Pirotta_et_al_Manuscript_final_withFigs

advertisement
1
Variation in harbour porpoise activity in response to seismic survey noise
2
Enrico Pirotta *,a, Kate L. Brookes b,c, Isla M. Graham b, Paul M. Thompson b
3
4
a
Institute of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen AB24 2TZ,
5
6
UK
b
Lighthouse Field Station, Institute of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of
7
8
Aberdeen, Cromarty IV11 8YL, UK
c
Marine Scotland Science, Marine Laboratory, Aberdeen AB11 9DB, UK
9
10
*Corresponding author: enrico.pirotta@abdn.ac.uk.
11
12
13
14
Short title: Seismic survey affects porpoise activity.
15
Summary
16
Animals exposed to anthropogenic disturbance make trade-offs between perceived risk and the cost of
17
leaving disturbed areas. Impact assessments tend to focus on overt behavioural responses leading to
18
displacement, but trade-offs may also impact individual energy budgets through reduced foraging
19
performance. Previous studies found no evidence for broad–scale displacement of harbour porpoises
20
exposed to impulse noise from a 10-day 2-D seismic survey. Here, we used an array of passive
21
acoustic loggers coupled with calibrated noise measurements to test whether the seismic survey
22
influenced the activity patterns of porpoises remaining in the area. We showed that the probability of
23
recording a buzz declined by 15% in the ensonified area, and was positively related to distance from
24
the source vessel. We also estimated received levels at the hydrophones and characterised the noise
25
response curve. Our results demonstrate how environmental impact assessments can be developed to
26
assess more subtle effects of noise disturbance on activity patterns and foraging efficiency.
27
Keywords: activity budget, anthropogenic disturbance, environmental impact assessment, foraging
28
efficiency
29
30
Introduction
31
Human disturbance may not lead to acute, easily-measurable behavioural responses in exposed
32
wildlife [1,2]. Less overt effects can arise from trade-offs that animals make [3], such as deciding to
33
remain in disturbed areas and tolerating higher exposure levels where prey are abundant [4].
34
However, these trade-offs may also influence activity budgets, potentially affecting fecundity and
35
survival [1,5] or having biologically significant consequences at a population level [6–9].
36
There is particular concern over the potential impacts of underwater anthropogenic noise on marine
37
mammals [10,11]. Aversive behaviour towards intense noise sources such as pile driving (e.g. [12])
38
and seismic surveys [13] may not necessarily lead to broad-scale displacement [14]. However, it is not
39
known whether there are more subtle changes in the activity budgets of animals remaining in exposed
40
areas.
41
Patterns of echolocation clicks can be used to characterise the activity of both bats [15] and
42
odontocetes [16–19]. Here, we use an array of passive acoustic loggers coupled with calibrated noise
43
measurements to assess the effects of a seismic survey on the activity pattern of harbour porpoises
44
(Phocoena phocoena).
45
Material and Methods
46
The study was conducted around a commercial 2-D seismic survey in NE Scotland. The survey vessel
47
used a 470 cu inch airgun array with a shot point interval of 5-6 seconds to survey a 200 km2 area
48
between 1st and 11th September 2011 (see electronic supplementary material). Full details are provided
49
in [14].
50
Patterns of porpoise echolocation clicks were characterised at 22 sites in and around the seismic
51
survey area throughout August and September 2011 (Fig. 1). Data from V.0 and V.1 CPODs
52
(www.chelonia.co.uk) had previously been used to characterise the occurrence of porpoises [14,19].
53
While the sensitivity of C-PODs at different sites could vary between different units or different local
54
conditions, our sampling design and mixed modelling approach were designed to account for this (see
55
electronic supplementary material).
56
We calculated the inter-click intervals (ICI) from the time series of detected clicks. A Gaussian
57
mixture-model was fitted to log-transformed ICIs to identify different patterns of echolocation clicks
58
[16]. Each ICI was classified as either a regular ICI (regular clicking for navigation and prey
59
searching), a buzz ICI (buzzes associated with attempted prey captures or social communication), or
60
an inter-train ICI (pauses between click trains) [20,21] (see electronic supplementary material).
61
Following [14], we used data from CPODs in 25 km by 25 km control and impact areas (Fig. 1; see
62
electronic supplementary material) to assess the effect of seismic surveys on the occurrence of buzz
63
ICIs when porpoises were present (i.e. in hours in which at least one ICI was detected). The presence
64
of buzz ICIs was modelled as a function of seismic period (before or during) and experimental block
65
(control or impact). Crucially, we also evaluated the interaction between these covariates to test
66
whether seismic surveys influenced buzzing activity. We fitted a binary mixed-effects model in R
67
13.01 [22], with location included as a random effect. Model selection was based on AIC and the
68
significance of retained covariates evaluated using Wald’s tests.
69
The presence of buzz ICIs for each minute in which at least one ICI was detected was then modelled
70
as a function of distance from the seismic vessel. Following previous studies [12,14] on likely spatial
71
scales of porpoise responses to noise, we used data from within 25 km of the vessel (see electronic
72
supplementary material). A binary Generalized Linear Model was fitted using Generalized Estimating
73
Equations (GEE-GLM) [23] (see electronic supplementary material). We used QICu for model
74
selection [24], and Wald’s tests to assess significance [25].
75
Calibrated noise measurements were made at 15 sites between 1.6 km and 61.8 km from the seismic
76
vessel [14]. Airgun noise was characterised in terms of broadband sound exposure level for single
77
pulses in dB re 1 μPa2s (using the region of the waveform that contained the central 90% of the
78
pulse’s energy; hereafter SEL). Peak-to-peak source levels were estimated to be 242 –253 dB re 1 μPa
79
at 1 m. We tested whether there was a relationship between buzz occurrence and estimated noise
80
levels at the corresponding CPOD location using a bootstrapping procedure (see electronic
81
supplementary material).
82
Results
83
There was a significant interaction between experimental block and seismic period (Wald’s test:
84
χ2=7.2; df=1; p=0.007), indicating that the seismic survey had a differential effect in the control and
85
impact block. This resulted in a 15% reduction in the occurrence of buzz ICIs in the impact block
86
during the survey (Fig. 2). Wide confidence intervals highlight high levels of natural variability in
87
detection of buzzes.
88
The probability of occurrence of buzzes increased significantly with distance from source (Wald’s
89
test: χ2=9.6; df=1; p=0.002), ranging from approximately 0.15 at 0 km (95% confidence interval (CI):
90
0.11-0.19) to 0.35 at 40 km (95% CI: 0.25-0.48) (Fig. 2).
91
Noise measurements allowed us to characterise this relationship in terms of estimated received noise
92
levels (Fig. 2). This probability increased from 0.07 (95% CI: 0.03-0.17) to 0.31 (95% CI: 0.16-0.52)
93
for SEL that varied from 165 to 130 dB re 1 μPa2s (Fig. 2).
94
Discussion
95
Short-term responses to this seismic survey did not result in broad scale displacement, suggesting that
96
impact assessments should focus on sub-lethal effects within affected sites [14]. Our results indicate
97
that porpoises remaining in the impact area reduced their buzzing activity by 15% during the seismic
98
survey. Moreover, the probability of detecting buzz ICIs when porpoises were present increased with
99
distance from the source vessel, suggesting that the likelihood of buzzing was dependent upon
100
received noise intensity. The baseline probability of occurrence of buzzes was around 0.4 in the
101
impact block before the survey, although with high natural variability. This probability declined to
102
0.1-0.2 at estimated received SEL of 150-165 dB re 1μPa2s (Fig. 2). Such changes were unlikely to
103
result from the presence of seismic noise, since this was mostly below 400 Hz [14], while most energy
104
in porpoise clicks is within 110-150 kHz [26].
105
Porpoises may use high-repetition click trains for prey capture or for social communication [27].
106
Thus, observed changes in buzzing occurrence could reflect disruption of either foraging or social
107
activities. Despite this uncertainty, these data provide a worst-case indication of the extent to which
108
foraging was disrupted by noise exposure. These effects may result from prey reactions to noise [28],
109
leading to reduced porpoise foraging rates. Alternatively, foraging effort may change if porpoises
110
adjust time budgets or diving behaviour to avoid noise. Irrespective of the proximate mechanism(s)
111
through which seismic surveys reduced buzzing rates, this clearly has the potential to affect the
112
energy balance of exposed animals.
113
High metabolic rates [29] mean that porpoises have limited ability to cope with prolonged starvation
114
[30]. Our results provide an estimate of the noise levels at which porpoise activity patterns are
115
disrupted, and an indication of the scale of potential reductions in foraging activity [6]. Porpoise
116
occurrence and activity is typically characterised by large seasonal and diel variability [19,31,32], as
117
also reflected in our results (Fig. 2). Further studies are now required to explore the environmental
118
conditions that drive this variation, and develop energetic models to assess whether this scale of
119
disturbance has long-term consequences for individual energy budgets [33].
120
Acknowledgements
121
We thank colleagues at Kongsberg Maritime, Moray First Marine, Gardline Geosurvey and the
122
Scottish Fishermen’s Federation for essential support in the field, and those at DECC, PA Resources
123
and Caithness Petroleum Ltd for ensuring that this research was effectively integrated into their
124
regulatory and commercial activities. The project benefitted at all stages from a scientific steering
125
group and broad stakeholder group with statutory and NGO representation. We thank them, and Tim
126
Barton, Keith Needham, Alex Douglas and the many other colleagues who provided support during
127
both field work and data analysis. Thanks to David Lusseau and two anonymous reviewers for their
128
useful comments and suggestions.
129
Data accessibility
130
The data set used in this paper is available in the Dryad data repository: doi:10.5061/dryad.1847s
131
Funding statement
132
Data collection was funded through a partnership project that was co-ordinated by the UK Department
133
of Energy and Climate Change (DECC) and jointly funded by DECC, Scottish Government, Oil &
134
Gas UK Ltd, and COWRIE. Enrico Pirotta was supported through the MASTS pooling initiative (the
135
Marine Alliance for Science and Technology for Scotland). MASTS is funded by the Scottish
136
Funding Council (grant reference HR09011) and contributing institutions. None of the analysis nor
137
write up of the paper involved or was influenced by the sponsors of the research.
138
References
139
1
140
141
Frid, A. & Dill, L. 2002 Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk.
Conserv. Ecol. 6, 11.
2
Gill, J. A., Norris, K. & Sutherland, W. J. 2001 Why behavioural responses may not reflect the
142
population consequences of human disturbance. Biol. Conserv. 97, 265–268.
143
(doi:10.1016/S0006-3207(00)00002-1)
144
3
145
146
Beale, C. M. & Monaghan, P. 2004 Behavioural responses to human disturbance: a matter of
choice? Anim. Behav. 68, 1065–1069. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.07.002)
4
Bejder, L., Samuels, A., Whitehead, H., Finn, H. & Allen, S. 2009 Impact assessment
147
research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife
148
responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 395, 177–185.
149
(doi:10.3354/meps07979)
150
5
McClung, M. R., Seddon, P. J., Massaro, M. & Setiawan, A. N. 2004 Nature-based tourism
151
impacts on yellow-eyed penguins Megadyptes antipodes: does unregulated visitor access affect
152
fledging weight and juvenile survival? Biol. Conserv. 119, 279–285.
153
(doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2003.11.012)
154
6
155
156
New, L. F. et al. 2013 Modeling the biological significance of behavioral change in coastal
bottlenose dolphins in response to disturbance. Funct. Ecol. 27, 314–322.
7
Duchesne, M., Côté, S. D. & Barrette, C. 2000 Responses of woodland caribou to winter
157
ecotourism in the Charlevoix Biosphere Reserve, Canada. Biol. Conserv. 96, 311–317.
158
(doi:10.1016/S0006-3207(00)00082-3)
159
8
160
161
Lusseau, D. 2003 Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: Using Markov
chains to model anthropogenic impacts. Conserv. Biol. 17, 1785–1793.
9
Blumstein, D. T. 2006 Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history and
162
natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Anim. Behav. 71, 389–399.
163
(doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.010)
164
10
165
166
to anthropogenic noise. Mammal Rev. 37, 81–115.
11
167
168
Nowacek, D. P., Thorne, L. H., Johnston, D. W. & Tyack, P. L. 2007 Responses of cetaceans
Weilgart, L. S. 2007 The impacts of anthropogenic ocean noise on cetaceans and implications
for management. Can. J. Zool. 85, 1091–1116. (doi:10.1139/Z07-101)
12
Brandt, M., Diederichs, A., Betke, K. & Nehls, G. 2011 Responses of harbour porpoises to pile
169
driving at the Horns Rev II offshore wind farm in the Danish North Sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
170
421, 205–216. (doi:10.3354/meps08888)
171
13
172
173
Stone, C. & Tasker, M. 2006 The effects of seismic airguns on cetaceans in UK waters. J.
Cetacean. Res. Manag. 8, 255–263.
14
Thompson, P. M., Brookes, K. L., Graham, I. M., Barton, T. R., Needham, K., Bradbury, G. &
174
Merchant, N. D. 2013 Short-term disturbance by a commercial 2-D seismic survey does not
175
lead to long-term displacement of harbour porpoises. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Bio. 280.
176
(doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.2001)
177
15
Schnitzler, H.-U., Moss, C. F. & Denzinger, A. 2003 From spatial orientation to food
178
acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18, 386–394. (doi:10.1016/S0169-
179
5347(03)00185-X)
180
16
Pirotta, E., Thompson, P. M., Miller, P. I., Brookes, K. L., Cheney, B., Barton, T. R., Graham,
181
I. M. & Lusseau, D. 2014 Scale-dependent foraging ecology of a marine top predator modelled
182
using passive acoustic data. Funct. Ecol. 28, 206–217. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12146)
183
17
Miller, P. J. O., Johnson, M. P. & Tyack, P. L. 2004 Sperm whale behaviour indicates the use
184
of echolocation click buzzes “creaks” in prey capture. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Bio. 271, 2239–
185
2247. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2863)
186
18
Madsen, P. T., Johnson, M., Aguilar Soto, N., Zimmer, W. M. X. & Tyack, P. L. 2005
187
Biosonar performance of foraging beaked whales (Mesoplodon densirostris). J. Exp. Biol. 208,
188
181–194. (doi:10.1242/jeb.01327)
189
19
190
191
Mammal Sci. 21, 1–12.
20
192
193
Thomas, J., Moss, C. & Vater, M. 2004 Echolocation in bats and dolphins. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
21
194
195
Carlström, J. 2005 Diel variation in echolocation behavior of wild harbor porpoises. Mar.
Madsen, P. T. & Surlykke, A. 2013 Functional convergence in bat and toothed whale
biosonars. Physiology 28, 276–283. (doi:10.1152/physiol.00008.2013)
22
R Development Core Team 2013 R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R
196
Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL
197
http://www.R-project.org/.
198
199
23
Liang, K. Y. & Zeger, S. L. 1986 Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models.
Biometrika 73, 13–22.
200
24
201
202
57, 120–125.
25
203
204
26
27
28
Popper, A. N., Fewtrell, J., Smith, M. E. & McCauley, R. D. 2003 Anthropogenic sound:
effects on the behavior and physiology of fishes. Mar. Technol. Soc. J. 6, 35–40.
29
211
212
Clausen, K., Wahlberg, M., Beedholm, K., DeRuiter, S. & Madsen, P. T. 2011 Click
communication in harbour porpoises Phocoena phocoena. Bioacoustics 20, 1–28.
209
210
Møhl, B. & Andersen, S. 1973 Echolocation: high-frequency component in the click of the
Harbour Porpoise (Phocoena ph. L.). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54, 1368.
207
208
Hardin, J. W. & Hilbe, J. M. 2003 Generalized estimating equations. 3rd edn. London:
Chapman & Hall/CRC Press.
205
206
Pan, W. 2001 Akaike’s information criterion in generalized estimating equations. Biometrics
Lockyer, C. 2007 All creatures great and smaller: a study in cetacean life history energetics. J.
Mar. Biol. Ass. U.K. 87, 1035–1045. (doi:10.1017/S0025315407054720)
30
Kastelein, R. A., Hardeman, J. & Boer, H. 1997 Food consumption and body weight of
213
harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). In The biology of harbour porpoise (eds A. Read P.
214
Wiepkema & P. E. Nachtigall), pp. 217–233. Woerden, The Netherlands: De Spil Publishers.
215
31
Verfuß, U. K., Honnef, C. G., Meding, A., Dähne, M., Mundry, R. & Benke, H. 2007
216
Geographical and seasonal variation of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) presence in the
217
German Baltic Sea revealed by passive acoustic monitoring. J. Mar. Biol. Ass. U.K. 87, 165.
218
(doi:10.1017/S0025315407054938)
219
32
Gallus, A., Dähne, M., Verfuß, U., Bräger, S., Adler, S., Siebert, U. & Benke, H. 2012 Use of
220
static passive acoustic monitoring to assess the status of the “Critically Endangered” Baltic
221
harbour porpoise in German waters. Endang. Species Res. 18, 265–278.
222
(doi:10.3354/esr00448)
223
33
Nabe-Nielsen, J., Sibly, R. M., Tougaard, J., Teilmann, J. & Sveegaard, S. 2014 Effects of
224
noise and by-catch on a Danish harbour porpoise population. Ecol. Model. 272, 242–251.
225
(doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2013.09.025)
226
227
Figure
228
229
Figure 1. Map of the study area showing the seismic survey lines and the locations of CPOD
230
deployments.
231
232
Figure 2. Modelling results. (a) Changes in the probability of feeding buzz occurrence in control and
233
impact blocks, before and during the survey. (b) Estimated relationship between the probability of
234
buzz occurrence and distance from the source vessel. (c) Estimated relationship between the
235
probability of buzz occurrence and unweighted sound exposure level (SEL).
Download