Prome 1 Najiyah Prome Cales UNIV 112 November 24, 2014 Unit III Essay Improvements in technology portray texting as a norm committed by many and persuade phone users to make texting their dominant source of communication over time. Even though technology has helped make texting more efficient, texting while driving is still an inefficient act that can result in horrifying accidents. For example, the “National Phone Survey on Distracted Driving Attitudes and Behavior” by Julie Tison, Neil Chaudhary, and Linda Cosgrove reports that 49% of drivers text while driving, alluding that texting while driving is becoming a norm that almost dominates texting. This form of distraction is be quite serious as texting while driving endangers and affects many lives. Texting while driving motivates drivers to value communication over safety, as drivers are willing to gamble people’s lives for the sake of replying or reading someone’s texts. Even though many can agree, that texting while driving is dangerous, they can mischaracterize drivers as careless. Drivers do not text and drive because they are ignorant or uneducated of the dangers of texting while driving. However, drivers text and drive because communication with friends and family is so important to them, they are willing to put their passenger, the public, and their own life in danger for the sake of replying to someone’s text. Drivers rank communication over safety because of the false notion that technology makes it easier to text and drive. Technology helps our lives become more efficient, likewise, the improvement of technologies also benefit communication. However, this improvement in Prome 2 communication does not justify texting while driving nor does it protect or prevent drivers from accidents when they are texting and driving. For example, the text, “The Choice to Text and Drive in Younger Drivers: Behavior May Shape Attitude” by Paul Atchley, Stephanie Atwood, and Aaron Boulton reports that young drivers are in danger of accidents due to texting while driving. This is because according to their study, “younger adults are using texting as a primary means of communication” (Atchley et al.) illustrating that as technology evolves, more humans are switching their communication method to texting. Although this revolution has its benefits in efficient communication, it can have potential risks especially when texting and driving. Improvement in technology usually forces humans to become heavily dependent on it because as it makes daily-life efficient. Thus, people began to rely on it to solve daily life problems. This can lead drivers to believe that texting while driving might not result accidents as technology can help make texting easy. However, technology has not gotten to the point that makes texting while driving safe, illustrating that drivers prioritize communication. This is because when claiming that technology makes driving safe, people usually debates the improvements in texting makes texting while driving safe rather than the driving component such as improvements in cars. Even though there has been great progresses in cars such as autonomous car or auto-drive functions in cars that can counter such notions, however, these improvements in cars has yet to illustrate their ability to prevent accidents from texting while driving. In addition, the article, “Texting while driving on automatic: Considering the frequencyindependent side of habit” by Joseph Bayer and Scott Campbell mentions “that mobile communication is turning people into zombies” (Bayer, and Cambell) further depicting that as technology evolves people become dependent on it that could possibly turn them into “technology-loving zombies”. This is dangerous because being too dependent on technology can Prome 3 encourage drivers to text while driving. Furthermore, advanced technologies can make drivers consider that improvements in technology can prevent accidents while texting and driving. Even though being a “technology-loving zombies” have its benefits in communication and entertainment, technology’s efficiency has yet to improve drivers’ abilities to text and drive at the same time. Similarly, as texting while driving becomes a familiar concept, drivers are becoming more aware and knowledgeable of the dangers of texting while driving. However, improvements in technology can blind that knowledge and awareness, eventually making drivers reliant on technology while texting and driving. Focusing on texting instead of driving not only creates a behavior of distracted driving but it also illustrates driver’s treasuring one life over another. When drivers are texting while driving, they are attempting to strengthen or maintain their personal bonds but at the cost of others. This is because when drivers are adding texting to their driving, they are setting up a scenario that illustrates that drivers are willing to risk lives. In such scenarios, drivers are attempting to extend their communication and strengthen it, however, at the cost of their and others lives. In addition, due to the lack personal bonds between the drivers, pedestrians, and others that share the road can encourage drivers to pay more attention to texting than driving or even encourage drivers to text and drive. Although no one intends to do such a thing purposefully, texting while driving reflects the notion of favoritism towards one or more lives and irresponsibility towards others. Therefore, depicting that drivers are willing to risk lives to maintain or strengthen their relationship to whom they were texting. Philosopher Stephen Asma’s, “The Myth of Universal Love” also agrees with this concept that people care more for their tribe than the outside world and raises issues such as whether people or drivers should care about strangers or pedestrians in the first place. In his article, Asma states “All people are not Prome 4 equally entitled to my time” (Asma 12) portraying that it is impossible for one person to solely dedicated their time to everyone. This is because humans treat other humans depending on their perspective of them and their interaction with them. Asma’s statement also parallels the notion that drivers might feel more obligated prioritize their friends and family’s texts more than their surrounding public causing them to text and drive, and feeling that the people they were texting are more deserving of their time. Thus, following Asma’s approach to the situation, drivers pay more attention towards texting then driving, because they feel more loyalty towards the people they were texting. In addition, the “National Phone Survey on Distracted Driving Attitudes and Behavior” also agrees with such notion. It reports that “Respondents’ decisions to accept…messages while driving cluster around how important they felt the other person or communication was” (Tison et al.), illustrating the concept that drivers do consider the role of communication when texting while driving and prioritize communication over safety. However, philosophers such as Peter Singer would argue against such notion. In his article, “The Drowning Child and the Expanding Circle” Singer states that “there is harmony between ethics and enlightened self-interest” (Singer 380) depicting drivers should not necessarily place communication over driving, but rather keep a balance between the two and avoid texting while driving at the same time. In addition, Singer’s view point suggest that texting while driving can be easily avoidable as self-interest and ethical considerations can be in harmony. However, in reality, self-interest can overshadow ethical considerations, as favoritism can dominates over ethical considerations towards strangers. Therefore, when drivers are texting while driving under the notion of favoritism, they can easily violate ethical matters because they are working in favor of self-interest to strengthen or maintain communication with friends and Prome 5 family. Texting while driving has serious consequences, yet drivers commit to such acts to maintain communication by valuing one life over another. A driver’s overconfidence on their ability to efficiently text and drive blinds them into focusing more on communication in the form of texting instead of driving. For example, the “National Phone Survey on Distracted Driving Attitudes and Behaviors” mentions that 25% of drivers have “said that texting or sending messages makes no difference on their driving performance” (Tison et al.) exemplifying an attitude of over overconfidence that a great amount of drivers that text and drives feels. Thus, drivers use that confidence as a method to justify their ability to text and drive. However, this feeling is problematic because it is impossible to know whether drivers’ behavior will result in dangerous events or not. This feeling of confidence also comes from drivers’ belief that they are efficient multitasker, thus, they should be able to text and drive efficiently boosting their confidence into overconfidence. As multitasking is becoming a way of life, multitasking involving texting is also common. However, multitasking when texting can encourage texting while driving because multitasking can cause overconfidence in their ability to text that causes drivers to believe they can text and drive safely. Furthermore, the study “Behavioral Performance and Visual Attention in Communication Multitasking: A Comparison between Instant Messaging and Online Voice Chat” by Wanga Zheng Prabu Davidb, Jatin Srivastavac, Stacie Powersa, Christine Bradya, Jonathan D'Angeloa, and Jennifer Morelanda also addresses implications of drivers’ overconfidence in their driving and multitasking abilities. The study mentions “tasks, such as texting…are conducted while driving…is expected to have a worse impact on the driving task” (Zheng et al.) portraying that texting does in fact have a great impact on drivers’ performances. This is contrasting, to the concept of multitasking that encourages texting while driving. As well as depicting multitasking Prome 6 within driving as harmful especially in terms of texting as it escalates over-confidence causing drivers to text and drive. Even though drivers can claim that they are efficient multitaskers, studies such as “Behavioral Performance and Visual Attention in Communication Multitasking: A Comparison between Instant Messaging and Online Voice Chat” reports “significant drop in performance when multitasking and communicating at the same time” (Zheng et al.). Thus, illustrating sufficient evidence that counters the idea that being good at multitasking can avoid accidents due to texting and driving. In addition, evidences from Zheng et al. study also elucidate drivers’ confidence in their skills to drive while multitasking such as texting and driving is invalid. When arguing that they are excellent drivers, drivers’ overconfidence make them pay more attention to communication instead of safety because overconfidence makes them devalue their ability in driving and focus more on their texting skills. Even though confidence is healthy, overconfidence is risky especially in terms of texting and driving because this behavior causes drivers to focus more on communicating with their friends and family than driving. Texting while driving depicts drivers willing to put one’s life over the other for the sake of maintaining communication with another person. Even though drivers commit this act unconsciously, it can have severe implications. It is not because the driver does not care or value about his or her surroundings but rather fails to acknowledge it under the influence of texting. Therefore, these influences encourages drivers into believing it is possible to text and drive at the same time and avoid accidents even though it is not true. Word Count: 1774 Prome 7 Works Cited Asma, Stephen T. "The Myth of Universal Love." Evolving Ideas: Focused Inquiry. 2014-2015 ed. N.p.: VCU, n.d. 12-16. Print. Atchley, Paul, Stephanie Atwood, and Aaron Boulton. “The Choice to Text and Drive in Younger Drivers: Behavior May Shape Attitude.” Science Direct. Elsevier B.V., Jan. 2011. Web. 13 Nov. 2014. Bayer, Joseph B., and Campbell, Scott W. “Texting While Driving on Automatic: Considering the Frequency-independent Side of Habit.” Computers in Human Behavior 28.6 (2012): 2083-090. Print Singer, Peter. "The Drowning Child and the Expanding Circle." Evolving Ideas: Focused Inquiry. 2014-2015 ed. N.p.: VCU, n.d. 380-83. Print. Tison, Julie, Neil Chaudhary, and Linda Cosgrove. National Phone Survey on Distracted Driving Attitudes and Behaviors. National Technical Information Service. N.p., Dec. 2011. Web. 28 Oct. 2014. Wanga, Zheng, Prabu Davidb, Jatin Srivastavac, Stacie Powersa, Christine Bradya, Jonathan D'Angeloa, and Jennifer Morelanda. "Behavioral Performance and Visual Attention in Communication Multitasking: A Comparison between Instant Messaging and Online Voice Chat." ScienceDirect. N.p., May 2012. Web. 23 Nov. 2014.