The assessment of animal welfare Paul H. Hemsworth Animal Welfare Science Centre, The Melbourne School of Land and Environment, The University of Melbourne, and the Department of Primary Industries (Victoria), Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia. In an ethical analysis of an animal use, science can provide the factual basis of understanding the impact of a husbandry or housing practice on the animal, particularly its impact on the welfare of the animal. However, there is considerable uncertainty within science on the concept of animal welfare. Although there is a wide acceptance of the scientific method in problem solving, its ability to contribute to the welfare debate has been limited to some extent by a lack of consensus on the scientific approach to studying animal welfare. For many scientists, animal welfare is defined and measured on the basis of how well the animal is performing from a biological functioning perspective. For others, animal welfare concerns affective states, such as suffering, pain, and other feelings or emotions, and thus animal welfare can be studied by measuring animal preference on the basis that preferences are influenced by the animal’s emotions, which have evolved to motivate behaviour in order to avoid harm and facilitate survival, growth, and reproduction. One concept in the literature, albeit not well enunciated, is predicated on the view that the welfare of animals is improved in environments or situations in which the animals display normal or “natural” behaviour. These different concepts or views on animal welfare lead to the different methodologies to assess an animal’s welfare. This scientific uncertainty does not necessarily diminish the robustness of the research utilising methodologies or measurements arising from these views or concepts, but it does raise the question of the relatedness of these concepts. In other words, is biological dysfunction associated with, or does it lead to, negative affective states and vice versa? In a similar context, is an inability to perform normal or ‘natural’ behaviours associated with biological dysfunction and/or negative affective states? Therefore, are the resultant methodologies measuring the same adverse physiological and mental state(s) in the animal? There are several commonalities in the rationale for these approaches. For example, it is considered that animals, at least in the wild, will be motivated to choose those resources or behaviours that maintain homeostasis or biological functioning to optimize their fitness; that is, optimize their growth, reproduction, injury status, health, and survival. Furthermore, feelings or subjective affective states have evolved to motivate behaviour to meet needs that have to be satisfied in order for the organism to grow and reproduce normally and to remain healthy and survive. The functional approach to welfare assessment includes both behavioural and physiological indicators of poor coping, as well as fitness variables such as growth, reproduction, injury and health. This conceptual convergence suggests a way forward in developing a broader consensus on the study of animal welfare by reducing both conceptual differences and consequently methodological differences in animal welfare science. The validity of the welfare criteria can be tested in several ways: first, with the finding that there are correlations between independent measures of different concepts of animal welfare; and second, with the finding that an intuitively aversive condition reduces animal welfare on the basis of the measures of different concepts of animal welfare. Therefore, the basis of the methodology used by scientists to assess animal welfare should routinely be provided so that individuals using science in their decision-making appreciate both the rationale for the methodology and its limitations. Research utilising wellaccepted stress models is required to understand the relationships between these concepts and methodologies, with the overall objective of developing a broader consensus on animal welfare methodologies to reduce the methodological and interpretative differences in animal welfare science. In the meantime, the approaches or methodologies that arise from the welfare concepts of normal biological functioning and affective states should guide current welfare research methodology. Indeed, while the general public may appreciate that longterm behavioural responses such as stereotypies and chronic stress are ‘harmful’, there is merit in understanding what animals prefer since as some scientists have noted, “we all know which part (approach) will be the most powerful argument for the audience (general public)”.