Rao 1 The adaptive significance of aggressive behavior in the Nazca Booby Photo credits: Antariksh Mahajan Galapagos Sophomore College 2015 By Meghana Rao Rao 2 Introduction The Nazca booby, Sula granti, is one of the three species of boobies that inhabits the Galapagos Islands. Galapagos is home to the Nazca booby, the blue-footed booby, and the redfooted booby. While none are endemic, the Nazca booby differentiates itself from the other species by its size, with a wingspan exceeding five feet. Due to its large body size, the Nazca booby is often found on cliffs, where height assists with propelling them into flight. They also have distinct brown facial features and a yellow bill and are pelagic feeders. Between the months of August and November and November through February, they build ground nests and breed monogamously on the island of Genovesa and Espanola, respectively. Nazca boobies lay two eggs per nest and have a long period of hatching asynchrony, the five-day period between the hatching of the first and second egg (Galapagos Conservancy 2015). Nazca booby spotted on a cliff The blue-footed booby, Sula nebouxxi The Nazca booby partakes in obligate siblicide. Obligate siblicide is the routine killing of the younger sibling by the older sibling. In practice, the older chick generally pushes the younger chick out of the nest moments after its birth, where the chick starves to death, in the presence of the parents. The process is known as “obligate” because it occurs regardless of external factors Rao 3 such as abundant food supply or negligible size dimorphism. In contrast, the blue-footed booby partakes in facultative siblicide, where siblicide only occurs if the food supply cannot support both offspring. The aggressive behaviors of the Nazca booby observed during obligate siblicide extend into adulthood, when non-parental adult visitor behavior (NAV behavior) becomes ubiquitous (Anderson 1990). Non-parental adult visitor behavior (NAV) is when non-breeding adults seek out unattended chicks and initiate various interactions with them. Non-breeding adults display keen interest in non-familial chicks. Research done by Anderson depicted that 51.5% of NAV behavior is aggressive, 46.3% is affiliative (grooming), and 12.2% is sexual (Anderson 2004). This behavior is widespread in large breeding groups, and most adults will initiate NAV at least once in it during their lifetime. Chicks become victims of NAV between the first 30 and 80 days of their lives, because this is when parents begin foraging for food and leaving them unattended. NAV behavior strays away from the traditional benefits of interaction between adult and nonrelated young (Anderson 2004). Generally, interactions between non-related adults and young are motivated by nutritional benefits, reduction of future competition by killing the young, or kin-selected actions. NAV behavior serves as an anomaly, posing the question of how and why it occurs. An adult Nazca booby amidst the opuntia Rao 4 Currently, researchers seek to better understand the relationship between the Nazca booby’s obligate siblicide practice and NAV behavior practice. The physiological implications of both aggressive behaviors catalyze questions regarding causation, evolutionary significance, and potential permanent neuroendocrine alterations. Recent research on Nazca booby aggression addresses factors related to the physiological understanding of aggression in vertebrate mammals. In vertebrate animals, androgen levels and corticosterone levels regulate aggression. Androgens are sex steroid hormones, including testosterone and progesterone, and corticosterone is a corticoid hormone secreted by the adrenal gland and comprises the primary avian stress response (Nelson 2006). Understanding the role of hormones and the adaptive significance of the aggressive behavior of the Nazca booby is a growing field of research Hypotheses In this research, two hypotheses focusing on siblicide and NAV behavior will be pursued. They are: 1. Lethal aggression levels in Nazca boobies are beneficial during the neonatal stage because they enable brood reduction. 2. Aggression becomes maladaptive in adulthood because it catalyzes a cycle of harmful nonparental adult visitor interactions (NAV). Each hypothesis will be evaluated through reading and analysis of existing literature. Findings Hypothesis 1: Rao 5 To address the first hypothesis, the significance of brood reduction through obligate siblicide must be established. Research demonstrates that the optimal brood size for the Nazca booby is one. A single chick is more likely to pass on its genes to future generations in comparison to a chick raised with a sibling. In 1962, Reduced Dorward proposed the egg-insurance hypothesis to Enlarged Figure 1: Proportion of clutches that produced at least one hatchling for is ideal (Dorward 1962). Nazca boobies hatch reduced and enlarged clutch sizes (Anderson and approximately 60% of the eggs they lay due to infertility, and therefore, theClifford second 2001, egg provides p. 343) explain the persistence of two-egg clutches if one chick an insurance policy if the first fails to hatch. In 2001, Clifford and Anderson experimentally tested the egg-insurance hypothesis. In their experiment, they manipulated clutch sizes, creating reduced clutches of one egg by removing eggs and then creating enlarged clutches by placing removed eggs into natural one-egg clutches. Figure 1 depicts reduced and enlarged clutches versus the probability of each hatching at least a single egg. Enlarged clutches had higher rates of hatching at least one egg, demonstrating that it is beneficial for the Nazca booby to lay two eggs (Clifford and Anderson 2001). As outlined in the introduction, androgen levels and CORT levels regulate aggression in vertebrate animals. In 2001, Tarlow studied CORT activity during Nazca booby brood reduction. In the research, a colony of 2-egg clutch 1-egg clutch 2-egg clutch Figure 2: CORT levels in A and B chicks and one-egg clutches (Tarlow 2001) Rao 6 10,000 boobies were studied on Espanola. Researchers observed the nests daily, collecting blood samples. Their findings, depicted in Figure 2, demonstrate that CORT levels are higher in Achicks (1st born) in comparison to B-chicks (2nd born). Single chicks from one-egg clutches had even lower levels of CORT than B-chicks. The research suggests that the differing CORT levels may illuminate inherent differences in aggression levels in the chicks based on their relationship to the siblicide process (victim vs. perpetrator vs. not involved). Research by Grace and Anderson in 2014 explains the significance of higher baseline CORT levels in the Nazca booby. Grace and Anderson discovered that high baseline levels correlate to increased likelihood of partaking in “gardening” and “aggression,” where gardening is anxiety and agitation-related moving of nest material and aggression consists of general biting and jabbing motions. The research done by Grace and Anderson and Tarlow indicate that Nazca boobies are born with inherent varying levels of aggression that enable their implementation of the evolutionarily beneficial obligate siblicide. In addition, studies have compared factors that influence aggression in both Nazca and blue-footed boobies. The comparison of the Nazca to the blue-footed booby provides effective evidence towards explaining the Nazca booby’s inherent aggression since the blue-footed booby does not partake in obligate siblicide. Instead, they partake in facultative siblicide, where siblicide only takes place in the case of limited food supply. In 2008, Mueller compared levels of androgens in Nazca boobies and blue-footed boobies moments after birth through blood sampling. The results, observable in Figure 3, demonstrate that the Nazca booby has approximately three times the androgen level of the blue-footed booby. In addition, first-born chicks (2ECA), the ones that initiate siblicide, had higher levels of androgens than second born (ECB). The differences correspond to distinct variations in exposure during the embryonic Rao 7 period, not upregulation, since the measurements were taken at birth. Through the comparison to the less aggressive relative, this study supports the hypothesis that the Nazca booby possesses a greater inherent aggression that enables obligate siblicide. For hypothesis one, the evidence demonstrates an evolutionary need for obligate siblicide and physiological explanations for inherent differences in the Figure 3: Comparison of Androgen Levels at Birth for Blue Footed vs. Nazca Booby (Muller 2008, p.8) aggression that enables it. Heightened levels of CORT and androgens in A-chicks and 2- egg clutches in comparison to B-chicks and one-egg clutches suggest varying aggressiveness based on the bird’s role in siblicidal act. The research demonstrated lower levels of androgens in the blue-footed boobies coupled with heightened levels of CORT and androgens at a young age for the Nazca booby, with levels higher in the older sibling. These increased levels are present during times of siblicide and not present in birds that partake in facultative siblicide, indicating a relationship between heightened aggression during the neonatal stage and the ability of the booby to commit obligate siblicide, supporting the hypothesis. Hypothesis Two: The second hypothesis seeks to understand the adaptive significance of the continuation of aggressive behavior into adulthood, as it manifests itself in NAV behavior. Researchers have identified a relationship between siblicide and NAV. Mueller’s research in 2008 elucidates the relationship; in Figure 4: Relationship between number of NAV events committed by siblicidal and nonsiblicidal chicks (Muller 2008, p. 3) Rao 8 a two-egg clutch where both eggs hatch (2ECA+) and siblicide occurs, the older chick is likely to initiate more NAV events than in a two-egg clutch where only one egg hatches (2EC-) and no siblicide occurs. Chicks that commit siblicide are more likely to commit NAV. In 2011, Mueller further explicated the nature of NAV behavior and defined it as a “cycle of violence.” As seen in Figure 5, the total number of NAV events experienced as a nestling directly correlates to an increase in the number of NAV events initiated by the same bird as an adult, creating a positive feedback loop, illustrated below as well. Interpretation of the Nazca booby cycle of violence from chick to adulthood Figure 5: The Cycle of Violence, relationship between NAV events performed as an adult and experienced as a nestling (Mueller 2011, p. 5) As observed by Mueller in 2008, siblicide may play an activational role for NAV behavior. However, even non-siblicidal chicks partake in NAV, indicating further causes. In 2003, Tarlow studied the hormonal activity during NAV behavior on the island of Espanola. The researchers observed NAV behavior opportunistically and initiated human interruption to take blood samples when observed. Blood sampling findings shown in Figure 6 demonstrate that CORT levels were significantly higher in adults initiating NAV behavior than Rao 9 in non-NAV adults. The increased CORT levels during NAV mirror the increased CORT levels observed in siblicidal chicks and indicate the involvement of inherent aggression levels. In 2011, Anderson et al researched the CORT levels in chicks during NAV interactions. He observed that NAV victims experience a five-fold increase in CORT during NAV interactions in comparison to baseline levels, as seen in Figure 6. The CORT levels remain at 2.8-fold the baseline level the following morning and take weeks to return to regular levels, revealing the magnitude of the impact of NAV behavior on the Nazca booby chicks. Figure 6: CORT levels immediately after NAV for victim (left) and the morning after (right) (Anderson et al 2014, p. 83) The stress response activated in the chicks during NAV is known as the HPA stress axis response and is regulated by the hypothalamus, pituitary, and adrenal glands. During the response, the adrenal gland excretes corticoid hormones as the final product of the negative feedback cycle (Mitrovic 2015). Repeated activation of the HPA stress axis has produced longterm neuroendocrine changes in rats, permanently organizing future adult maltreatment in their lives (Boccia and Pederson 2001). The HPA axis stress response is also observed in the human cycle of violence, where children who are abused are more likely to become abusers. Abused Rao 10 women and children experience the same hypercortisol release followed by a slow decline that NAV victim chicks experience. Nazca boobies serve as a non-human model for the cycle of violence, an unusual behavior pattern that may span across multiple vertebrate classes. Currently, there is no direct explanation for NAV behavior. While the hypothesis proposes that NAV behavior is maladaptive, the frequency of its occurrence and its widespread integration within the life cycle of the Nazca booby makes it difficult to interpret as such. When a Nazca booby is not mating, it engages in NAV behavior. It is possible that the NAV trait emerged due to a strong positive selection for siblicide, which may permanently organize the chick to initiate NAV behavior when older. In contrast, NAV behavior may be weakly selected against due to the nonfatal nature of the harm it induces (Anderson 2004). While the evidence presented illuminates a strong correlation between siblicide and NAV behavior and a correlation between NAV behavior and permanent hormonal damage, it does not support the labeling of NAV behavior as maladaptive. Conclusion and Recommendations This research studied literature regarding the adaptive significance of obligate siblicide and NAV behavior. The first hypothesis that was investigated proposed that the lethal aggression levels in Nazca boobies are beneficial because they enable brood reduction. Research by Anderson and Clifford established the necessity for brood reduction and the benefits of a twoegg clutch. Tarlow demonstrated how first born chicks have higher CORT levels than second born chicks and chicks born in one-egg clutches. Higher baseline CORT levels correlate to an increased likelihood of partaking in aggressive behavior. In addition, Nazca boobies have three times higher androgen levels than blue-footed boobies at birth, which are facultatively siblicidal. The increased baseline androgen and CORT levels in first-born Nazca boobies suggest that they Rao 11 possess an inherent aggression that enables obligate siblicide, supporting the hypothesis. The second hypothesis proposed a correlation between youthful and adult aggressive behaviors and hypothesized that NAV behavior was maladaptive. Muller’s work demonstrates that siblicidal birds are more likely to perform NAV behavior when older and that a cycle of violence emerges. There is a direct correlation between the number of NAV events a chick experiences and the number of events they initiate when they are older. CORT levels are also significantly higher than the baseline in adults performing NAV and are increased by five-fold in chicks who are victimized. The cyclical and ubiquitous nature of NAV behavior makes it difficult to label as maladaptive. Rather, it is potentially a trait that is weakly selected against de to strong selective pressures for obligate siblicide, which may catalyze the cycle of violence through hormonal alterations. Therefore, the evidence does not support the second hypothesis. Future research regarding Nazca booby aggression can lead to a better understanding of human behavior. Since the Nazca booby serves as one of the few non-human models of the cycle of violence, it could be beneficial to observe and understand physiological and physical aspects of it. Through gaining a deeper understanding of a parallel cycle of violence, researchers may discover the mechanisms that break the cycle. Also, it would be valuable to understand how NAV behavior spreads and whether it can spread between species through observing the interactions between the Nazca booby and other booby species. Acknowledgements Thank you so much Bill, Mari, and Annette for unconditionally supporting our group and for selflessly and patiently teaching us about the Galapagos. Thank you so much for instilling in me a newfound love for the natural world, for opening my eyes to hundreds of new species and their evolutionary adaptations, and for giving me the opportunity to visit a surreal place. In Rao 12 addition, I have found a family of passionate peers through this process, and I could not have possibly dreamed of a better experience. “Nazca booby, living just to fly the ocean” – Don’t Stop Competin’ by Isabella Jibillian Please note the Nazca booby in the left corner of the cliff Photo credits: Isabelle Crary Bibliography Anderson, D. J., Porter, E. T., & Elise D. Ferree 2004, ‘Non-breeding Nazca Boobies (Sula Granti) Show Social and Sexual Interest in Chicks: Behavioural and Ecological Aspects.’, Behaviour, vol. 141, no. 8, pp. 959-977. Available from <http://users.wfu.edu/djanders/labweb/reprints/Anderson%20et%20al%20NAV%202004.pdf> [25 August 2015]. Anderson 1990, ‘Evolution of Obligate Siblicide in Boobies: A Test of the Insurance-Egg Hypothesis’, The American Naturalist, vol. 135, no. 3, pp. 335-350. Available from < http://users.wfu.edu/djanders/labweb/reprints/Anderson%201900%20Am%20Nat.pdf > [25 August 2015]. Boccia M.L, Pederson C.A 2001, ‘Brief vs. long maternal separations in infancy: contrasting relationships with adult maternal behavior and lactation levels of aggression and anxiety’, Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 26, pp. 657-672. Available from <http://www.psyneuenjournal.com/article/S0306-4530(01)00019-1/abstract> [15 September 2015]. Dorward, D. F. 1962. Comparative biology of the white booby and the brown booby Sula spp. at Ascension. Ibis 103b: 174-200. Escontrela, D., 2014. Maltreatment as hatchlings predisposes Nazca boobies to more frequent violent episodes as adults. Available from: < http://rjd.miami.edu/conservation/maltreatment-as- Rao 13 hatchlings-predisposes-nazca-boobies-to-more-frequent-violent-episodes-as-adults > [25 August 2015]. Galapagos Conservancy Fact Sheet: The Three Boobies of the Galapagos Islands, 2015. Available from < http://guides.is.uwa.edu.au/ld.php?content_id=13183347> [27 August 2015]. Grace JK, Dean K, Ottinger M, & Anderson DJ 2011, ‘Hormonal effects of maltreatment in Nazca booby nestlings: Implications for the “cycle of violence”’, Hormones and Behavior, vol. 60, no. 1, pp.78-85. Available from <http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0018506X11000663 > [25 August 2015]. Liang AC, Grace JK, Tompkins EM, & Anderson DJ 2014, ‘Yawning, acute stressors, and arousal reduction in Nazca booby adults and nestlings,’ Physiology & Behavior, vol. 140, no. 1, pp. 38-43. Available from <http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0031938414005460> [25 August 2015]. Mitrovic, Igor 2015, ‘Introduction to the Hypothalamus-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Stress Response’, University of California San Francisco Biochemistry. Available from <http://biochemistry2.ucsf.edu/programs/ptf/mn%20links/HPA%20Axis%20Physio.pdf > [10 September 2015]. Morandini, V & M. Ferrer 2015, ‘Sibling aggression and brood reduction: a review’, Ethology Ecology & Evolution, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 2-16. Available from <http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03949370.2014.880161> [25 August 2015]. Muller MS, Brennecke JF, Porter ET, Ottinger MA, & Anderson DJ 2008, ‘Perinatal Androgens and Adult Behavior Vary with Nestling Social System in Siblicidal Boobies.’, PLoS ONE vol. 3, no. 6. Available from <http://www.plosone.org/article/fetchObject.action?uri=info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.po ne.0002460&representation=PDF> [25 August 2015]. Muller MS, Porter ET, Grace JK, Westbrock MA, & Anderson DJ 2011, ‘Maltreated Nestlings Exhibit Correlated Maltreatment As Adults: Evidence of a “Cycle of Violence” in Nazca Boobies (Sula Granti),’ The Auk: An International Journal of Ornithology, vol. 128, no. 4, pp. 615-619. Available from < http://aoucospubs.org/doi/pdf/10.1525/auk.2011.11008 > [25 August 2015]. Nazca Boobies 2011, Beauty of Birds. Available from: <http://beautyofbirds.com/nazcaboobies.html> [25 August 2015]. Nelson, R.J. Biology of Aggression, Oxford University Press, New York, 2006. Tarlow, Elisa M, Martin Wikelski, & David J Anderson 2001, ‘Hormonal Correlates of Siblicide in Galapagos Nazca Boobies.’, Hormones and Behavior 40, vol. 1, pp. 14-20. Available from <http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11467880> [25 August 2015]. Rao 14 Wake Forest University 2008. ‘High Hormone Levels in Seabird Chicks Prepare Them To Kill Their Siblings,’ ScienceDaily. Available from <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2008/06/080618172949.htm> [25 August 2015].