1 Variation in harbour porpoise activity in response to seismic survey noise 2 Enrico Pirotta *,a, Kate L. Brookes b,c, Isla M. Graham b, Paul M. Thompson b 3 4 a Institute of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen AB24 2TZ, 5 6 UK b Lighthouse Field Station, Institute of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of 7 8 Aberdeen, Cromarty IV11 8YL, UK c Marine Scotland Science, Marine Laboratory, Aberdeen AB11 9DB, UK 9 10 *Corresponding author: enrico.pirotta@abdn.ac.uk. 11 12 13 14 Short title: Seismic survey affects porpoise activity. 15 Summary 16 Animals exposed to anthropogenic disturbance make trade-offs between perceived risk and the cost of 17 leaving disturbed areas. Impact assessments tend to focus on overt behavioural responses leading to 18 displacement, but trade-offs may also impact individual energy budgets through reduced foraging 19 performance. Previous studies found no evidence for broad–scale displacement of harbour porpoises 20 exposed to impulse noise from a 10-day 2-D seismic survey. Here, we used an array of passive 21 acoustic loggers coupled with calibrated noise measurements to test whether the seismic survey 22 influenced the activity patterns of porpoises remaining in the area. We showed that the probability of 23 recording a buzz declined by 15% in the ensonified area, and was positively related to distance from 24 the source vessel. We also estimated received levels at the hydrophones and characterised the noise 25 response curve. Our results demonstrate how environmental impact assessments can be developed to 26 assess more subtle effects of noise disturbance on activity patterns and foraging efficiency. 27 Keywords: activity budget, anthropogenic disturbance, environmental impact assessment, foraging 28 efficiency 29 30 Introduction 31 Human disturbance may not lead to acute, easily-measurable behavioural responses in exposed 32 wildlife [1,2]. Less overt effects can arise from trade-offs that animals make [3], such as deciding to 33 remain in disturbed areas and tolerating higher exposure levels where prey are abundant [4]. 34 However, these trade-offs may also influence activity budgets, potentially affecting fecundity and 35 survival [1,5] or having biologically significant consequences at a population level [6–9]. 36 There is particular concern over the potential impacts of underwater anthropogenic noise on marine 37 mammals [10,11]. Aversive behaviour towards intense noise sources such as pile driving (e.g. [12]) 38 and seismic surveys [13] may not necessarily lead to broad-scale displacement [14]. However, it is not 39 known whether there are more subtle changes in the activity budgets of animals remaining in exposed 40 areas. 41 Patterns of echolocation clicks can be used to characterise the activity of both bats [15] and 42 odontocetes [16–19]. Here, we use an array of passive acoustic loggers coupled with calibrated noise 43 measurements to assess the effects of a seismic survey on the activity pattern of harbour porpoises 44 (Phocoena phocoena). 45 Material and Methods 46 The study was conducted around a commercial 2-D seismic survey in NE Scotland. The survey vessel 47 used a 470 cu inch airgun array with a shot point interval of 5-6 seconds to survey a 200 km2 area 48 between 1st and 11th September 2011 (see electronic supplementary material). Full details are provided 49 in [14]. 50 Patterns of porpoise echolocation clicks were characterised at 22 sites in and around the seismic 51 survey area throughout August and September 2011 (Fig. 1). Data from V.0 and V.1 CPODs 52 (www.chelonia.co.uk) had previously been used to characterise the occurrence of porpoises [14,19]. 53 While the sensitivity of C-PODs at different sites could vary between different units or different local 54 conditions, our sampling design and mixed modelling approach were designed to account for this (see 55 electronic supplementary material). 56 We calculated the inter-click intervals (ICI) from the time series of detected clicks. A Gaussian 57 mixture-model was fitted to log-transformed ICIs to identify different patterns of echolocation clicks 58 [16]. Each ICI was classified as either a regular ICI (regular clicking for navigation and prey 59 searching), a buzz ICI (buzzes associated with attempted prey captures or social communication), or 60 an inter-train ICI (pauses between click trains) [20,21] (see electronic supplementary material). 61 Following [14], we used data from CPODs in 25 km by 25 km control and impact areas (Fig. 1; see 62 electronic supplementary material) to assess the effect of seismic surveys on the occurrence of buzz 63 ICIs when porpoises were present (i.e. in hours in which at least one ICI was detected). The presence 64 of buzz ICIs was modelled as a function of seismic period (before or during) and experimental block 65 (control or impact). Crucially, we also evaluated the interaction between these covariates to test 66 whether seismic surveys influenced buzzing activity. We fitted a binary mixed-effects model in R 67 13.01 [22], with location included as a random effect. Model selection was based on AIC and the 68 significance of retained covariates evaluated using Wald’s tests. 69 The presence of buzz ICIs for each minute in which at least one ICI was detected was then modelled 70 as a function of distance from the seismic vessel. Following previous studies [12,14] on likely spatial 71 scales of porpoise responses to noise, we used data from within 25 km of the vessel (see electronic 72 supplementary material). A binary Generalized Linear Model was fitted using Generalized Estimating 73 Equations (GEE-GLM) [23] (see electronic supplementary material). We used QICu for model 74 selection [24], and Wald’s tests to assess significance [25]. 75 Calibrated noise measurements were made at 15 sites between 1.6 km and 61.8 km from the seismic 76 vessel [14]. Airgun noise was characterised in terms of broadband sound exposure level for single 77 pulses in dB re 1 μPa2s (using the region of the waveform that contained the central 90% of the 78 pulse’s energy; hereafter SEL). Peak-to-peak source levels were estimated to be 242 –253 dB re 1 μPa 79 at 1 m. We tested whether there was a relationship between buzz occurrence and estimated noise 80 levels at the corresponding CPOD location using a bootstrapping procedure (see electronic 81 supplementary material). 82 Results 83 There was a significant interaction between experimental block and seismic period (Wald’s test: 84 χ2=7.2; df=1; p=0.007), indicating that the seismic survey had a differential effect in the control and 85 impact block. This resulted in a 15% reduction in the occurrence of buzz ICIs in the impact block 86 during the survey (Fig. 2). Wide confidence intervals highlight high levels of natural variability in 87 detection of buzzes. 88 The probability of occurrence of buzzes increased significantly with distance from source (Wald’s 89 test: χ2=9.6; df=1; p=0.002), ranging from approximately 0.15 at 0 km (95% confidence interval (CI): 90 0.11-0.19) to 0.35 at 40 km (95% CI: 0.25-0.48) (Fig. 2). 91 Noise measurements allowed us to characterise this relationship in terms of estimated received noise 92 levels (Fig. 2). This probability increased from 0.07 (95% CI: 0.03-0.17) to 0.31 (95% CI: 0.16-0.52) 93 for SEL that varied from 165 to 130 dB re 1 μPa2s (Fig. 2). 94 Discussion 95 Short-term responses to this seismic survey did not result in broad scale displacement, suggesting that 96 impact assessments should focus on sub-lethal effects within affected sites [14]. Our results indicate 97 that porpoises remaining in the impact area reduced their buzzing activity by 15% during the seismic 98 survey. Moreover, the probability of detecting buzz ICIs when porpoises were present increased with 99 distance from the source vessel, suggesting that the likelihood of buzzing was dependent upon 100 received noise intensity. The baseline probability of occurrence of buzzes was around 0.4 in the 101 impact block before the survey, although with high natural variability. This probability declined to 102 0.1-0.2 at estimated received SEL of 150-165 dB re 1μPa2s (Fig. 2). Such changes were unlikely to 103 result from the presence of seismic noise, since this was mostly below 400 Hz [14], while most energy 104 in porpoise clicks is within 110-150 kHz [26]. 105 Porpoises may use high-repetition click trains for prey capture or for social communication [27]. 106 Thus, observed changes in buzzing occurrence could reflect disruption of either foraging or social 107 activities. Despite this uncertainty, these data provide a worst-case indication of the extent to which 108 foraging was disrupted by noise exposure. These effects may result from prey reactions to noise [28], 109 leading to reduced porpoise foraging rates. Alternatively, foraging effort may change if porpoises 110 adjust time budgets or diving behaviour to avoid noise. Irrespective of the proximate mechanism(s) 111 through which seismic surveys reduced buzzing rates, this clearly has the potential to affect the 112 energy balance of exposed animals. 113 High metabolic rates [29] mean that porpoises have limited ability to cope with prolonged starvation 114 [30]. Our results provide an estimate of the noise levels at which porpoise activity patterns are 115 disrupted, and an indication of the scale of potential reductions in foraging activity [6]. Porpoise 116 occurrence and activity is typically characterised by large seasonal and diel variability [19,31,32], as 117 also reflected in our results (Fig. 2). Further studies are now required to explore the environmental 118 conditions that drive this variation, and develop energetic models to assess whether this scale of 119 disturbance has long-term consequences for individual energy budgets [33]. 120 Acknowledgements 121 We thank colleagues at Kongsberg Maritime, Moray First Marine, Gardline Geosurvey and the 122 Scottish Fishermen’s Federation for essential support in the field, and those at DECC, PA Resources 123 and Caithness Petroleum Ltd for ensuring that this research was effectively integrated into their 124 regulatory and commercial activities. The project benefitted at all stages from a scientific steering 125 group and broad stakeholder group with statutory and NGO representation. We thank them, and Tim 126 Barton, Keith Needham, Alex Douglas and the many other colleagues who provided support during 127 both field work and data analysis. Thanks to David Lusseau and two anonymous reviewers for their 128 useful comments and suggestions. 129 Data accessibility 130 The data set used in this paper is available in the Dryad data repository: doi:10.5061/dryad.1847s 131 Funding statement 132 Data collection was funded through a partnership project that was co-ordinated by the UK Department 133 of Energy and Climate Change (DECC) and jointly funded by DECC, Scottish Government, Oil & 134 Gas UK Ltd, and COWRIE. Enrico Pirotta was supported through the MASTS pooling initiative (the 135 Marine Alliance for Science and Technology for Scotland). MASTS is funded by the Scottish 136 Funding Council (grant reference HR09011) and contributing institutions. None of the analysis nor 137 write up of the paper involved or was influenced by the sponsors of the research. 138 References 139 1 140 141 Frid, A. & Dill, L. 2002 Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conserv. Ecol. 6, 11. 2 Gill, J. A., Norris, K. & Sutherland, W. J. 2001 Why behavioural responses may not reflect the 142 population consequences of human disturbance. Biol. Conserv. 97, 265–268. 143 (doi:10.1016/S0006-3207(00)00002-1) 144 3 145 146 Beale, C. M. & Monaghan, P. 2004 Behavioural responses to human disturbance: a matter of choice? Anim. Behav. 68, 1065–1069. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.07.002) 4 Bejder, L., Samuels, A., Whitehead, H., Finn, H. & Allen, S. 2009 Impact assessment 147 research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife 148 responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 395, 177–185. 149 (doi:10.3354/meps07979) 150 5 McClung, M. R., Seddon, P. J., Massaro, M. & Setiawan, A. N. 2004 Nature-based tourism 151 impacts on yellow-eyed penguins Megadyptes antipodes: does unregulated visitor access affect 152 fledging weight and juvenile survival? Biol. Conserv. 119, 279–285. 153 (doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2003.11.012) 154 6 155 156 New, L. F. et al. 2013 Modeling the biological significance of behavioral change in coastal bottlenose dolphins in response to disturbance. Funct. Ecol. 27, 314–322. 7 Duchesne, M., Côté, S. D. & Barrette, C. 2000 Responses of woodland caribou to winter 157 ecotourism in the Charlevoix Biosphere Reserve, Canada. Biol. Conserv. 96, 311–317. 158 (doi:10.1016/S0006-3207(00)00082-3) 159 8 160 161 Lusseau, D. 2003 Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: Using Markov chains to model anthropogenic impacts. Conserv. Biol. 17, 1785–1793. 9 Blumstein, D. T. 2006 Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history and 162 natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Anim. Behav. 71, 389–399. 163 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.010) 164 10 165 166 to anthropogenic noise. Mammal Rev. 37, 81–115. 11 167 168 Nowacek, D. P., Thorne, L. H., Johnston, D. W. & Tyack, P. L. 2007 Responses of cetaceans Weilgart, L. S. 2007 The impacts of anthropogenic ocean noise on cetaceans and implications for management. Can. J. Zool. 85, 1091–1116. (doi:10.1139/Z07-101) 12 Brandt, M., Diederichs, A., Betke, K. & Nehls, G. 2011 Responses of harbour porpoises to pile 169 driving at the Horns Rev II offshore wind farm in the Danish North Sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 170 421, 205–216. (doi:10.3354/meps08888) 171 13 172 173 Stone, C. & Tasker, M. 2006 The effects of seismic airguns on cetaceans in UK waters. J. Cetacean. Res. Manag. 8, 255–263. 14 Thompson, P. M., Brookes, K. L., Graham, I. M., Barton, T. R., Needham, K., Bradbury, G. & 174 Merchant, N. D. 2013 Short-term disturbance by a commercial 2-D seismic survey does not 175 lead to long-term displacement of harbour porpoises. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Bio. 280. 176 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.2001) 177 15 Schnitzler, H.-U., Moss, C. F. & Denzinger, A. 2003 From spatial orientation to food 178 acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18, 386–394. (doi:10.1016/S0169- 179 5347(03)00185-X) 180 16 Pirotta, E., Thompson, P. M., Miller, P. I., Brookes, K. L., Cheney, B., Barton, T. R., Graham, 181 I. M. & Lusseau, D. 2014 Scale-dependent foraging ecology of a marine top predator modelled 182 using passive acoustic data. Funct. Ecol. 28, 206–217. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12146) 183 17 Miller, P. J. O., Johnson, M. P. & Tyack, P. L. 2004 Sperm whale behaviour indicates the use 184 of echolocation click buzzes “creaks” in prey capture. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Bio. 271, 2239– 185 2247. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2863) 186 18 Madsen, P. T., Johnson, M., Aguilar Soto, N., Zimmer, W. M. X. & Tyack, P. L. 2005 187 Biosonar performance of foraging beaked whales (Mesoplodon densirostris). J. Exp. Biol. 208, 188 181–194. (doi:10.1242/jeb.01327) 189 19 190 191 Mammal Sci. 21, 1–12. 20 192 193 Thomas, J., Moss, C. & Vater, M. 2004 Echolocation in bats and dolphins. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 21 194 195 Carlström, J. 2005 Diel variation in echolocation behavior of wild harbor porpoises. Mar. Madsen, P. T. & Surlykke, A. 2013 Functional convergence in bat and toothed whale biosonars. Physiology 28, 276–283. (doi:10.1152/physiol.00008.2013) 22 R Development Core Team 2013 R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R 196 Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL 197 http://www.R-project.org/. 198 199 23 Liang, K. Y. & Zeger, S. L. 1986 Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika 73, 13–22. 200 24 201 202 57, 120–125. 25 203 204 26 27 28 Popper, A. N., Fewtrell, J., Smith, M. E. & McCauley, R. D. 2003 Anthropogenic sound: effects on the behavior and physiology of fishes. Mar. Technol. Soc. J. 6, 35–40. 29 211 212 Clausen, K., Wahlberg, M., Beedholm, K., DeRuiter, S. & Madsen, P. T. 2011 Click communication in harbour porpoises Phocoena phocoena. Bioacoustics 20, 1–28. 209 210 Møhl, B. & Andersen, S. 1973 Echolocation: high-frequency component in the click of the Harbour Porpoise (Phocoena ph. L.). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54, 1368. 207 208 Hardin, J. W. & Hilbe, J. M. 2003 Generalized estimating equations. 3rd edn. London: Chapman & Hall/CRC Press. 205 206 Pan, W. 2001 Akaike’s information criterion in generalized estimating equations. Biometrics Lockyer, C. 2007 All creatures great and smaller: a study in cetacean life history energetics. J. Mar. Biol. Ass. U.K. 87, 1035–1045. (doi:10.1017/S0025315407054720) 30 Kastelein, R. A., Hardeman, J. & Boer, H. 1997 Food consumption and body weight of 213 harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). In The biology of harbour porpoise (eds A. Read P. 214 Wiepkema & P. E. Nachtigall), pp. 217–233. Woerden, The Netherlands: De Spil Publishers. 215 31 Verfuß, U. K., Honnef, C. G., Meding, A., Dähne, M., Mundry, R. & Benke, H. 2007 216 Geographical and seasonal variation of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) presence in the 217 German Baltic Sea revealed by passive acoustic monitoring. J. Mar. Biol. Ass. U.K. 87, 165. 218 (doi:10.1017/S0025315407054938) 219 32 Gallus, A., Dähne, M., Verfuß, U., Bräger, S., Adler, S., Siebert, U. & Benke, H. 2012 Use of 220 static passive acoustic monitoring to assess the status of the “Critically Endangered” Baltic 221 harbour porpoise in German waters. Endang. Species Res. 18, 265–278. 222 (doi:10.3354/esr00448) 223 33 Nabe-Nielsen, J., Sibly, R. M., Tougaard, J., Teilmann, J. & Sveegaard, S. 2014 Effects of 224 noise and by-catch on a Danish harbour porpoise population. Ecol. Model. 272, 242–251. 225 (doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2013.09.025) 226 227 Figure 228 229 Figure 1. Map of the study area showing the seismic survey lines and the locations of CPOD 230 deployments. 231 232 Figure 2. Modelling results. (a) Changes in the probability of feeding buzz occurrence in control and 233 impact blocks, before and during the survey. (b) Estimated relationship between the probability of 234 buzz occurrence and distance from the source vessel. (c) Estimated relationship between the 235 probability of buzz occurrence and unweighted sound exposure level (SEL).