and his colleagues on cultural differences in cognitive processing offer a solution to this problem. Nisbett(2003) suggested that Asians and Westerners have different attentional allocation styles in problem solving, and have demonstrated this difference under laboratory settings. Ji, Peng and Nisbett(2000) found that Americans, compared with East Asians, were more field-independent, in that their perception of an object was less likely to be influenced by the background in which it appeared. Chua, Boland and Nisbett(2005) went further to examine if the field-independence difference was due to the ways Westerns and Asians allocate their attention in perception. They studied the eye movements of Chinese and American graduate students when perceiving scenes with a single foreground object on a complex background. American students were found to fixate more on the focal object, while Chinese students made more saccades to the background. These patterns of results suggest that Asians tends to allocate their attention broadly in scene perception while Westerns would focus narrowly on the focal object. Asian students were also more likely to bind the foreground object with the background, and they were also less likely to recognize correctly old foreground objects when presented in a new background. American students were less influenced by the backgrounds of the scenes, and they outperformed Asian students in recognizing the focal objects against either new or old background (Chua, Boland, & Nisbett, 2005). A similar pattern of performance differences was observed when Masuda and Nisbett(2006) asked Asian and Western students to view a sequence of scene pictures while changes in focal object and background were introduced during the sequence of presentations. They 146 found that Asian students were more likely to notice changes in contextual information (e.g., change of the background color) while Western students were more likely to notice changes in the foreground object. In summary, the findings of studies carried out by Nisbett and his colleagues converge to support the view that Asians have a tendency to allocate their attention broadly, while Westerners are usually narrowly focused in problem solving. If it is the case that an individual’s attentional allocation style is a moderator of the incubation effect, then an East-West difference in the degree to which they benefit from different types of incubation period is predicted. In Experiment IV, University students from Britain and Hong Kong were recruited because of the known differences in attentional allocation styles between Asians and Westerners. They were asked to solve a list of visual insight problems in a non-incubation condition, an incubation condition with a long preparation period, or an incubation condition with a short preparation period. The study predicted a significant moderating effect of the length of preparation period and attentional allocation style on incubation, and a significant interaction between these two factors. Method Participants Fifty University students from Hong Kong Universities (Female: 26, Male: 24) and 59 University students from Lancaster University (Female: 30, Male: 29) were recruited for this study. The mean age of the HK participants was 22.63 with a standard deviation of 3.74. The mean age of the UK participants was 19.19 with a standard deviation of 1.12. The reason for recruiting students from these two cultural 147 backgrounds is due to reported differences in attentional allocation styles between Asians and Westerners (Nisbett, 2003). Materials Eight visual insight problems were used as the problem-solving tasks in this study, and they were clustered into four different sets (two in each set), in terms of the nature of restructuring required. The English and Chinese4 descriptions of the problems are presented in Appendix I. These problems have been used as insight problems in many insight studies (Ash & Wiley, 2006; Gilhooly & Murphy, 2005; Ohlsson, 1992), excepting Problem 7 (Set 4), which is the modified version of the eight-coin insight problem used only by Ormerod, MacGregor, & Chronicle (2002). All these problems are knowledge-lean, in that their solutions require a shift of search strategy. Also, these visual problems are relatively language-free, which is very important for this study with participants speaking different languages. The intermediate-load Mental Rotation Tasks (MRTs) and Arithmetic Tasks (ATs) presented in Experiment III were used as the incubation tasks in this experiment. Witkin’s Group Embedded Figure Test(GEFT, Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, & Karp, 1971) was used to determine participants’ attentional allocation style. GEFT is originally designed as a measure of individuals’ field-dependence: the degree of perception of an object is influenced by the background or environment in which it resides. The test required individuals to trace out a simple shape embedded in a larger complex figure. It consists of a practice section containing seven simple items and 4 The Chinese descriptions were translated and confirmed by a bilingual (English-Chinese) psychology postgraduate in the University of Hong Kong. 148 two test sections, each of which contains nine more difficult items. The completion time limit for the second and the third sections was 5 minutes each. The test scores ranged from 0 to 18 depending on the number of shapes traced correctly. Field-independent individuals should be more effective in ignoring the surrounding field and disembedding the simple shapes from the complex figures, and in turn, score higher on the GEFT. There is a strong relationship between an individual’s degree of field-dependence and their attentional allocation style. Field-dependent individuals tend to allocate attention broadly and are more capable of noticing the global aspects of the to-be-processed information, while field-independent individuals tend to focus on partial aspect and better at focused and control processes (Bennink, 1982; Clark & Roof, 1988; Cochran & Davis, 1987; Goode, Goddard, & Pascual-Leone, 2002; Miyake, Witzki, & Emerson, 2001; Tsakaniko, 2006). In this study, GEFT was used to measure participants’ attentional allocation styles. It was expected that participants with more focused attentional allocation styles should have high scores on the GEFT. Procedure This experiment entailed a between-subjects design with incubation conditions (NI: No Incubation; IL: incubation with long preparation length; and IS: incubation with short preparation length) varying randomly between participants. Participants were tested individually and were randomly assigned to one of the three experimental conditions. In all conditions, participants were informed that they had first to solve a list of visual insight problems, MRTs and ATs, which were presented on a computer using bespoke software, and then proceed to finish the GEFT. 149 Before the start of the experiment, participants were first given task instructions and were exposed to one practice visual non-insight matchstick problem for 180 seconds or until the problem had been solved, and one practice MRT(8 seconds) and one practice AT(10 seconds). The main experiment consisted of 8 trials. In each trial, regardless of condition, participants were exposed to the same number of tasks: 1 visual non-insightful matchstick problem for 180 seconds in total or until the problem had been solved, and 5 MRTs (8 seconds each) and 8 ATs (10 seconds each). The presentation order and the relative timing of these tasks differed between conditions. In the NI condition, an insight problem was presented for 180 seconds or until it was solved, then participants performed 5 MRTs and 8 ATs. After that, they proceeded to the next trial involving another insight problem, followed by MRTs and ATs. In the IS condition, the insight problem was first presented for 90 seconds or until it was solved, participants were then prompted to solve the 5 MRTs and 8 ATs (incubation period). Following the interruption, participants proceeded to the next trial if they had solved the insight problem before the interruption. If not, the insight problem was presented for another 90 seconds (or until a solution was reached). Participants in the IL condition were interrupted to perform the MRTs and ATs while solving the insight problem, but the maximum presentation time of the insight problem at the pre- and post- incubation stage was 120 seconds and 60 seconds, respectively. Figure 5.1 illustrates the sequence of the various tasks in each condition. Eight trials were included in each condition, and the presentation order was varied over participants, and insight problems in the same set were not presented in consecutive trials. During the presentation of the insight problems, participants could respond at any time by first pressing the C key to pause the timer, then writing down the 150 response on the sheet provided and showing it to the experimenter for immediate judgment. If their answer was correct, they were prompted to do the MRTs and ATs; otherwise, they were asked to continue to solve the problem until the time was up or until a solution was reached. The presentation of the MRTs and ATs was the same as in Experiment II. The number of visual insight problems that were solved and the responses time were recorded. In the analyses, responses time was dropped as a performance indicator because a large variance in participants’ responding styles was observed, which may affect the reliability of this measure. Conservative participants pressed C and presented their answers to the researcher only if they were certain about the accuracy of their answers, while some participants would press C and present their answer even they were not sure whether the answers were correct or not. After finishing the computerized tasks, participants completed a paper-based GEFT. The test was administered following the standard procedures outlined in the GEFT manual. IS 90s on insight problem IL 120s on insight problem NI 180s on insight problem 90s on insight problem 120s on MRTs + ATs 120s on MRTs + ATs 60s on insight problem 120s on MRTs + ATs Figure 5.13. Task Presentation Order in Each Trial in Experiment IV. 151 Results To assess the incubation effect, the number of insight problems solved in the post-incubation period in the IS and the IL conditions was compared with performance in the NI condition in the equivalent time period5. If incubation periods facilitate problem solving processes, then participants in the IS and the IL conditions should solve more insight problems in the post-incubation period as compared with those in the NI condition in the equivalent time period. Furthermore, such performance differences between the IL and the NI conditions should be larger than the difference between the IS and the NI conditions if the preparation length is a positive moderator of incubation. Apart from this, a positive correlation between the post-incubation improvement and individuals’ GEFT scores is expected if individuals who tend to allocate attention narrowly show more likely to benefit from an incubation period. Also, the strength of this correlation in the IS condition should be stronger than in the IL condition if the proposed link between preparation length and attentional allocation styles on the incubation effect is correct. Background Statistics Participants’ gender and ethnic distribution, and the mean and the standard deviation of their GEFT scores (a higher score implies a more focused attentional allocation style) in each condition are presented in Table 5.1. 5 Performance during the 90th- 180th second in the NI condition was compared with the post-incubation performance in the IS condition. Performance during the 120 th- 180th second in the NI condition was compared with the post-incubation performance in the IL condition. 152 Table 5.138 Ethnic and Gender Distribution, and Means and Standard Deviations of the GEFT Scores by Condition. HK Gender UK GEFT Gender GEFT scores scores Male Female M (SD) Male Female M (SD) NI 7 7 16.00 (2.15) 9 9 12.28 (4.11) IS 10 7 15.82 (2.35) 10 10 13.75 (5.17) IL 9 9 15.72 (2.93) 10 10 14.55 (4.08) Condition A 2 (Ethnicity) x 3 (Condition) ANOVA using GEFT scores as the dependent variable was carried out to check if there was a HK-UK difference on GEFT scores within each condition. There was a significant Ethnicity effect on GEFT scores, F(1, 101) = 10.21, p = .002, η2 = .092, indicating that HK participants had significantly higher GEFT scores than the UK participants. This implies that HK participants are more field-independent. This is opposite to the findings reported by Nisbett(2003). An individual’s GEFT performance has been reported to be positively correlated with his/her academic achievement(e.g., Copeland, 1983; Luk, 2002; Murphy, Casey, Day, &Young, 1997), and therefore, the opposite difference may be due to the possibility that the HK participants outperformed the UK participants in academic matters. This sampling bias, however, does not invalidate this study. The core reason for recruiting participants from HK and UK was to maximize the variance in attentional allocation styles among the participants. The significant Ethnicity effect confirms that there was 153 a wide variance in attentional allocation styles (focused vs. diffuse) between participants, albeit in the opposite direction to that expected initially. This pattern of differences was found in all the three conditions, as neither the Condition effect (p = .54) nor the Ethnicity x Condition (p = .37) effect was significant. Table 5.2 presents the solution rate of each visual problem. In order to calculate the solution rate in an un-interrupted problem solving setting, only data from participants in the No-Incubation condition (N = 32) were used. Insight problems in Set 4 were excluded in all the following analyses because of the extreme within-set and between-set differences in solution rate. 154 Table 5.239 Solution Rate for the Visual Insight Problems (N = 32) Solution Rate Set 1: Insight Problem 1 88% Insight Problem 2 72% Set 2: Insight Problem 3 59% Insight Problem 4 31% Set 3: Insight Problem 5 78% Insight Problem 6 78% Set 4: Insight Problem 7 90% Insight Problem 8 12% 155 Table 5.3 presents the mean and the standard deviations for the number of insight problem solved during the pre- and post-incubation period in the IS and the IL conditions, and during the equivalent time period in the NI condition. GEFT scores were positively correlated with the number of insight problems solved at the pre-incubation stage. This is consistent with the past findings (e.g., Frank & Noble, 1985; Witkin et al., 1971) that higher GEFT scores predicts better insight problem solving performance. Table 5.340 Means and Standard deviations of the Number of Insight Problem Solved in the Pre and Post-Incubation Period in the IS and IL Conditions, and in the Equivalent Time Period in the NI Condition. Number of Insight Problem Solved IS NI IL NI M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Pre-Incubation 2.95 (1.45) 3.00 (1.63) 3.00 (1.26) 3.50 (1.24) Post-Incubation 0.70 (0.78) 1.06 (1.01) 1.00 (0.89) 0.56 (0.56) 0.29+ 0.47** 0.39* 0.46** Time Period Correlation between no. of problems solved in pre-incubation period and GEFT scores + p < .06. *p < .05. **p < .001. 156 In the main analyses, regressions were carried out to examine the incubation effect in the IS and the IL conditions separately. To avoid over-fitting the regression models with irrelevant variables, two backward stepwise multiple regressions were first carried out to examine if any of the proposed predictor variables was redundant: one regression for the IS vs. NI comparison and one for the IL vs. NI comparison,. In both backward regression analyses, the outcome variable was the number of insight problems solved during the post-incubation period or the equivalent time period in the NI condition. The predictor variables included Initial Performance (the number of problem solved during the pre-incubation period or the equivalent time period in the NI condition), Gender (female vs. male), Ethnicity (UK vs. HK), Condition (No Incubation vs. Incubation), GEFT (GEFT scores, ranging from 0-18). The categorical variables Gender, Ethnicity, and Condition were dummy-coded. The categories (with the associated predictor variables following in parentheses) of female (Gender), HK (Ethnicity), and No Incubation (Condition) were used as reference groups in the analyses, and their coefficients were restricted to zero. Table 5.4 presents a summary of the results of the complete and the backward eliminated regression models. In both the IS vs. NI and the IL vs. NI regression analysis, Ethnicity was found to be a non-significant predictor variable and was removed in the backward eliminated model. Therefore, Ethnicity was not included in the main regression analyses. The tolerance statistics of each of the remaining predictors are all well above 0.2, suggesting that there was no collinearity within the data. 157 Table 5.441 Summary of the Results of the Complete and the Backward Eliminated Model IS vs. NI (N = 69) B SE B β Tolerance IL vs. NI (N = 72) F Adjusted B SE B β R2 statistics Tolerance F Adjusted R2 statistics Complete 4.90* .223 .219+ .997 .004 .870 .084 -.239+ .737 .020 .027 .098 .733 .334 .189 .213+ .884 .342 .176 .219+ .997 -.153 .082 -.240+ .752 Gender .506 .199 .281* .932 .342 Ethnicity -.232 .220 -.129 .775 .007 Initial Performance -.318 .075 -.129* .727 -.152 GEFT .047 .027 .210* .802 Condition -.399 .194 -.221+ .986 .478 .198 .265* .948 .070 -.491* .818 .177 .190 2.63* .104 3.33* .118 Backward Eliminated Gender Initial Performance -.292 5.83* .221 158 + GEFT .054 .026 .239* .848 .020 .026 .097 .781 Condition -.399 .194 -.221* .986 .334 .187+ .213+ .884 p < .10. *p < .05. 159 Moderating Effect of Preparation Length and Attentional Allocation Style Two hierarchical regressions were carried out, one for the IS vs. NI comparison and one for the IL vs. NI comparison, to examine the impact on post-incubation performance of the preparation length and attentional allocation styles, as well as the interaction between them. In both regression analyses, the outcome variable was the number of insight problems solved during the post-incubation period or the equivalent time period in the NI condition. The predictor variables included Initial Performance, Gender, Condition, GEFT, and the interaction variable GEFT x Condition, which was the multiplicative term of the variable GEFT and Condition. The predictor variables were separated into four blocks. The variables in the first block were the control variables, which included Gender, Ethnicity, and Initial Performance. To examine the impact of the incubation period on insight problem solving performance, Condition was added in the second block. GEFT and GEFT x Condition were placed in the third and the forth block of each model respectively to examine if participants’ attentional allocation style could moderate the incubation effect and if the moderating effect was more significant in the IS condition. A positive coefficient of Condition was expected if the incubation period enhances performance. The coefficient in the IL vs. NI model should be larger than in the IS vs. NI model if the preparation length is the positive moderator of the incubation effect. A positive coefficient of GEFT is also expected because of the reported positive correlation between GEFT scores and insight problem solving performance (e.g., Frank & Noble, 1985; Witkin et al., 1971). The magnitude of the coefficient of GEFT x Condition could indicate if there is any link between attentional allocation style and incubation. If the hypothesis that individuals with a focused attentional allocation style benefit more from the incubation period is correct, then a significant and positive coefficient of this interaction variable is expected. Also, 160 according to the predicted interaction between the preparation length and attentional allocation styles on the incubation effect, the coefficient of the variable GEFT x Condition in the IS vs. NI model should be larger than the one in the IL vs. NI model. Table 5.5 presents the summary of the results of these two hierarchical regression models as each additional block of variable was entered. 161 Table 5.542 Summary of Results of the Two Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses IS vs. NI (N = 69) Block 1 Variable B SE B F IL vs. NI (N = 72) Adjusted Sig. R2 F-change .155 .001 B SE B .347 .179 .222+ -.159 .073 -.250* .348 .175 .223* -.399* -.124 .073 -.195+ .198 -.179+ .370 .181 .236* .198 .265* .342 .176 .219+ Gender .458 .205 .255* Initial -.236 .068 -.397* Gender .445 .202 .247* Initial -.237 .067 Condition -.355 Gender .478 7.24* F Adjusted Sig. R2 F-change 4.11* .082 .021 4.27* .123 .044 3.33* .118 .448 Performance 2 6.06* .182 .078 Performance 3 5.83** .221 .044 162 -.292 .070 -.491* -.153 .082 -.240+ Condition -.399 .194 -.221* .334 .187 .213+ GEFT .054 .026 .239* .020 .026 .097 Gender .460 .201 .256* .375 .178 .240* Initial -.285 .072 -.480** -.155 .082 -.243+ Condition -.817 .742 -.453 1.082 .707 .690 GEFT .036 .041 .159 .048 .037 .234 GEFT x .029 .050 .259 -.052 .048 -.529 Initial Performance 4 4.68* .213 .576 Performance Condition + p < .08. *p < .05. **p < .001. 163 2.91* .120 .277 In the IS vs. NI model, the negative coefficients of Condition, 2-Block: = -.179, p = .07, 3-Block: = -.221, p = .04, indicated that having an incubation period preceded by a short preparation period impaired performance. Yet, a positive incubation effect was found with a long preparation period, as reflected by the positive coefficient of Condition in the IL vs. NI model, 2-Block: = .236, p = .04. This supports the moderating effect of the length of preparation period on incubation. The predicted interaction between GEFT and Condition is not found, IS vs. NI: = -.259, p = .56, IL vs. NI: = .-53, p = .28, indicating that participants with different GEFT score did not benefit differentially from an incubation period. This does not support the hypothesis that individuals with more focused attentional allocation style benefit more from the incubation period. However, there was an unexpected interaction between preparation length and GEFT on the outcome variable. In the IS vs. NI model, the coefficient of GEFT was .24, p = 0.04, indicating that participants with focused attentional allocation styles solved more insight problems during the post-incubation period or during the equivalent time period in the NI condition. The inclusion of GEFT in the regression model also amplified the negative incubation effect (without GEFT: = -.197, p = .08; with GEFT: = -.221, p = .04). This indicated that controlling for individual differences in attention allocation styles improved the stability of the negative incubation effect. In the IL vs. NI model, the impact of GEFT scored was attenuated, = .097, p = .45, and adding the variable GEFT to the model decreased the explanatory power of it, ∆ Adjusted R2 = -.005. This discrepancy suggests that the impact of attentional allocation style on incubation varies depending on the preparation length. 164 The number of problems solved at the pre-incubation period was a strong negative predictor in both the IS vs. NI and the IL vs. NI models. This is not surprising because the more problems participants solved at the beginning, the fewer problems remained unsolved during the post-incubation period or the equivalent time period in the NI condition, and this certainly limited the maximum number of problems individuals could solve during that time period. There was also a robust Gender effect, such that male participants outperformed female participants in the post-incubation problem solving stage. The effect was significant even after controlling for the pre-incubation performance and individuals’ attentional allocation styles. Discussion This experiment demonstrated a positive incubation effect on insight problem-solving and a significant moderating effect of preparation length on incubation, and more importantly, revealed that there is an interaction between individuals’ attentional allocation styles and preparation length on the incubation effect. The difference in the regression coefficient of Condition between the IS vs. NI and the IL vs. NI model suggests that individuals benefit from an incubation period only if it is preceded by a long preparation period. This is consistent with the meta-analysis reported in Chapter 3, and supports the suggestion that a long preparation period allows individuals to explore the entire initial faulty problem space and reach an impasse, which would maximize the likelihood of the occurrence of the restructuring processes during the incubation period (Seifert et al., 1995). The negative impact of Condition in the IS model suggests that preparation length not only can amplify, but also reverse the incubation effect. In the IS 165 condition, the preparation period was too short for participants to exhaust the initial problem space and reach an impasse. An “early” incubation period may interrupt the initial conscious search, delays the onset of the impasse, and inhibits the occurrence of restructuring. Therefore, continued effort will indeed be more effective because individuals can have more time to search through the problem space and reach an impasse, and move on to the post-impasse restructuring stage. One may argue that the positive coefficient of Condition in the IL vs. NI model does not necessarily imply a positive role of incubation period in visual insight problem-solving. It could be that participants in the IL condition had inferior pre-incubation performance, as compared with those in the NI condition. As a consequence, participants in the IL condition had a better chance to show improvement in the post-incubation problem solving period. If an initial performance difference existed between participants in these two conditions, then a significant association between the predictor variables Condition and Initial Performance in the IL vs. NI model would be expected. A multiple regression was carried out using the Initial Performance as the outcome variable, and Gender and Condition as the predictor variables, was carried out. The regression model did not reach significance, F(1, 69) = 1.56, p = .22, and none of the predictor variables had a significant impact on the outcome variable, Gender: = .067, p = .58, Condition: = -.197, p = .10. Therefore, this suggestion can be rejected, and the findings of this experiment support the conclusion that preparation length is a positive moderator of the incubation effect with visual insight problems. Another key finding is the differential impact of GEFT in the IS vs. NI and the IL vs. NI models. The IS vs. NI comparison revealed that participants with focused attentional allocation style (indicated by the GEFT scores) solved more insight problems during the post-incubation period or during the equivalent time period in the NI condition. One may 166 suggest that this finding simply reflects the positive correlation between GEFT scores and insight problem solving performance, which has been reported in past studies (e.g., Frank & Noble, 1985; Witkin et al., 1971). However, this explanation fails to account for the non-significant contribution of GEFT in the IL vs. NI model. Therefore, a more sophisticated account is required, and it is suggested that it could again be related to the importance of reaching an impasse during insight problem-solving. As mentioned previously, the preparation period in the IS condition may be too short for participants to exhaust the initial problem space to reach an impasse. When participants in the IS condition re-approached the problem in the post-incubation period, they resumed the interrupted conscious search. Participants with a more focused attentional state should be more effective in navigating the problem space, implying that they would be more likely to reach an impasse during the post-incubation period and restructure the problem representation. Therefore, they would still outperform those participants who have a diffused attentional allocation style. The positive impact of having a focused attentional state would then overcome the general negative effect of the “early” incubation period. Accordingly, controlling individual differences in this cognitive style should make the negative effect of incubation period more significant. In line with this prediction, the regression coefficient of “Incubation” was changed from non-significant to significantly negative after adding the variable “GEFT” into the IS model. This reach-impasse explanation also account for the non-significant contribution of GEFT in the IL vs. NI model. Participants in the IL condition had a longer preparation period to explore the initial problem space. This increased the chance of encountering an impasse at pre-incubation stage, especially for those with a diffused attentional allocation style. Therefore, participants with different attentional allocation style would still have the 167 same opportunity to benefit from the incubation period. It means that individuals’ attentional allocation style should no longer be a strong predictor on the post-incubation performance in the IL condition. This explains the non-significant impact of “GEFT” in the IL model. The combined findings of the IS vs. NI and the IL vs. NI model support the importance of reaching an impasse in order to benefit from an incubation period. Inconsistent with the hypothesis, participants with different attentional allocation styles benefited from (IL condition) or were impaired by (IS condition) the incubation period in the same degree, as indicated by the non-significant coefficient of the interaction variable GEFT x Condition in both models. It may be the case that the link between attentional allocation style and the magnitude of the incubation effect is significant only when external cues are presented during the incubation period. Individuals who are narrowly focused are less capable of noticing the external cues, as compared with those allocating their attention broadly (Ansburg & Hill, 2003). Performing incubation tasks should help these individual become less focused, and increase the chance of cue-assimilation. There is also an unexpected finding that male participants demonstrated a larger improvement than female participants in the post-incubation insight problem-solving stage. This effect remained significant even after controlling for the pre-incubation performance and GEFT scores. This observed association cannot be simply attributed to the male-female differences in general visual insight problem-solving ability. One possible explanation is that male participants benefited more than the female participants from the incubation period. To examine this possibility, the interaction variable Gender x Condition was added into the 4-block regression models presented in Table 5.5. However, this additional variable was not a significant predictor in the IS vs. NI, = -.185, p = .33, and the IL vs. NI model, = -.204, p = .32, suggesting that male and female participants benefitted from the incubation 168 period to a similar degree. An alternative account of this gender effect may be related to their differential tendency to switch spatial/visual search strategies at the post-impasse stage. Male participants might be more likely than female participants to switch to other spatial/visual search strategies after reaching an impasse, and therefore, they are more likely to overcome the impasse and solve the problems in the post-impasse period. This explanation is not without ground. There are numerous studies revealing that males tend to outperform female on a diverse set of spatial tests (see review in Linn & Petersen, 1985), and flexible spatial switching may be one of the mechanisms of their superior performance on this aspect. The findings of this experiment not only further the current understanding of the nature of incubation, but also offer us a new perspective on the nature of processes underlying restructuring. The current debate on the nature of post-impasse restructuring is dominated by two opposite accounts: conscious- and unconscious-work. Researchers in the conscious-work stream consider insight problem solving as a conscious and controlled strategic search of the problem space, with impasse being due to the use of wrong search heuristics and with the resolution of impasse dependent on modifying the initial search heuristic (Kaplan & Simon, 1990; MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001). Alternatively, the unconscious-work account proposes that impasse is caused by the inappropriate use of task-specific information and knowledge, and it can be overcame by inhibiting the activation of misleading items and spreading-activation to relevant items in long-term memory. The findings of this study offer support to both of these two polarized accounts. The positive incubation effect in the IL condition suggests that the post-impasse restructuring processes should function at a sub-conscious level. Otherwise, the restructuring processes 169 and incubation tasks would compete for the same limited cognitive resource, and participants’ insight problem solving performance should be impaired. Researchers supporting the conscious-work account on incubation may argue that the incubation period provides extra time for participants to work on the problem covertly. However, this explanation is contradicted by the finding of a negative incubation effect in the IS condition. The findings of this experiment support the suggestion that incubation tasks occupy part of the problem solver’s attention, and the resulting diffused attentional state facilitates the occurrence of unconscious post-impasse restructuring processes (Ansburg & Hill, 2003; Finke, Ward, & Smith, 1992; Martindale, 1995). The processes underlying restructuring, however, may not necessarily be the memory-activation processes proposed by the unconscious-work account. The insight problems adapted in this experiment were knowledge-lean problems (except the matchstick arithmetic problems), in that their solution does not call upon access (or suppression) of the task-specific knowledge. The restructuring processes of these insight tasks are more likely to be at the meta-cognitive level (e.g., strategic switching). This account is consistent with provides the strategy-based restructuring proposed by the conscious-work account. In sum, Experiment IV supports the positive role of incubation, and suggests that preparation length and individuals’ attentional allocation styles determine how likely it is that individuals encounter an impasse before the incubation period, which in turn affect whether they benefit from an incubation period. The significant moderating effect of individuals’ attentional allocation styles also implies that, even under identical experimental settings, it is not guaranteed that we can always replicate the same pattern of incubation effect. In order to examine the true effect of incubation periods, future studies should control for the individual differences in this cognitive style. Our results also provide 170 empirical evidence for unconscious restructuring, and suggest that restructuring may rely on both cognitive and meta-cognitive processes. An integration of unconscious- and conscious-work accounts is needed in order to build a complete model of insight. 171 CHAPTER VI: GENERAL DISCUSSION A meta-analysis and four experiments were carried out in an attempt to identify the potential procedural and cognitive moderators of incubation, and to examine the three dominant unconscious-work hypotheses of the incubation effect: spreading activation, selective forgetting, and cue assimilation. The findings converge to support the existence of unconscious processes during incubation, and suggest that the magnitude of the incubation effect varies depending on the loading of the incubation tasks, the type and the difficulty of the problem, preparation length, and an individual’s attentional allocation style. The meta-analysis, synthesizing results of 117 studies (extracted from 29 publications), revealed that the incubation effect is highly susceptible to task-specific procedural moderators. In linguistic insight problems, light-load incubation tasks yielded a stronger incubation effect than did rest or heavy load tasks. Light-load tasks may help divert an individual’s attention to other areas, and this facilitates helpful unconscious processes. In visual insight problems, the length of the preparation period was the only significant moderator. Four experiments were then carried out to examine the incubation effect on linguistics and visual insight problems. Three experiments were conducted on linguistic problems (Experiment I, II, & III) with the objectives being to verify the three unconscious-work hypotheses, and to examine the impact on incubation of the loading of the incubation tasks. These three experiments compared the incubation effects when solving linguistic problems at different difficulty levels (easy, intermediate, and difficult) and when having incubation period filled with tasks of varying cognitive loadings (low, intermediate, and high) were compared. To examine the incubation effect on problem-solving performance and sensitivity to relevant and irrelevant 172 items, the traditional paradigm that only measures performance improvement was supplemented with lexical decision tasks to assess the change of sensitivity to relevant and irrelevant concepts. Experiments I, II, and III found that cue assimilation occurred when solving problems at an intermediate level of difficulty and when the incubation period was filled with tasks at intermediate-load. This supports the cue-assimilation hypothesis and the meta-analysis findings, and also suggests an additional moderator: problem difficulty. Also, participants were sensitized to the relevant items and could ignore the misleading cues, even though they could not solve the problems at the first attempt. However, the sensitivity to these two item types remains the same after the incubation period, suggesting the absence of any activation redistribution. Therefore, the Spreading-Activation and Selective-Forgetting hypotheses are not supported. Experiment IV was carried out to examine the impact of the preparation length and attentional allocation style on incubation, and the interaction between them. Again, the findings of the experiment were in line with the meta-analysis results, revealing that increasing the preparation time can change the incubation effect size from negative to positive. Also, controlling individuals’ attentional allocation styles helps reduce the variance in incubation effect size among participants. The findings also confirm the interaction between preparation length and attentional allocation styles of individuals on incubation. It is suggested that individuals with focused attentional allocation styles are more likely to exhaust the initial search space and reach an impasse, and reaching an impasse is a necessary condition for the occurrence of restructuring, which can be further facilitated by performing incubation tasks. The combined findings of the meta-analysis and the experimental studies have offered an explanation of the inconsistencies between past findings, and further our current 173 understanding of the incubation period. The impact of the procedural and cognitive moderators on incubation suggests that the variance of past findings can be attributed to differences in experimental parameters and the lack of control of cognitive moderators. However, attentional allocation style is not the only cognitive moderator of the incubation effect. It has been reported that individuals’ working memory span is an indicator of their ability to control attention (Kane, Bleckley, Conway, & Engle, 2001), and this affects how fast they are in exhausting the initial problem space and reaching an impasse. Therefore, an individual’s working memory should also affect they can benefit from an incubation period. Another potential moderator is the characteristics of an individual’s knowledge network. As mentioned earlier, the structure of an individual’s knowledge network (e.g., the richness of the connectivity and the strength of the inter-connections), may affect the occurrence of spreading activation during the incubation period, and in turn moderate the incubation effect. However, none of these proposed cognitive factors have been examined in past incubation studies, and so further research adopting an individual-differences approach should be carried out in order to fill this gap. The novel paradigm used in these experiments has also generated strong data to examine the underlying mechanism of incubation. It has long been suggested that there are gradual and unconscious cognitive processes underlying incubation. However, the heterogeneity of settings, insensitivity of measurements, and diversity of the findings make it very difficult to draw any conclusions on the role of incubation. The multi-dependent-variable paradigm used in these experiments offered strong data to support the cue-assimilation hypothesis and confirm the absence of spreading-activation and selective-forgetting during the experimental incubation periods. However, Sio and Rudowicz (2007), using a similar paradigm, reported evidence for the Spreading-Activation 174 hypothesis. They revealed, after a short incubation period, GO experts became more sensitive to the answer of the RATs which contain irrelevant GO information. These mixed findings point to the possibility that spreading-activation occurs only if the fixation effect is very strong or/and only if individuals’ task-related knowledge is well connected (rich and strong inter-node connections) and easily activated. Therefore, instead of completely rejecting the Spreading-Activation hypothesis, further studies have to be carried out to examine the conditions for the occurrence of spreading-activation. Both the present experiments and those of Sio and Rudowicz (2007) reported the absence of selective forgetting during the incubation period. These studies offer evidence to reject the Selective-Forgetting hypothesis. Although spreading activation and selective forgetting did not occur during the incubation period, participants’ sensitivity towards the target words and misleading hints remained unchanged after the incubation period. There was no dissipation on the activation of the target words and any weakening of the inhibition of the misleading hints even when participants had shifted attention to the incubation tasks. This finding is analogous to the replication of the Zeigarnik effect (1927, 1938), in that individuals remember unsolved problems better. The present experiments show that the representation of an unsolved problem is maintains at a heightened level. The persistence of such activation may provide a basis for some other unconscious problem-solving processes to occur during the incubation period. The non-significant change of participants’ sensitivity to relevant and irrelevant items after the incubation period in the linguistic experiments suggests that these unconscious processes may not be semantically-based. The positive incubation effect on visual insight problems, in which solving them requires strategic switching/releasing unnecessary search constraints, suggests the possibility of the occurrence of meta-cognitive 175 processes during an incubation period. These findings not only offer a new perspective on the mechanism of incubation period, but also question the commonly held assumption in human cognitive research that meta-cognitive processes require conscious awareness (Brown, Bransford, Ferrara, & Campione, 1983; Flavell, 1979; Flavell & Wellman, 1977). Recently, a few studies have supported the possibility of unconscious meta-cognition, showing that individuals can change their problem-solving strategy to optimize performance without conscious awareness (Newton & Roberts, 2005; Reder & Schunn, 1996; Siegler & Stern, 1998). However, the tasks examined in these studies are relatively simple, such as arithmetic computation (Reder & Schunn, 1996; Siegler & Stern, 1998) and spatial tasks (Netwon & Roberts, 2005). The simplicity of the tasks used in these studies has limited the implications of the findings. Examining the strategic aspect of insight problem-solving and the impact of an incubation period should bring further data to the debate on unconscious meta-cognition. The findings of this thesis also challenge the current perspective of insight problem-solving. The current debate on the nature of insight is dominated by two accounts: The Representational Change Theory (RCT, Knoblich, Ohlsson, Haider, & Rhenius, 2001; Ohlsson, 1992;) and the Criterion for Satisfactory Progress theory (CSPT, Macgregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001). RCT proposes that, unlike non-insight problem solving, insight emerges after an individual restructures the problem representation, which is the outcome of the occurrence of unconscious processes. CSPT, however, suggests insight problem-solving relies on a conscious change in search strategy, and such strategic shifts occur only if an individual reaches an impasse, and this is no different from the non-insight problem processes. Studies have been carried out to test between these the two theories. However, it may be a mistake to consider the RCT and CSPT as two opposite and mutually 176 exclusive accounts. The findings of the linguistic experiments suggest that unconscious cognitive processes occur during the incubation period, and in turn, facilitate the post-incubation problem solving performance. This supports RCT in that insight problemsolving processes are unconscious. The findings of the visual experiments, however, support CSPT. CSPT suggests that insight problem solving relies on a conscious change in search strategy, and such strategic shifts occur only if an individual reaches an impasse. In the visual experiments, participants benefit from the incubation period only if they have reached an impasse already, and introducing the incubation period too early induces a negative effect on problem-solving performance. Instead of perceiving the RCT and CSPT as two competing accounts, it is possible that these two theories capture different aspects of the insight problem-solving process. An integration of these two theories is needed in order to explain fully the insight problem-solving process. The present findings also suggest that it is a misconception to regard insight and non-insight problem-solving processes as involving two distinctive types of processes. Instead, it is suggested that insight problem-solving can be divided into two stages. In the first stage, individuals carry out a focused search of the initial problem space to find out and execute the moves to reach a solution, and this process is no different from the non-insight problem-solving processes. However, the settings of the insight problem usually mislead individuals to build an inappropriate initial problem representation, which does not include all important moves for solving the problem. Therefore, instead of reaching a solution, individuals usually reach an impasse. Restructuring the problem representation will then occur to overcome the impasse, and this process is facilitated by a diffuse attentional state. A recent study of Ash and Wiley (2006) also offers some empirical support to this proposal. They asked participants to solve a list of visual insight problems with different, initially 177 faulty search spaces. They found that increased individual ability to control attention predicted a better performance in solving insight problems with a large search space. However, such a correlation was not observed when solving problems with a small search space. They suggest that when solving problem with a large search space, participants with better control of attention are quicker in exploring the initial search space and better at maintaining different failed solutions in memory. Therefore, it is quicker for such participants to realize that the initial search space is incorrect, and so they started the restructuring process earlier. This is consistent with the proposal that the pre-impasse insight problem stage involves a controlled and focused search through the problem space. When solving problems with a small faulty search space, most participants would reach an impasse quickly. Ash and Wiley suggest that performance on the problems depends solely on how successful the restructuring process is, and the absence of any link between the ability to control attention and success in solving problems with a small initial search space implies that the restructuring process is not necessarily a conscious search process. In sum, this PhD thesis suggests that successful insight problem-solving requires a shift of attentional state from focused to diffuse after reaching an impasse. However, no studies have yet been done that measure the attentional state at pre- and post-impasse insight problem-solving stages, and correlate them with the occurrence of insight-related cognitive processes and the insight problem solving performance. To measure attentional state and the cognitive mechanisms during insight problem-solving, advanced techniques such as EEG have to be adapted. EEG data can identify neural activities associated with different cognitive processes, such as focused search, generating constraints, and encoding and processing semantic information (Klimesch, 1999; Sauseng, Klimesch, Schabus, & Doppelmayr, 2005, Klimesch, Doppelmayr, Pachinger, & Ripper, 1997). Comparing the 178 EEG data at the pre-and post-impasse stage should offer strong evidence to test the two-stage model. There are few attempts to measure the neural activity during insight problem-solving. However, most of them only record the neuroimaging data either at pre-problem presentation (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Kounios et al., 2006) or during the pre-solution period (Kounios et al., 2006). For those assessing the actual insight problem-solving processes, they have not taken the importance of reaching an impasse into account. For example, Luo, Niki, and Knoblich (2006) examined brain activation levels at different time intervals. However, the validity of this approach depends on whether individuals reach the same problem state at similar times. This PhD thesis, along with the study of Ash and Wiley (2006), suggest that there are individual differences in the onset of impasse, depending on how quickly an individual executes the controlled search and exhausts the initial problem space. Therefore, in order to capture accurately the nature of pre-impasse and post-impasse insight problem solving processes, one must know the onset of impasse for each individual. Examining an individual’s eye-movement allows us to time-lock the onset of impasse without interrupting the problem solving process (Knoblich, Ohlsson, & Raney, 2001). One direction for future research would be combining EEG and eye-tracking, or other kinds of advanced techniques to study insight. Overall, this PhD thesis has furthered the current theoretical understanding of incubation and insight. The meta-analysis and the experimental studies reveal that the incubation effect is moderated by numerous procedural and cognitive moderators, and the inconsistency of past studies is very likely due to the lack of control for these factors. The positive effect of performing irrelevant incubation tasks on insight problem solving supports the conceptualization of unconscious restructuring. Furthermore, the findings of the present 179 experiments suggest that unconscious restructuring occurs only if an individual has exhausted the search space and reached an impasse. This implies that successful insight problem solving is the joint effort between pre-impasse conscious search and post-impasse unconscious processes. This offers support to both the RCT and CSPT theories, and also suggests the necessity of integrating these two theories. In a broader sense, the positive incubation effect with knowledge-lean problems raises a question about whether meta-cognition requires awareness. In order to have a more complete picture of how insight emerges and how it can be facilitated, there are few questions that we need to address, including the nature of cognitive processes in pre- and post-impasse insight problem-solving (conscious vs. unconscious), and the nature of the restructuring (semantic, strategic, or both). To answer these questions, further studies have to combine different advanced techniques to capture neural, cognitive, and behavioral data on insight problem solving. 180 REFERENCES (References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis) Adelson, B. (1984). When novices surpass experts: The difficulty of a task may increase with expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10 (3), 483-495 Anderson, R. J. (1994). Representations and requirements: The value of ethnography in system design. Human-Computer Interaction, 9 (2), 151-182. Ansburg, P. I., & Hill, K. (2003) Creative and analytic thinkers differ in their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differences. 34, 1141-1152. Ash, I. K., & Wiley J. (2006). The nature of restructuring in insight: An individual-differences approach. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13 (1), 66-73. Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn M. J. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A Meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24. *Beck, J. (1979). The effect of variations in incubation activity and incubation time on creative response production. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University. Bedard, J. & Chi, M. T. H. (1992). Expertise. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1 (4), 135-139. *Bennett, S. (1975). Effect of incubation on the associative process of creativity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Notre Dame. Bennink, C. D. (1982). Individual differences in cognitive style, working memory, and 181 semantic integration. Journal of Research in Personality, 16, 267-280. *Both, L., Needham, D., & Wood, E. (2004) Examining tasks that facilitate the experience of incubation while problem solving. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 50 (1), 57-67. Bowden, E.M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Aha! Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 730-737. Bowden, E. M., & June-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35 (5), 634-639. Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C. G., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72-110. *Brockett, C. (1985). Neuropsychological and cognitive components of creativity and incubation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Virginia Commonwealth University. Brown, A. L., Bransford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In J. H. Flavell, & E. M. Markman ( Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed.): Cognitive development, 3, 77-166. New York: Wiley. *Browne, B. A., & Cruse, D. F. (1988). The incubation effect: Illusion or Illumination. Human Performance, 1 (3), 177-185. Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55-81. Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121-152. *Christensen, B.T., & Schunn, C. D. (2005). Spontaneous access and analogical incubation 182 effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17 (2&3), 207-220. Chronicle, E.P., MacGregor, J.N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). What makes an insight problem? The roles of heuristics, goal conception and solution recoding in knowledge-lean problems Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 30, 14-27. Chronicle, E. P., Ormerod, T. C., & MacGregor, J. N. (2001). When insight just won't come: The failure of visual cues in the nine-dot problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 903-919. Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 12629-12633. Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A Critique of Language statistics in psychology research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335-359. Clark, H. T., & Roof, K. D. (1988). Field dependence and strategy use. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 303-307. Cochran, K. F., & Davis, J. K. (1987). Individual differences in inference processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 197-210. Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (1994). The Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Copeland, B.D., 1983. The relationship of cognitive style to academic achievement of university art appreciation students. College Student Journal, 17 ( 2), 157–162. Dodds, R. A., Smith, S. M., & Ward, T. B. (2002). The use of environmental clues during incubation. Creativity Research Journal, 14 (3/4), 287-305 183 *Dodds, R. A., Smith, S. M., & Ward, T. B. (2002). The use of environmental clues during incubation. Creativity Research Journal, 14 (3/4), 287-305 Dodds, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (in press). Incubation in problem solving and creativity. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity Research Handbook, Vol. 3. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. *Dominowski, R. L., & Jenrick, R. (1972). Effect of hints and interpolated activity on solution of an insight problem. Psychonomic Science, 26 (6), 335-338. *Dorfman, J, Shames, V. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Intuition, incubation, and insight: implicit cognition in problem solving. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Implicit Cognition (pp. 257–287). Oxford: Oxford University Press. *Dreistadt, R. (1969). The use of analogies and incubation in obtaining insights in creative problem solving. The Journal of Psychology, 71, 159-175. Durlak, J.A., & Lipsey, M.W. A practitioner's guide to meta-analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 291-332. Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273-305. Finke, R.A., Ward, T.B., & Smith, S.M. (1992). Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-development inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911. Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail, Jr. & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition, 3-33. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Frank, B. M., & Noble, J. P. (1985). Field independence-dependence and cognitive 184 restricting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47 (5), 1129-1135. *Frankel, A. (1990). The effect of incubation and eidetic imagery training on tasks of problem solving. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Fordham University. *Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. P. (1968). Short-term incubation in divergent production. American Journal of Psychology, 81, 241-246. *Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. (1972). A further investigation of short-term incubation. Acta Instituti Psychologici, 64-73, 67-70. *Gall, M., & Mendelsohn, G.A. (1967). Effects of facilitating techniques and subject experimenter interaction on creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 211-216. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., & DeSoto, M. C. (2004) Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 121–151. Ghiselin, B. (1985). The Creative Process. A Symposium. Berkeley: University of California Press. Gilhooly, K. J., & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non-insight problem. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, 279-302 *Goldman, W., Wolters, N., & Winograd, E. (1992). A demonstration of incubation in anagram problem solving. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 36-38. Goode, P. E., Goddard, P. H., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2002). Event-related potentials index cognitive style differences during a serial-order recall task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 43, 123-140. Gorski, N.A., & Laird, J.E. (2006) Experiments in Transfer Across Multiple Learning 185 Mechanisms. Proceedings of the ICML-06 Workshop on Structural Knowledge Transfer for Machine Learning. Pittsburgh, PA. *Hansberry, M. T. (1999). Fixation and incubation effects in problem solving. Unpulished doctoral dissertation. University of Hampshire Heyworth, R. M. (1989). Expert-novice differences in the solving of a basic problem in chemistry. Chinese University Education. Journal, 17 (1), 59-71 Hedges L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. Orlando: Academic Press. *Henley, R. J. (1999). Priming Incubated Problems. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Sussex. *Jamieson, B. A. (1999). Incubation and aging: The nature of processing underlying insight. Unpublished doctoral dissertation University of Georgia. Ji, L., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 943-955. Jones, G. (2003). Testing two cognitive theories of insight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29 (5), 1017-1027. Jung-Beeman, M., & Bowden, E. (2000). The right hemisphere maintains solution-related activation for yet-to-be solved insight problems . Memory & Cognition, 28, 1231-1241. Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R.W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,130 (2), 169-183. *Kaplan, C. (1990). Hatching a theory of incubation: Does putting a problem aside really help? If so, why? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie-Mellon University. 186 Kavakli, M. & Gero, J. S. (2002): Structure of Concurrent Cognitive actions: A Case Study on Novice & Expert Designers. Design Studies, 23 (1), 25-40 Kershaw, T.C., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30 (1), 3-13. Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Review, 29, 169-195. Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pachinger, T., & Ripper, B. (1997). Brain oscillations and human memory performance: EEG correlates in the upper alpha and theta bands. Neuroscience Letters, 238, 9-12 Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1534–1555. Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., & Raney, G. E. (2001). An eye movement study of insight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1000-1009. Kounios, J., Frymiare, J.L., Bowden, E.M., Fleck, J.I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T.B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17, 882-890. Larkin, J. H. (1983). The role of problem representation in physics. In A.L. Stevens, & D. Gentner (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 75-99). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate. Lesgold, A., Rubinson, H., Feltovich, P., Glaser, R., Klopfer, D., & Wang, Y. (1988). Expertise in a complex skill: diagnosing X-ray pictures. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. Farr (Eds.), The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbam. 187 Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Luk, S. C. (2002). The relationship between cognitive style and academic achievement. British Journal of Educational Technology, 29 (2), 137-147. Luo, J., NiKi, K. & Knoblich, G. (2006). Perceptual contributions to problem solving: Chunk decomposition of Chinese characters. Brain Research Bulletin, 70 (4-6), 430-443. Lynch, M. D., & Swink, E. (1967). Some effects of priming, incubation and creative aptitude on journalism performance. Journal of Communication, 17 (4), 372-382. Macaskill, P., Walter, S. D., & Irwing, L. (2001). A comparison of methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 20, 641-654. MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (2001). Information-processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 176-201. Martindale, C. 1995. Creativity and connectionism. In S.M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.) The Creative Cognition Approach (pp. 249-268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Science, 30, 381-399 *Medd, E., & Houtz, J, C. (2002). Educational Research Quarterly, 26 (2), 13-16. Metcalfe, J., & Weibe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and non-insight problem-solving. Memory and Cognition, 15, 238-246. *Mednick, M., Mednick, S., & Mednick, E. (1964). Incubation of creative performance and specific associative priming. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 84-88. Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 188 69, 220-232. Mill, G. A. (1956). The Magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. Miyake, A., Witzki, A., & Emerson, M. (2001). Field dependence-independence from a working memory perspective: A dual-task investigation of the Hidden Figures Test. Memory, 9, 455-457. *Moss, S. A. (2002). The Impact of Environmental Clues in Problem Solving and Incubation: The Moderating Effect of Ability. Creativity Research Journal, 14 (2), 207. Murphy, H. J., Casey, B., Day, D. A., & Young, J. D. (1997). Scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test by undergraduates in information management. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 1135-1138. *Murray, H. G., & Denny, J. P. (1969). Interaction of ability level and interpolated activity (opportunity for incubation) in human problem solving. Psychological Reports, 24 (1), 271-276. Newell, A. & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall Newton, E.J., & Roberts, M.J. (2005). The window of opportunity: A model for strategy discovery. In M.J. Roberts and E.J. Newton (Eds.), Methods of Thought: Individual Differences in Reasoning Strategies (pp. 129-158). Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press. Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently and why. New York: The Free Press. Ohlsson, S. (1992). Information-processing explanations of insight and related phenomena. 189 In M. T. Keane & K. J. Gihooly (Eds.), Advances in the psychology of thinking (pp. 1-44). Hemel Hempstead : Harvester Wheatsheaf Olton, R. M. (1979). Experimental studies of incubation: Searching for the elusive. Journal of Creative Behavior, 13, 9-22. *Olton, R. M., & Johnson D. M. (1976). Mechanism of incubation in creative problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 89 (4), 617-630. Ormerod, T. C., MacGregor, J. N. & Chronicle, E. P. (2002). Dynamics and Constraints in Insight Problem Solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28 (4), 791-799. Patalano, A. L., & Seifert. C. M. (1994). Memory for impasses during problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 22 (2), 234-242. *Patrick, A. S. (1986). The role of ability in creative "incubation". Personal and Individual Differences, 7 (2), 169-174. *Penney, C. G., Godsell, A. Scott, A., & Balsom, R. (2004). Problem variables that promote incubation effects. Journal of Creative Behavior, 38 (1), 35-55. Perkins, D. N. (1995). Insight in minds and genes. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight. (pp. 495-533). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. *Peterson, C. (1974). Incubation effects in anagram solution. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 3, 29-30. Posner, M.I. (1973). Cognition: An Introduction. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Rae, C. M. (1997). The creative power of doing nothing. Writer, 110 (7), 13-15. Reder, L. M., & Schunn, C. D. (1996). Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and memory. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 45–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 190 Rickards, T. (1991). Innovation and creativity: woods, trees and pathways. R & D Management, 21 (2), 97-108. Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 183-192. Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M., & Doppelmayr, M. (2005). Fronto-parietal coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 97-103. Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 166–183. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and Learning in Computers and People. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Schank, R. C. (1999). Dynamic Memory Revisited. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Scheerer, M. (1963). Problem solving. Scientific American, 208 (4), 118-128 *Seabrook, R., & Dienes, Z. (2003). Incubation in problem solving as a context effect. Proceedings of the 25th meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, July 31-Aug 2 , 2003. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. *Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 16 (1), 141-149. Seifert, M. C., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.). The Nature of Insight(pp. 65-124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: A microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 377-397. *Silveira, J. (1971). Incubation: The effect of interruption timing and length on problem 191 solution and quality of problem processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon. Simon, H. A. (1966). Scientific discovery and the psychology of problem solving. In R. G. Colodny (Ed.), Mind and Cosmos: Essays in Contemporary Science and Philosophy (pp. 22-40). University of Pittsburgh Press. *Sio, U. N. & Rudowicz, E. (2007). The Role of an incubation period in creative problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 19 (2-3), 307-318. Smilek, D., Enns, J. T., Eastwood, J. D., & Merikle, P. A. (2006). Relax! Cognitive style influences visual search. Visual Cognition, 14, 543-564. Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and creative thinking. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.). The Creative Cognition Approach (pp. 135-146). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. *Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27 (4), 311-314. *Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 61-87. *Snyder, A., Mitchell, D. J., Ellwood, S., Yates, A., & Palllier, G. (2004). Nonconscious idea generation. Psychological Reports, (94), 1325-1330 Thornton, A., & Lee, P. (2000). Publication bias in meta-analysis – its causes and consequences. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53 (2), 207-216. *Torrance-Perk, J. (1997). The incubation effect: Implication for underlying mechanism. Unpulished doctoral dissertation. University of Waterloo. Tsakanikos, E. (2006). Associative learning and perceptual style: Are associated events perceived analytically or as a whole? Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 192 579-586. VanLehn, K. (1988). Towards a theory of impasse-driven learning. In H. Mandl & A. Lesgold (Eds), Learning: Issues for Intelligence Tutor System (pp. 19-41). New York: Springer-Verlag. *Vul, E., & Pashler, H. (2007).Incubation is helpful only when people are misled. Memory, and Cognition, 35 (4), 701-710. Wallas, G. (1926). The Art of Thought. London: Jonathan Cape. Wang, Y. & Laird, J. E. (2007). The importance of action history in decision making in reinforcement learning. In proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling. Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA. Webster, A., Campbell, C., & Jane, B. (2006). Enhancing the creative process for learning in primary technology education. International Journal of Technology & Design Education, 16 (3), 221-235. Wertheimer, M. (1985). A Gestalt perspective on computer simulations of cognitive processes. Computers in Human Behavior, 1, 19–33. Witkin, M.A., Oltman, P., Raskin, E., Karp, S. (1971). A Manual for the Embedded Figures Test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental Psychology. New Delhi: Oxford & IBH. Xu, J., Wintermute, S., Wang, Y. J., & Laird, J. (2009). Transferring Learned Search Heuristics (Report No. CCA-TR-2009-04). Ann Arbor, MI: Center for Cognitive Architecture, University of Michigan. *Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E, (1987). Activation and meta-cognition of inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving. Journal of 193 Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13 (2), 187-205. Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsychologie: III. Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Handlungen [Investigations on the psychology of action and affection: III The memory of completed and uncompleted actions]. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1-85. Zeigarnik, B. (1938). On finished and unfinished tasks. In W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A sourcebook of Gestalt psychology (pp.300-314). New York: Humanities press. Zhong, C. B., Dijsterhuis, A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The Merits of Unconscious Thought in Creativity. Psychological Science, 19, 912-918. 194 Appendix A Insight Problems Used in Incubation Studies Task Type Description Solution/Sample Task Participant has to list all the uses he/she can think of / Creative Problems Brick Task for a brick. Consequence Task Participant has to list out as many consequences of an Sample Task: event as he/she can foresee What would be the results if everyone suddenly lost the ability to read and write? Creative Writing -- Sample Task: Write about three concrete objects: a 195 Koosch ball, a wooden type of propeller, and a triangular frisbee Visual Insight Problems Candle Problem Participant has to support a candle on wall by using Solution: some matches and a box of tacks. Use a tack to attach the box to the wall, then drip some wax onto the box and mount the candle on the box Farm Problem Participant has to divide an L-shaped farm into four parts which have the same size and shape. 196 Solution: Hat Rack Problem Participant has to construct a stable hatrack by using Solution: two boards and a Camp. Wedge the two boards between the ceiling and the floor, holding them in place with the clamp, and with the clamp also serving as a hook. Insightful mathematic Participant has to compute separately the area of the Solution problem square and that of the parallelogram shown below: Restructure the given shape into partially overlapping triangle ABG and ECD. The sum of their area is 2 x ab/2 = ab 197 Necklace problem Necklace Problem Solution: Participant is given four pieces of chain, and each chain Open all 3 links of one chain, and join the is made up of three links, he/she has to join all the other three chains together. pieces by only opening and closing three links. Radiation Problem A patient has an inoperable tumor in the middle of the Solution: body, and there is a ray at a strong intensity that can Direct multiple low-intensity rays destroy the tumor, but the ray also harms the healthy simultaneously toward the tumor from tissue that it travels through. At low intensities, the ray different directions will spare the healthy tissue, but will not destroy the tumor. Participant has to think out a way to use the ray to destroy the tumor without damaging healthy tissue. Saugstad’s “ball Problem” Participant has to transfer steel balls from a drinking Solution: glass to a cylinder from a distance of 8 ft by using the Bend the nail into a hook then attach it to 198 following objects: nail, a pair of pliers, a length of the string. Use it to drag back the frame string, a pulley, elastic bands, and newspaper. The glass and remove the balls. Transfer the balls sits on a moveable frame. into the cylinder by using a tube constructed of newspapers and elastic band. Tree Problem Participant has to plant 10 trees in five rows with four Solution: trees in each row (The trees are represented by the dots) 199 Linguistic Insight Problems Anagram Participant has to rearrange the scrambled letters to Sample Task: form a new word The scrambled letters are “t s l t i n e”, and one possible solution is “silent” RAT Three words are presented to the participant; he/she has Sample Task: to think of a word that can form associations with each The three stimulus words are “ blue, of them. cake, cottage”, and one possible solution is the word “cheese” Rebus Participant has to figure out the phrase portrayed by the Sample task: pictogram The pictogram 200 The answer is “first aid” Riddle -- Sample Task A wine bottle is half-filled and corked. How can you drink all of the wine without removing the cork from the bottle? The answer is: The cork can be opened by pushing it in. Word Associates Task Six words are presented to the participant, he/she has to Sample Task: think out a word that can form an association with each The six stimulus words are “school, chair, of them. jump, noon, heels, wire”, and one possible solution is the word “high” Word Fragment Participant has to complete a word that has various Sample task: Completion Task letters missing The stimulus is “OC_ _N”, and one 201 possible answer is “OCEAN” 202 Appendix B Settings of Incubation Studies Misleading cues Cues during Preparation Incubatio Year Author Problem embedded in Included in Incubation Task incubation period Other Factors Incubation Effect n period Meta-Analysis some period problems Mednick, M. T., Analogy task Mednick, S. A., 1964 RAT Mednick, E. Yes, in "Analogy Not No 1 min No, no control vs. Analogy Yes -- task +Cues" specified group task + Cues (Experiment 1) 203 condition Cues are either relevant, Mednick, M. T., Yes, high ability Analogy task Mednick, S. A., 1964 Not RAT No 1 min Mednick, E. No, no incorrect, or group in "Analogy information for vs. Analogy Yes irrelevant; specified task + Cues task + Cues" estimating effect condition size Participants' (Experiment 2) problem solving ability Non-verbal task Gall,M., & Yes, estimated vs. Free 1967 Mendelsohn, G. RAT No 2 min 0, 25 min Yes -- No from the associates task A. p-less-than value + Cues Yes, estimated from the Number-Series Fulgosi, A., Consequence 1968 0, 10, 20 No Guilford, J. P. Task 2 min Task ( 10 and min No Yes, in 20 min statement of condition significance, and -- 20 min) the p-less-than value 204 In control Farm Problem: Yes, condition, half Farm in "Guess card Guess Card vs. of the Problem & 1969 Dreistadt, R +Cues" condition. No 5 min 0, 8 min Guess Card + Yes participants Tree Planting Yes Tree Planting cues received cues Problem Problem: No during the whole incubation effect. experiments. Multiple choice logical Murry, H. G. & 1969 Participants' traced complex No problem solving Yes, in low ability No Denny J. P. syllogisms, and Saugstad's 5 min 0, 5 min ball problem Yes group. sequences of ability numbers and letters No, no Number-Series Fulgosi, A., Consequence 1972 0, 30 min, No Guilford, J. P. Task 2 min Task (30 min 60 min No Yes, in 30-min information for condition estimating effect -- and 60 min) size 205 Free associate task vs. One group of Anagram vs. participants Yes, estimated Dominowski, R., Hat Rack 1972 & Jenrick,R. from the No 5 min 0, 10 min Free associate Yes received cues No Problem statement of ( Experiment 1) task +Cues vs. during the Anagram + whole experiment significance Cues Free associate One group of task vs. participants Anagramvs. received cues Yes, estimated Dominowski, R., Hat Rack 1972 & Jenrick,R. from the No 3 min 0, 3 min Free associate No during the whole No Problem ( Experiment 2) statement of task +Cues vs. experiment .Parti Anagram + cipants' problem Cues solving ability significance 206 Read book (30 Yes, in long min) vs. Read Silveira, J. M. Necklace 1972 3 min vs. 13 0, 30, No (Experiment 1 ) Problem preparation (13 book + Free min No -- 210 min Yes min) and incubation Activity (210 (210 min)group min) Silveira, J. M. Necklace 1972 Read book + No (Experiment 2) Problem Silveira, J. M. Consequence 1972 1974 Peterson, C. 210 min -- Yes Yes No -- No Yes Task Difficulty Yes Yes -- No Read book + 2 min 210 min Task Anagram No Free Activity No (Experiment 3) 13 min Free Activity No 20 sec 3.6 mins Other Anagram No Listen music vs. Solve 1975 Bennett, S. M. RAT No 1 min 10 min No, no control No mathematics problems 207 group Rest vs. Stroop test + backward Counting vs. Review the Olton, R, M., & Farm Johnson, D. M. Problem 1976 0, 15 No 10 min problem vs. Yes -- No Yes min Have lecture on creative problem solving vs. Listen Music Relax (20 min Verbal Yes, the longer the and 30 min) vs. Divergent 1979 Beck, J. 0, 20, 30 No 12 min Thinking incubation the Writing Essay No -- min Yes stronger the (20 min and Task benefits 30 min) Brick and 1985 Brockett, C. A. No 10 min 0, 20 min Questionnaires No RAT 208 -- Yes Yes Yes, estimated Conversation 1986 Patrick, A. S. RAT No 2 min 0, 5 min vs. Mental Yes, in No -- rotation task "Mental from the rotation task" p-less-than value, Condition and the statement of significance No, did not Rare-word Yes, participants Yaniv, I, & 1987 Definition Not Not specified Specified No Meyer, D. E. Questionnaire No -- Task measure the were more sensitive post-incubation to relevant concepts problem solving performance Listen music, Graph drawing, Browne, B. A . & Farm 1988 Donna F. Cruse Memorizing a No Problem 20 min 0, 5 min No portion of an (Experiment 1) oceanorgaphy text 209 -- No Yes Listen music, Graph drawing, Browne, B. A . & Farm 1988 Donna F. Cruse 20 min or 25 No Problem Memorize a 0, 5 min min No -- No Yes portion of an (Experiment 2) oceanorgaphy text Yes, the performance of 15 Rest( 5 min and Yes, min incubation > 5 15 min) vs. min incubation and Smith, S. M. & Useful cues were Misleading 1989 Blankenship, S. Rebus 0, 5, 15 Music 0.5 min or Useful control group. No min presented in the Perception Task E. (Experiment 1) Yes Larger forgetting second attempt cues ( 5 min and 15 effect in 15 min min) incubation group. No main effect of the filler task 210 Yes, but no difference between Yes, Rest( 5 min and the 5 min and the 15 min) vs. 15 min conditions. Smith, S. M. & Misleading 1989 Blankenship, S. Rebus 0, 5, 15 Music 0.5 min or Useful Larger forgetting on No min -- Yes Perception Task misleading cues ( 5 min and 15 in the 15 min min) group. No main E. (Experiment 2) cues effect of the filler task 211 Rebus (10 min) vs. Music perception task+rebus (15 Yes, Yes, but no Smith, S. M. & min) vs. Misleading 1989 Blankenship, S. Rebus Yes, estimated 0, 10, 15 0.5 min or Useful difference among Mathematics No -- min E. (Experiment 3) from the different incubation Task +Rebus p-less-than value cues conditions (15 min) vs. Rest+ Rebus (15min) vs. Rebus (15 min) Yes, estimated Yes, Read story ( 5 Misleading min ) vs. Smith, S. M. & 1989 Blankenship, S. from the Rebus 0.5 min 0, 5 min Yes, in "Read No or Useful Mathematic cues tasks ( 5 min) E. (Experiment 4) -- p-less-than value story" condition and the statement of significance 212 Other word Yes, in both Dorfman, J. Word Puzzle (Experiment 3) Task 1990 No 0, 5, 15 problem vs. min other word 0.49 min No -- incubation Yes conditions problem + Cues Number series Yes, estimated task , and cues Dorfman J Word Puzzle 1990 0, 3, 8, 13 No (Experiment 4) 0.49 min Task from the presented in No, not significant delay statement of sub-sequence significance phase Kaplan, C. A. Consequence 1990 Psychometric No (Experiment 1) Task Kaplan, C. A. Consequence 1990 Task Kaplan, C. A. Consequence (Experiment 3) Task 0, 30 min No -- Yes Yes No -- Yes Yes, test battery Division task No (Experiment 2) 2 min 2 min 0, 32 min +lecturre Yes, estimated 1990 No 2 min 0, 30 min Lecture No -- No from statement of significance 213 Yes , solve a list of problem in randomized order Division Kaplan, C. A. Consequence 1990 ( problem + No (Experiment 4) 2 min 40 min problem and No -- Task Yes incubation task) t = insight problem 1.82, p < 0.05 (10, 10, number of participants) Yes, when solving problems with Smith, S. M. & Participants' Read science 1991 Blankenship, S. E RAT Yes 0.5 min 0, 5 min misleading cues and No problem solving fiction Yes greater effect for (Experiment 1). ability low ability participants Smith, S. M. & Participants' Read 1991 Blankenship, S. RAT Yes 1 min 0, 5 min Yes, when solving science No problem solving problem with fiction E. (Experiment 2) ability 214 misleading cues Yes Yes, when solving Yes, estimated problem with Smith, S. M. & Free association Participants' 0, 0.5, 2 1991 Blankenship, S. RAT Yes 0.5 min tasks ( 0.5 min No problem solving min E. (Experiment 5) from p-less-than misleading cues, value and smaller effect for and 2 min) ability statement of low ability significance participants General knowledge questionnaire Goldman, W. P., (20 min) vs. Wolters, N. C. 1992 0, 20, Anagram No 40 min W., & Winograd, Yes, in "1440 min" General No -- 1440 min Yes, condition. knowledge E. questionnaire + free activity (1440 min) Houtz, J. C. & Life-relevant Franke;, A. D. problem 1992 No 10 min 10 min Anagram task 215 No -- Yes Yes Word Fragment Rest +lexical Completion, decision task Torrance-Perks, Candle 1997 Yes, 25 min 0, 10 min vs. Rest + J. (Experiment 1) Problem, lexical decsion Radiation task + cues Problem, RAT 216 Yes -- No Yes Candle Problem: Memory test vs. Candle Memory test + Problem, Yes, Radiation Misleading Problem, cues cues; Radiation Torrance-Perks, 1997 1 min 0, 8 min Problem: Read Yes J. (Experiment 2) RAT story vs. Read story +Cues; RAT: Analogy vs. Analogy + cues 217 -- No Yes Two-String Problem : Analogy + Two-String Cues; Hatrack Problem, Yes, Hatrack Misleading Torrance-Perks, 1997 Problem: Paired 3 min 0, 8 min J. (Experiment 3) Yes -- Yes Yes No -- No Yes problem solving No Yes associated task Problem, cues + Cues ; Radiation Radiation Problem: Read Story + Cues Hansberry, M. T. 1998 Riddles Yes, 1 min 0, 15 min RAT (Experiment 2) Participants' Hansberry, M. T. 1998 RAT Yes, 0.5 min 0, 10 min RAT No (Experiment 3) ability Yes, estimated Henley, R. J. 1999 0, 1440 Anagram (Experiment 3.2) No 0.25 min Free Activity No -- No from statement of min significance 218 Yes, estimated Henley, R. J. 1999 0, 1440 Anagram No 0.93 min (Experiment 4) Free Activity No -- No from statement of min significance Jamieson, B. A. 1999 Mathematics RAT Yes 0.33 min 0, 5 min (Experiment 1) Jamieson, B. A. 1999 No -- No Yes No -- No Yes vs. Insight Nature of the Yes, only when problem + cues ( answer, answer or unrelated relevant word was presented test vs. Insight information or during the problem + related word) incubation period problem Mathematics RAT Yes 0.33 min 0, 5 min (Experiment 2) problem Insight problem + Drawing task Dodds , R., Smith, S. M., & 2002 No, no control RAT No 10 min 15 min Make a word Yes Ward, T. B. group (Experiment 1) Make a word test + Cues 219 Insight problem + Drawing task vs. Insight Nature of the Yes, in informed problem + cues ( answer, group and only relevant when answer was vs. information or presented during Insight problem related word) incubation Dodds , R., Smith, S. M., & 2002 RAT No 10 min 0, 15 min Make a word Yes Ward, T. B. test (Experiment 2) + Make a word test + Cues 220 Yes Half of the participants were Yes, if the problem told that they solvers were Unrelated would turn back working related writing task vs. to the initial writing task during Yes, estimated from the Medd., E & Creative Houtz, J. Writing 2002 Yes, 10 min 0, 10 min No p-less-than value Related Writing assignment later, incubation period task and prompted to and knew that they and statement of significance think about it had to get back to during the the problem later incubation period 221 Yes. High ability Verbal Tasks were participants: solve described as more solvable solvable or RATs after unsolvable. incubation. Low Participants’ ability participants: problem solve solving ability unsolvable RATs No, no control 2002 Moss, S. A. RAT No 0.5 min 0, 15 min reasoning task Yes group +Cues more after incubation Nature of the Seabrook, R., & 2003 Word Anagram No 0.25 min 7 min Dienes, Z Yes, estimated Yes, in Yes cues ( Irrelevant generation tasks from p-less-than Relevant-Cue group or Revelant) value Searching a Both, L., specific letter 2004 Needham, D., & Anagram No 1.67 min 0, 6 min Yes, in Incubation No (A to Z) and Wood, E. Questionnaire 222 -- Yes group Searching a specific letter Both, L., (A to Z) , 2004 Needham, D., & Anagram Yes 1.67 0, 6 min Yes, in Incubation Yes -- Questionnaire Yes group Wood, E and backward digit task Word Penny, C. G., completion task Godsell, A., + Cues (15 Yes, in 15 min 15 , 180 2004 Scott, A., & Anagram No 5.75 min condition, filler min, 180 min) min No No, no control -tasks did not make group Balsom, R. vs. Inactivity (Experiment 1) ( 15 a different min) 223 min, 180 Word Penny, C. G., completion task Yes, in 15 min Godsell, A., 15, 1440 + Cues (15 min, 2004 Scott, A., & Anagram No 5.75 min condition, filler No min No, no control -- 1440 min) vs. tasks did not make group Inactivity (15 a different Balsom, R. (Experiment 2) min, 1440 min) Word Yes, but no completion task Penny, C. G., difference between +Cues (30 min, Godsell, A., 2004 0, 30, 120 Anagram No 5.75 min Scott, A., & the short and long 120 min) vs. No -- min Yes incubation period. Inactivity Balsom, R. Filler tasks did not ( 30min, 120 make a different min) Read paper ( 4 Yes, in word puzzle Insightful min, 12 min) 0, 4 , 12 2004 Segal, E. Mathematic No 20 min condition, and vs. Word min Puzzle No -- Yes larger effect in 4 puzzle ( 4 min, min condition 12 min) 224 Snyder, A., Mitchell, J., Divergent Ellwood, S., & thinking task 2004 No 5 min 5 min Conversation No -- Unknown No Yates A. No, preparation Nature of the Christensen, B. Analogous or Insight 2005 T. & Schunn, C. Not specified specified No Puzzle D. Not and incubation cues ( analogous Yes, in analogous distracter rating Yes periods cue or distracter task cue condition unspecified and cue) no control group 225 The RATs were Yes, when solving either problem with Chess-Related, Mental misleading cues, Neutral, or Rotation Task No, did not experts were more Chess-Unrelated Sio, U. N & 2007 + Mathematics RAT Yes 1 min 0, 2 min Rudowicz, E measure the sensitive to relevant No containing Task vs. post-incubation concepts after an misleading chess Listening problem solving incubation period cues. The Music performance filled with mental participants were rotation and either experts or mathematics task novices in chess Vul, E. & 2007 Anagram No 12 min 0, 5 min Video game No Task difficulty RAT Yes 12 0, 5 min Video game No Task difficulty No Yes Pashler,H. Vul, E. & 2007 Yes, in the fixation min Pashler,H. Yes group 226 Appendix C Information Extracted from Each Independent Study Included in the Meta-Analysis Misleading ID Year Author Total N Nature of the Problem Unbiased Preparation Incubation Incubation Task Cues Cues Effect Size 2 (Non-Verbal 1 1967 Gall, M. & Mendelsohn, G. 60 2 (RAT) 0 2 25 0 -.58 1 -.58 0 0 0 .52 0 .34 1 .99 Task) 2 (Associations 2 1967 Gall, M. & Mendelsohn, G. 60 2 (RAT) 0 2 25 Training) Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. 3 1967 2 (Number series 50 1 (Consequence Task) 0 2 10 P. task) Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. 4 1967 2 (Number series 49 1 (Consequence Task) 0 2 20 P. task) 1 (Guess playing 5 1969 Dreistadt R. (Study 1) 20 1 (Farm Problem) 0 5 8 card) 1 (Guess playing 6 1969 Dreistadt R. (Study 2) 20 1 (Farm Problem) 0 5 8 card) 227 1 (Guess playing 7 1969 Dreistadt R. (Study 3) 20 1 (Tree Problem) 0 5 8 0 .21 1 .48 0 .62 0 -.59 0 .11 card) 1 (Guess playing 8 1969 Dreistadt R. (Study 4) 20 1 (Tree Problem) 0 5 8 card) 2 (Multiple choice logical syllogisms, 1 (Saugstad's ball Murry, H. G. & Denny J. P. 9 1969 and traced 36 problem, low ability 0 5 5 (Study 1) sequences of group) numbers and letters) 2 (Multiple choice logical syllogisms, 1 (Saugstad's ball Murry, H. G. & Denny J. P. 10 1969 and traced 36 problem, high ability 0 5 5 (Study 2) sequences of group) numbers and letters) 11 1971 Silveria, J. M. (Study 1) 18 1 (Necklace Problem) 0 228 3 30 1 (Read book) 1 (Read book for .06 12 1971 Silveria, J. M. (Study 2) 18 1 (Necklace Problem) 0 3 210 30 mins and free 0 activity 3 hrs) 13 1971 Silveria, J. M. (Study 3) 18 1 (Necklace Problem) 0 13 30 1 (Read book) 0 .17 0 .42 0 .44 0 0 0 0 1 (Read book for 14 1971 Silveria, J. M. (Study 4) 18 1 (Necklace Problem) 0 13 210 30 mins and free activity 3 hrs) 1 (Read book for 15 1971 Silveria, J. M. (Study 5) 32 1 (Necklace Problem) 0 13 210 30 mins and free activity 3 hrs) Dominowski, R. L., & 16 1972 2 (Free 27 1 (Hat Rack Problem_ 5 10 Jenrick, R. Association) Dominowski, R. L., & 17 1972 2 (Free 30 1 (Hat Rack Problem) 3 3 Jenrick, R. 18 1974 19 1976 Peterson, C. Association) 24 1 (Anagram) 21 1 (Farm Problem) .33 1.8 Anagram 1 .65 10 15 0 (Rest) 0 .10 Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 0 D. M. (Study 1) 229 2 (Stroop test + Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 20 1976 21 1 (Farm Problem) 0 10 15 Counting 0 .11 1 .10 D. M. (Study 1) backward) Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 21 1976 2 ( Review the 21 1 (Farm Problem) 0 10 15 D. M (Study 1) problem) Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 22 1976 21 1 (Farm Problem) 0 10 15 2 ( Have lecture) 1 0 20 1 (Farm Problem) 0 10 15 0 (Listen music) 0 -.10 20 1 (Farm Problem) 0 10 15 0 (Rest) 2 .10 20 1 (Farm Problem) 0 10 15 2 (Having lecture) 2 -.03 0 12 20 0 (Relax) 0 2.19 0 12 30 2 (write essay) 0 1.07 D. M. (Study 1) Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 23 1976 D. M. (Study 1) Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 24 1976 D. M. (Study 1) Olton, R. M., & Johnson, 25 1976 D. M. (Study 1) 0 (verbal divergent 26 1979 Beck, J. (Study 1) 60 thinking task) 0 (verbal divergent 27 1979 Beck, J. (Study 2) 60 thinking task) 230 0 (verbal divergent 28 1979 Beck, J. (Study 3) 60 0 12 20 0 (Relax) 0 4.07 0 12 30 2 (Write essay) 0 4.04 0 10 20 2 (Questionnaire) 0 .41 0 .33 20 2 (Questionnaire) 0 .36 0 2 5 2 (Conversation) 0 0 0 2 5 0 .66 0 .47 thinking task) 0 (verbal divergent 29 1979 Beck, J. (Study 4) 60 thinking task) 30 1985 Brockett, C. A. (Study 1) 30 31 1985 Brockett, C. A. (Study 2) 30 0 (Brick) 2 ( Remote Associates Task) 2 (Remote Associate 32 1986 Patrick, A. S. 30 Tasks) 2 (Remote Associate 33 1986 Patrick, A. S. 30 2 (Mental Tasks) Rotation Task) Browne, B. A . & Cruse, D. 34 1988 0 (Listening 60 1 (Farm Problem) 0 20 5 F. (Study 1) music) Browne, B. A . & Cruse, D. 35 1988 53 1 (Farm Problem) 0 20 5 1 (Graph drawing) 2 .24 55 1 (Farm Problem) 0 20 5 2 (Memory test) 0 -.17 F. (Study 1) Browne, B. A . & Donna F. 36 1988 Cruse (Study 3) 231 Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 37 1989 26 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 5 26 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 5 0 (Rest) 0 .45 0 .48 0 .49 0 .50 1 .05 1 .05 1 .41 1 .41 1 .56 (Study 1) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 38 1989 2 (Music (Study 1) Perception Task) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 39 1989 26 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 26 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (Rest) (Study 1) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 40 1989 2 (Music (Study 1) Perception Task) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 41 1989 25 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 5 25 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 5 0 (Rest) (Study 2) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 42 1989 2 (Music (Study 2) Perception Task) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 43 1989 25 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 25 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (Rest) (Study 2) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 44 1989 2 (Music (Study 2) Perception Task) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 45 1989 29 2 (Rebus) 1 (Study 3) 232 0.5 10 2 (Rebus) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 46 1989 29 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (Rebus +Rest) 1 .56 29 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 2 (Rebus+Maths) 1 .56 29 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (Rebus+Music) 1 .56 29 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 15 2 (Rebus) 1 .56 49 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 5 1 (Read Story) 0 0 49 2 (Rebus) 1 0.5 5 2 (Maths Task) 0 .49 0 .49 15 0 0 0 1.07 0 0 (Study 3) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 47 1989 (Study 3) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 48 1989 (Study 3) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 49 1989 (Study 3) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 50 1989 (Study 4) Smith, S. M. & Steven, B. 51 1989 (Study 4) 2 ( Word associate 52 1990 Dorfman, J. (Study 1) 15 2 (Number Series task) Task) 2 ( Word associate 53 1990 Dorfman, J. (Study 1) 15 2 (Number Series 0 .49 5 task) Task) 2 ( Word associate 54 1990 Dorfman, J. (Study 2) 27 2 (Number Series 0 task) .49 3 Task) 233 2 ( Word associate 55 1990 Dorfman, J. (Study 2) 27 2 (Number Series 0 .49 8 task) 2 ( Word associate 56 1990 Dorfman, J. (Study 2) 26 0 0 0 0 0 .06 0 .71 0 0 0 1.08 0 1.70 0 .64 Task) 2 (Number Series 0 .49 13 task) Task) 2 (Psychometric 57 1993 Kaplan, C. A. (Study 1) 278 0 (Consequence test) 0 2 30 test battery) 2 (Division 58 1993 Kaplan, C. A. (Study 2) 64 0 (Consequence test) 0 2 2 problem) 59 1993 Kaplan, C. A. (Study 3) 36 0 (Consequence test) 0 2 30 60 1993 Kaplan, C. A. (Study 3) 20 0 (Consequence test) 0 4.57 28.08 2 (Lecture) 2 (Division and insight problem) Smith, S. M. & 2 (Remote Associates 61 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 18 1 (Read science 1 .5 5 Test ) fiction) 1) Smith, S. M. & 2 (Remote Associates 62 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 21 1 (Read science 0 Test ) .5 5 fiction) 1) 234 Smith, S. M. & 63 1991 2 (Remote Associates 30 Blankenship, S. E. (Study2 1 (Read science 0 1 5 Test ) 0 .37 0 .74 0 0 0 0 0 .99 0 .99 fiction) Smith, S. M. & 2 (Remote Associates 64 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 30 1 (Read science 1 1 5 Test ) fiction) 2) Smith, S. M. & 2 ( Free 2 (Remote Associates 65 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 16 0 .5 2 Associations Test ) 5) Task) Smith, S. M. & 2 ( Free 2 (Remote Associates 66 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 18 0 .5 .5 Associations Test ) 5) Task) Smith, S. M. & 2 ( Free 2 (Remote Associates 67 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 17 1 .5 .5 Associations Test ) 5) Task) Smith, S. M. & 2 ( Free 2 (Remote Associates 68 1991 Blankenship, S. E. (Study 18 1 .5 2 Associations Test ) 5) Task) 235 Goldman, W. P., 69 Wolters, 1992 N. C. W., & Winograd, E. 2 ( General 36 2 (Anagram) 0 .25 20 (Study 1) Knowledge 0 .13 0 .66 0 .03 0 -.38 1 -.41 0 .14 1 .07 Questionnaire) 2 ( General Goldman, W. P., 70 Wolters, 1992 N. C. W., & Winograd, E. Knowledge 36 2 (Anagram) 0 .25 1140 (Study 1) Questionnaire + Free Associations Task) 71 1992 Houtz, J.C. & Frankel 72 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 1) 105 1 (Creative Writing) 0 10 10 0 0.5 10 2 (Word fragment 15 0 (Rest+Lexical completion) decision test) 2 (Word fragment 73 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 2) 15 0 (Rest+Lexical 0 0.5 10 completion task) decision test) 2 (Word fragment 74 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 3) 15 0 (Rest+Lexical 1 0.5 10 completion) decision test) 2 (Word fragment 75 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 4) 15 1 (Anagram) 0 (Rest+Lexical 1 completion) 0.5 10 decision test) 236 2 (Memory test: cues presented as 76 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 5) 8 1 (Candle Problem) 0 1 8 2 .17 0 -.71 1 .41 0 .00 2 .15 0 .14 one of the stimulus) 77 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 6) 8 1 (Candle Problem) 0 1 8 2 (Memory test ) 1 (Read story : 78 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 7) 8 1 (Radiation Problem) 0 5 8 analogy to the problem) 1 (Read unrelated 79 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 8) 8 1 (Radiation Problem) 0 5 8 story) 2 (Analogy : have 2 (Remote Associates 80 1997 Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 9) 8 the same solution 0 1 8 Tasks) as the unsolved RAT) Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 81 1997 2 (Remote Associates 7 10) 2 (Neutral 0 Tasks) 1 8 analogy) 237 2 (Memory Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 82 1997 test :cue presented 7 1 (Candle Problem) 1 1 8 11) 2 .56 0 -.33 1 .52 0 .00 2 .17 0 .28 a one of the stimulus) Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 83 1997 7 1 (Candle Problem) 1 1 8 2 (Memory test ) 12) 1 (Read Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 84 1997 7 1 (Radiation Problem) 1 5 8 story :analogy to 13) the problem) Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 85 1997 1 (Read unrelated 7 1 (Radiation Problem) 1 5 8 14) story) 2 (Analogy: have Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 86 1997 2 (Remote Associates 7 15) the same solution 1 1 8 Tasks) as the unsolved RAT) Torrance-Perks, J. (Study 87 1997 2 (Remote Associates 7 16) 2 (Neutral 1 Task) 1 8 analogy) 238 2 (Remote 88 1998 Hansberry, M. T. (Study 1) 32 2 (Riddle) 0 1 15 0 .66 0 .04 0 .28 associates task) 2 (Remote Associates 89 1998 Hansberry, M. T. (Study 2) 20 2 (Remote 0 .5 10 Task) associates task) 2 (Remote Associates 90 1998 Hansberry, M. T. (Study 2) 20 2 (Remote 1 .5 10 Task) associates task) 2 ( Remote Associates 91 1999 Jamieson, B. A. (Study 1) 52 0 .33 5 2 (Maths problem) 0 .00 0 .33 5 2 (Maths problem) 0 .10 Task) 2 ( Remote Associates 92 1999 Jamieson, B. A. (Study 2) 52 Task) 93 1999 Henley, R. J. (Study 3.2)* 48 2 ( Anagram) 0 .25 1440 1 (Free activity) 0 0 94 1999 26 2 ( Anagram) 0 .93 1440 1 (Free activity) 0 0 2 .31 1 -.14 Henley, R. J. (Study 4)* 2 (Insight Problem Dodds , R., Smith, S. M., & 95 2002 2 (Remote Associates 45 Ward, T. B. (Study 1) 0 10 .5 + Make a word Test ) task) 2 (Insight Problem Dodds , R., Smith, S. M., & 96 2002 2 (Remote Associates 45 Ward, T. B. (Study 2) 0 10 .5 + Make a word Test ) task) 239 2 (Insight Problem Dodds , R., Smith, S. M., & 97 2002 2 (Remote Associates 42 Ward, T. B. (Study 3) 0 10 .5 + Make a word 0 .10 0 .00 Test ) task) Dodds , R., Smith, S. M., & 98 2002 2 (Remote Associates 70 Ward, T. B. (Study 4) 99 2 (Insight Problem 0 10 .5 Test ) + Drawing test) 2002 Medd, E., Houtz, J. C. 15 1 ( Creative Writing) 0 10 10 2 (Writing) 1 1.05 100 2002 Medd, E., Houtz, J. C. 15 1 (Creative Writing) 0 10 10 2 (Writing) 0 0 60 2 (Anagram) 0 6 7 1 .74 2 ( Word 101 2003 Seabrook, R., & Dienes, Z* generation) 2 (Search letter Both, L., Needham, D., & 102 2004 .48 98 2 (Anagram) 0 1.67 6 and Answer 0 Wood, E. questionnaire) 2 (Search letter Both, L., Needham, D., & 103 2004 82 2 (Anagram) 0 1.67 6 and Answer 0 .09 1 .74 Wood, E. questionnaire) Penney, C., Godsell, A., 104 2004 9 2 (Anagram) 0 Scott, A., & Balsom, R. 240 5.75 30 1 ( Free Activity) Penney, C., Godsell, A., 105 2004 9 2 (Anagram) 0 5.75 120 1 ( Free Activity) 1 1.05 9 2 (Anagram) 0 5.75 30 1 ( Free Activity) 0 .62 9 2 (Anagram) 0 5.75 120 1 ( Free Activity) 0 .70 0 20 4 1 (Read paper) 0 1.17 0 20 12 1 (Read paper) 0 1.09 0 20 4 2 (Word puzzle) 0 .90 0 20 12 2 (Word puzzle) 0 .57 0 1 5 2 (Video game) 0 .86 1 1 5 2 (Video game) 0 -.14 Scott, A., & Balsom, R Penney, C., Godsell, A., 106 2004 Scott, A., & Balsom, R Penney, C., Godsell, A., 107 2004 Scott, A., & Balsom, R 1 ( Insightful 108 2004 Segal, E. (Study 1) 20 mathematic problem) 1 ( Insightful 109 2004 Segal, E. (Study 2) 21 mathematic problem) 1 ( Insightful 110 2004 Segal, E. (Study 3) 20 mathematic problem) 1 ( Insightful 111 2004 Segal, E. (Study 4) 23 mathematic problem) Vul, E. & Pashler,H. (Study 112 2007 2 (Remote Associates 25 1) Test ) Vul, E. & Pashler,H. (Study 113 2007 2 (Remote Associates 25 2) Test ) 241 Vul, E. & Pashler,H. (Study 114 2007 14 2 (Anagram) 0 1 5 2 (Video game) 0 -.17 14 2 (Anagram) 0 1 5 2 (Video game) 0 -.35 14 2 (Anagram) 0 1 5 2 (Video game) 0 .36 14 2 (Anagram) 0 1 5 2 (Video game) 0 .25 3) Vul, E. & Pashler,H. (Study 115 2007 4) Vul, E. & Pashler,H. (Study 116 2007 5) Vul, E. & Pashler,H. (Study 117 2007 6) *Study with a within-subjects design: Individual was in both incubation and control conditions, the weighting formula would not apply to it, hence, it would be excluded from the regression analyses. 242 Appendix D Description of the selection of the dummy coded vectors and the multiplicative terms in the regression model on all studies (with interaction terms, N = 114) The regression model describing the interaction effect of Nature of Problem and Incubation Task on the weighted unbiased effect size estimate, included the multiplicative terms between Nature of Problem (In, Li, Di) and Incubation Task (H, L, I) as predictors. The regression model is specified as follows: Yj = ajXj + b1LijHj + b2LijLj + b3Lij Rj + b4VjHj + b5VjLj + b6VjRj + b7CjHj + b8 Cj Lj + b9 CjRj + kj where Yj is the weighted unbiased effect size estimation of the study j, and Xj is a vector of other categorical and explanatory variables (Misleading Cues, Cue, and Preparation Period, Incubation Period) of that study. aj is the corresponding vector of coefficients. b1j to b9j are the coefficients of the multiplicative terms, and kj is the error term. The dummies I j and Di are eliminated through substituting equations Li j+In j+Di j = 1 and I j +L j +Hj = 1 into the model. The transformed regression model is as follows: Yj = ajXj + (b3-b9)Lij+ (b6-b9)Vj + (b7-b9)Hj + (b8-b9)Lj + (b1-b7-b3+b9)LijHj + (b2-b8-b3+b9) LijLj + (b4-b7-b6+b9) InjHj + (b5-b8-b6+b9)VjLj. Regression analysis was carried out to find out the coefficient of the each variable in the transformed model. To examine the interaction effect of Nature of the Problem and Incubation Task, the difference between coefficients’ multiplicative terms in the 243 original model was compared. For example, the coefficient of the compound dummies VjRj and CjRj was compared to check if an incubation period filled with low cognitive tasks would improve the performance on Visual problems more than on divergent thinking tasks. A comparison between the original and the transformed model indicated that the coefficient of each variable in the transformed model was actually the combination of the coefficients in the original model. The coefficients’ difference between the compound dummies in the original model could be found by re-interpreting the coefficient in the transformed model. Table D1 represents a list of coefficient differences between compound dummies in the original model and the equivalent combination of the coefficients in the transformed model 244 Table D143 Regression Coefficients in Original and Transformed Regression Models The coefficient difference between The combination of coefficients compound dummies in the original in the regression analysis regression model LijHj – Lij Rj LijHj + Hj LijL j- Lij Rj LijLj + Lj VjHj- Vj Rj VjHj + Hj VjLj - Vj Rj VjLj + Lj CjHj - Cj Rj Hj CjLj - Cj Rj Lj VjHj - CjHj VjHj + Vj LijHj - CjHj LijHj + Lij VjL j- CjLj VjLj +Vj LijLj- CjLj LijL j+ Lij Vj Rj - Cj Rj Vj Lij Rj - Cj Rj Lij 245 Appendix E Description of the selection of the dummy coded vectors and the multiplicative terms in the regression model for studies excluding creative problem (N= 100) The third regression model describing the interaction effect of Nature of Problem and Incubation Task on the weighted unbiased effect size estimate, included the multiplicative terms between Nature of Problem (V, Li) and Incubation Task (H, L, R) as predictors. The regression model is specified as follows: Yj = ajXj + b1LijHj + b2LijLj + b3Lij Rj + b4VjHj + b5VjLj + b6Vj Rj + kj where Yj is the weighted unbiased effect size estimation of the study j, and Xj is a vector of other categorical and explanatory variables (Misleading Cues, Cue, and Preparation Period, Incubation Period) of that study. aj is the corresponding vector of coefficients. b1j to b9j are the coefficients of the multiplicative terms, and kj is the error term. The dummy Li and I j were eliminated through substituting equations Li j+V j = 1 and R j +L j +Hj = 1 into the model. The transformed regression model is as follows: Yj = ajXj + (b6-b3)Vj + (b1-b3)Hj + (b2-b3)Lj + (b3-b1-b6+b4) VjHj + (b5-b2-b6+b3)VjLj. Regression analysis was carried out to find out the coefficient of the each variable in the transformed model. To examine the interaction effect of Nature of the Problem and Incubation Task, the difference between coefficients’ multiplicative terms in the original model was compared. For example, the coefficient of the compound dummies Vj Rj and Lij Rj was compared to check if an incubation period 246 filled with low cognitive tasks would improve the performance on Visual problems more than on linguistic insight tasks. A comparison between the original and the transformed model indicated that the coefficient of each variable in the transformed model was actually the combination of the coefficients in the original model. The coefficients’ difference between the compound dummies in the original model could be found by re-interpreting the coefficient in the transformed model. Table E1 represents a list of coefficient differences between compound dummies in the original model and the equivalent combination of the coefficients in the transformed model. Table E144 Regression Coefficients in Original and Transformed Regression Models The coefficient difference between The combination of coefficients compound dummies in the original in the regression analysis regression model LijHj - Lij Rj Hj LijL j- Lij Rj Lj LijL j- LijHj Lj - Hj V jHj- Vj Rj VjHj + Hj VjLj - Vj Rj VjLj + Lj VjLj - VjHj VjLj + Lj -VjHj - Hj LijHj - VjHj VjHj + Vj LijLj - VjLj VjLj + Vj Lij Rj - Inj Rj Vj 247 Appendix F RATs used in Experiment I Answer Solution Rate* Over, Plant, Horse power 10% Wise, Work, Tower clock 13% Tooth, Potato, Heart sweet 28% Spoon, Cloth, Card Table 26% Stick, Maker, Point Match 21% Home, Arm, Room rest 21% battle 18% fair 10% Fence, Card, Master Post 13% Pea, Shell, Chest Nut 23% Rope, Truck, Line tow 21% Office, Mail, Hat Fall 41% Reading, Service, Stick lip 10% Cut, Cream, War cold 31% Rain, Test, Stomach acid 31% Off, Military, First base 31% Set A Cry, Front, Ship Way, Ground, Weather Set B *Solution rates reported in Bowden and Jung-Beeman’s study (2003) 248 Appendix G RATs used in Experiment II Stimulus Answer Solution Rate* Soap 74% Cottage, Swiss, Cake Cheese 64% Glass, Rush, Happy Hour 72% Safety, Cushion, Point Pin 66% Polish, Finger, Down Nail 76% Fur, Rack, Tail Coat 79% River, Note, Account Bank 79% Note, Chain, Master Key 26% Spoon, Cloth, Card Table 26% Rain, Test, Stomach Acid 31% Guy, Rain, Down Fall 41% Age, Mile, Sand Stone 44% Chamber, Mask, Natural Gas 44% Health, Taker, Less Care 44% Easy RATs Opera, Hand, Dish Intermediate RATs *Soluion rates reported in Bowden and Jung-Beeman’s study (2003) 249 Appendix H Rebuses Used in Experiment III Rebus Problem Answer Relevant Misleading Item Cue 1 NA Add insult to injury add (filler item) Just between you 2 between both small centre fall tear and me It is a small world 3 after all 4 Waterfall 250 NA 5 Long john long (filler task) 6 Search high and low high Gap 7 Splitting headache split She thick pool too mirror Blood is thicker than 8 water 9 Too big to ignore 251 Through thick and 10 thin thin 252 repeat Appendix I Visual Insight Problems Used in Experiment IV Set 1 Problems Solutions Problem 1 English Description: Move part of the operator Move one stick to make the following statement into a correct one. Chinese Description: 移動1根火柴使以下算式變得正確 1 Restructuring Problem 2 English Description: Move one stick to make the following statement into a correct one Chinese Description: 移動1根火柴使以下算式變得正確 253 2 Problem 3 Re-conceiving the English Description: There are 10 coins on the table, please move 3 orientation of the shape coins to make the triangle point downwards Chinese Description: 枱上有10個硬幣, 請移動3個硬幣 使三角形 指向下方 2 Problem 4 English Description: Add 2 more square enclosures that would put each circle in a pen by itself Chinese Description: 加上2個正方形,使每個圓形也有自已的空間 254 3 Problem 5 Shapes in different sizes English Description: Remove 2 sticks to leave two squares with no loose end. Chinese Description: 移歨2根火柴使剩下2個正方形, 火柴需互相 連結, 沒有缺口 3 Problem 6 English Description: There are 9 sticks on the table arranged, please move 3 sticks to make 5 equilateral triangles. 255 Chinese Description: 枱上有9根火柴, 請移動3根火柴以變出5個等 邊三角形 4 Problem 7 English Description: Re-arrange into a 3-D object There are 4 coins on the table, please move 1 coin so that each coin touches exactly 3 other coins Chinese Description:枱上有4個硬幣,請移動1個硬幣, 使每個硬幣 能觸到另外的3個硬幣 256 4 Problem 8 English Description: There are 6 sticks on the table, please use them to make 4 equal-sided triangles Chinese Description:枱上有6根火柴, 請用它們砌出4個等邊三角形 257 258