1 Putting a name to a face: An examination of the relationship between the processing of names and faces. The ability to pair a name with a face is important in our society, as it is essential to the recognition of people. However, the cognitive mechanism underlying this pairing is still debated. This experiment aimed to answer whether there is an inherent relationship between the processing of names and faces. Forty-nine Wellesley College undergraduate students completed 4 study/test blocks in which they studied abstract nouns alone, proper names alone, abstract nouns paired with faces, and proper names paired with faces. They then identified the previously studied word in a forced choice task. We hypothesized that the pairing of a name with a face would enhance memory for that name. Contrary to our prediction, however, faces impaired memory for names, and had no effect on memory for abstract nouns. Though the results are not what we predicted, they do indicate that the processes of memory for names and faces are not independent. Results The number of studied stimuli correctly identified in a forced choice task was measured and averaged for each experimental condition. A 2x2 repeated measures ANOVA with independent variables of stimulus type (abstract noun vs. proper name) and presentation (word alone vs. word with a face) was completed for this experiment. Results show the main effect of stimulus type was significant (F(1, 49) = 8.94, p < .05), indicating that abstract nouns were recalled better than proper names regardless of presentation. Results also showed the main effect of presentation to be significant (F(1, 49) = 18.51, p < .001). This indicates that both abstract nouns and proper names presented alone are recalled better than presented paired with a face. As seen in Table 1, the mean number of words correctly recalled for proper names when presented 2 alone is 75.7. This number is larger than any other condition, accounts for the main effects observed, and points to an interaction. Critically, the interaction of presentation by stimulus type is significant (p = .019). This shows that presentation does differ across stimulus type. More specifically for proper names, performance was better when the names were presented alone compared to when paired with a face, whereas for abstract nouns performance was similar when the noun was presented alone and paired with a face. To support this, paired t tests comparing abstract nouns alone and paired with a face, and proper names and abstract nouns both paired with faces revealed no significant differences between number of correct answers for each of these conditions, t(49) = 1.265, p > .200, and t(49) = -1.018, p > .300, respectively. In contrast, the number of correct answers for proper names alone was significantly higher than those for abstract nouns alone, t(49) = -3.650, p = .001, and proper names paired with a face, t(49) = 4.707, p < .001. Additionally, the results showed that there was no significant effect of order on stimulus type (F(1, 49) = .646, p > .80) nor on presentation (F(1, 49) = 1.83, p > .05). Thus, counterbalancing was successful as participants were unaffected regardless of the order in which the study/test blocks were presented. 3 Table 1 Mean Number of Correct Answers for Nouns and Names With and Without Faces Word Alone Paired with Face Mean Abstract Nouns 69.2 67.5 68.3 Proper Names 75.4 69.3 72.3 Mean 72.3 68.4 4 Figure 1: Mean Number of Correct Answers for Nouns and Names With and Without Faces. ±2 SE. 5 Abstract Noun 79 Proper Names Mean Correct Answers 77 75 73 71 69 67 65 Word Alone Paired with Face