How Many Is a Zillion? Sources of Number Distortion Lance J. Rips Northwestern University Send correspondence to: Lance Rips Psychology Department Northwestern University 2029 Sheridan Road Evanston, IL 60208 Email: rips@northwestern.edu Phone: 847-491-5947 Keywords: Number concepts, Numerical reasoning, Representations of mathematics How Many Is a Zillion? / 2 Abstract When young children attempt to locate the positions of numerals on a number line, the positions are often logarithmically rather than linearly distributed. This finding has been taken as evidence that the children represent numbers on a mental number line that is logarithmically calibrated. This article reports a statistical simulation showing that log-like positioning is a consequence of two factors: the bounded nature of the number line and greater uncertainty about the meaning of the larger, less frequent number words. Two experiments likewise show that even college students produce log-like placements under the same two conditions. In Experiment 1, participants identified positions on a number line for a set that included both conventional and fictitious numbers (e.g., a zillion). In Experiment 2, participants did the same for conventional numbers that included some larger, unfamiliar items (e.g., a nonillion). Both experiments produced results better fit by logarithmic than by linear functions. How Many Is a Zillion? / 3 Our most primitive indicator of numerical information may be a continuous mental quantity that varies directly with the number of objects we perceive. This approximate number system (or analog magnitude system) renders larger numbers of entities as larger continuous amounts: The larger the number of perceived objects, the larger the amount of activation (e.g., Dehaene, 1997; Gallistel & Gelman, 1992; Wynn, 1992). Thus, the amount of activation can serve as an approximate measure of the cardinality of a set. The approximate number system can detect the number of objects in a visual array, the number of tones in a sequence, and the number of jumps of a puppet (see Feigenson, 2007, for a review). Evidence from many tasks suggests that this system is at work in animals, infants, and adults (Gallistel, Gelman, & Cordes, 2006). Studies of the approximate number system agree that the system is differentially sensitive to small numbers of objects. For example, the system represents the difference between 4 objects and 5 as larger than the difference between 14 objects and 15. The system appears to follow a ratio principle: For example, humans and animals perform similarly in discriminating 5 objects from 10 as in discriminating 10 objects from 20. One way to understand this pattern is to view the system as transducing the number of entities into an internal measure approximately equal to its logarithm. Because log(10) – log(5) = log(20) – log(10), the logarithmic transformation correctly predicts the data on equal ratios, in accord with classical Fechnerian psychophysics (e.g., Marks & Algom, 1998). Results supporting a logarithmic transformation come from experiments in which American or European children and Amazon natives (both children and adults) are asked to locate the position of specific numerals on a number line, such as Figure 1a (for children, see Berteletti, Lucangeli, Piazza, Dehaene, & Zorzi, 2010; Booth & Siegler, 2006; Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Byrd-Craven, 2008; Opfer & Siegler, 2007; Siegler & Booth, 2004; Siegler & Opfer, 2003; for Amazonians, see Dehaene, Izard, Spelke, & Pica, 2008). On each trial, participants see a bare number line, with only the end points labeled, for example, at 0 and 100. Participants also see a numeral, such as 73, and they must mark its position on How Many Is a Zillion? / 4 the line. A graph of the observed positions of participants’ marks against their true positions often produces a distribution that is better fit by a logarithmic than by a linear function. American children show an approximately logarithmic function for the range 0-100 until second grade, when the functions become more nearly linear. Even second graders, however, produce log-like functions for the range 0-1000, attaining linear spacing only in fourth to sixth grade (Booth & Siegler, 2006; Opfer & Siegler, 2007; Siegler & Opfer, 2003). The function for Amazon natives is log-like even for the 0-10 range (Dehaene et al., 2008). Controversy surrounds the exact shape of the function relating estimated position to true position on the number line (Barth & Palladino, 2011; Cohen & Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011; Ebersbach, Luwel, Frick, Onghena, & Verschaffel, 2008; Landy, Silbert, & Goldin, in press; Moeller, Pixner, Kaufmann, & Nuerk, 2009). (See Barth, Slusser, Cohen, & Paladino, 2011, and Opfer, Siegler, & Young, 2011, for debate on this issue). However, no one doubts the descriptive result that young children’s placement exhibits compression with respect to the larger numbers. The present paper examines factors that could contribute to this compression. In what follows, I will continue to use log-like to refer to such distributions, with the understanding that other, non-logarithmic functions could account for them. Two Assumptions about Number Lines An alternative account of the number-line data begins with the idea that younger children are more uncertain about the meaning of the larger numbers in the tested range. Kindergarteners, for example, may be more familiar with the meanings of numerals near 0 than with those near 100, since the numerals’ frequency of occurrence decreases with their size (Dehaene & Mehler, 1992). Lack of familiarity may translate into greater uncertainty about the position of the numerals on the number line. Increasing variability with larger numerals is a hallmark of some theories of the approximate number system (e.g., Gallistel et al., 2006; Gibbon, Church, & Meck, 1984), and these models present an alternative to the view that people represent cardinality on an internally compressed log-like scale. Cantlon, Cordes, Libertus, and Brannon (2009) and Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer (2011) have pointed out that these How Many Is a Zillion? / 5 alternative systems can also help explain the number-line placement results. For present purposes, however, we need not commit to an internal representation with increasing variability. All that is essential is that children’s unfamiliarity with the meaning of the larger numerals can lead to greater variability in their positioning of these numerals (see Matthews & Chesney, 2011). One further fact is helpful in explaining the number-line data. Because the physical number line is bounded (e.g., at 0 and 100 in Figure 1a), children are constrained to place the tested numbers within these limits. Thus, the distribution of possible positions for a numeral is truncated by the ends of the number line (Cohen & Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011). This truncation can lead to estimates of the numeral’s position that are too close to the center (see Huttenlocher, Hedges, & Prohaska, 1988, for a similar model of other bounded estimation tasks). A Statistical Simulation of Number-Line Placement The unfamiliarity of larger numbers and the boundedness of the number line suffice to explain the log-like placement of the numerals. Figure 1b shows the results of a statistical simulation that incorporates these assumptions. The individual distributions in the figure represent the possible placements of the numerals one to nine on a number line that extends from 0 to 10. (This smaller range is adopted for simplicity.) Each distribution was generated from a Gaussian whose mean was exactly equal to the corresponding number. For example, the right-most distribution comes from a Gaussian with mean 9. The standard deviations of these distributions, however, increase in proportion to the number. This increase captures the idea that the larger numerals are associated with greater uncertainty. The individual distributions are also truncated at 0 and 10: If a sampled number fell outside this range, it was discarded and replaced with a new random number from the same underlying distribution. The black vertical lines in Figure 1b show the means of the resulting truncated distributions. Although the underlying distributions have true means at 1, 2, 3, …, 9, the means of the corresponding truncated distributions are compressed at the top. For example, the difference between the means for the truncated distributions for one and two is almost exactly 1, but the difference between the means for eight How Many Is a Zillion? / 6 and nine is only 0.46. Similarly, the mean for the numeral one is at position 1.00 on the x-axis, but the mean for nine is shifted to 8.14. Thus, the mean positions of the truncated distributions show log-like spacing similar to children’s positioning of the numerals in the experiments cited earlier. Two Experimental Simulations of Number Line Placement College students know that the natural numbers have a linear structure: They know, at least implicitly, that the naturals have a unique first number, 0, and that each of the remaining numbers is generated from the immediately preceding one by adding one (see Leslie, Gelman, & Gallistel, 2008; Rips, Bloomfield, & Asmuth, 2008). They therefore know that the difference between successive numbers is linearly spaced and not logarithmically spaced. College students may retain an analog magnitude system and display evidence for such a system in speeded judgments and estimation tasks (e.g., Moyer & Landauer, 1967). But they would be unable to arrive at correct answers to even elementary arithmetic problems (e.g., 5 – 4 = 15 – ?) unless they understood the correct spacing of the numbers. Despite their knowledge of natural numbers, college student may yet show log-like placement of numerals on a number line if they are unfamiliar with the larger items in the range. This prediction follows from the same two assumptions embodied in the simulation of Figure 1b: First, the larger unfamiliar numerals should be associated with greater uncertainty in their positions on the number line. And, second, truncation at the ends of the line skews the resulting distributions. The two experiments in this article test this prediction by asking college students to place numerals on a number line, following the procedure of the developmental studies cited earlier. The present experiments, however, include numerals whose values are unknown or unfamiliar to adults. Experiment 1: From Zero to a Gazillion Participants’ task in this study was to place a series of 20 numerals on number lines. The task followed the procedure of earlier number-line placement experiments, but with a novel set of numerals. Nine of the numerals, items a-i in Table 1, are conventional numbers, including some very large ones. How Many Is a Zillion? / 7 The remaining numerals (items j-t in Table 1) were fictitious, such as forty-three umptillion and twentyfive, that are sometimes used to express large, arbitrary amounts. For each numeral, participants marked the position on a number line where they thought it should go. The line itself resembled the one in Figure 1a, but with the upper end labeled “One Gazillion” to accommodate the fictitious numerals. Method Procedure. Each participant received a booklet containing a page of instructions followed by 20 pages of number lines. The instructions showed how to put a mark on the line and then continued: Although some of the numbers will be ordinary whole numbers …, other numbers will be fictitious. For example, you may be asked about numbers like “three bagillion” that aren’t among the usual integers. For these numbers, please do your best to decide where the numbers should go on the line if they were true numbers. On each of the next 20 booklet pages, one of the numerals from Table 1 appeared, centered above a blank number line (108 mm in length), whose ends were labeled “Zero” and “One Gazillion.” Participants marked the position of the numeral on the line with pen or pencil. The Table 1 numerals appeared in the booklet in a new random order for each participant. (All numerals appeared in the natural-language form of Table 1, since there is no mathematical notation for the fictitious numbers.) Materials. All the stimulus numerals except for three have the form: x-y-number word-z-w (e.g., twenty-three thousand forty-five). The numbers x-y and z-w were both chosen randomly from a uniform distribution of the integers 10 to 99. The intent was to keep the items approximately constant in length, and about equal in the precision they convey. The conventional numerals followed the American English system (see the dictionary entry for “Number,” 1961). The fictitious number words came from a Wikipedia article on “Indefinite and Fictitious Numbers” (2012) and are attested in novels and other sources (citations to these sources appear in the article). How Many Is a Zillion? / 8 Participants. Twenty-three college students participated in this study as part of a requirement for an introductory psychology course. Two had high school precalculus or statistics as their highest-level math course; the rest had taken calculus in either high school or college. Results and Discussion Participants should be uncertain about the positions of the fictitious numerals and the larger conventional numerals. If uncertainty can produce compression in the placement of these numerals, we should observe this compression in the data. Figure 2a plots the mean position of the numerals on the number line and shows this is the case. The points indicate the numerals, with labels corresponding to those in Table 1. In seven cases, participants skipped a page or marked a position off the number line. These instances are treated as missing data. The first six conventional numbers (points a-f in Figure 2a) are correctly ordered in the mean data, relative to each other. However, the ordinal positions of sixty quintillion and ten and eighteen sextillion and forty-five (points h and i) are reversed, and several fictitious numbers precede these in the sequence. These two numerals, along with the fictitious items, are also associated with large standard errors, in accord with their relative unfamiliarity. Of course, we can’t plot the observed position of these numerals against their true positions, since the fictitious numbers have no true value. Still, we can see whether a linear or a logarithmic function best predicts the observed values, just as we could ask whether the position of beads on a string are linearly or logarithmically spaced. If oi is the obtained ordinal position of numeral i, and di is its metric distance along the number line, the question is whether Equation (1a) or (1b) yields better predictions: (1) a. di = m + boi b. di = m + b log oi Linear and logarithmic regression evaluated these equations, and both models significantly predicted the data (for the best-fitting linear model, F(1,18) = 128.36, p < .001, R2 = .88; for the best-fitting logarithmic model, F(1,18) = 307.29, p < .001, R2 = .94). The logarithmic model, however, provided a better fit than How Many Is a Zillion? / 9 the linear one, as appears in Figure 2b. A comparison of the absolute values of the residuals from these models showed smaller deviations for the log model, t(19) = 3.03, p = .007, d = .957. Much the same conclusion about the superiority of the logarithmic function holds, even if we restrict attention to the conventional numbers. Figure 2a shows that these items (points a-i) are roughly equally spaced on the number line, with the exceptions of b and c and the reversal of h and i, mentioned earlier. For example, the obtained distance between 28,032 (point c) and 75,000,035 (point d) is about the same as the latter is from 29,000,000,091 (point e). However, the true numeric difference between the second pair is about 400 times the difference between the first pair! To examine this effect, we can fit logarithmic and linear functions to the means from the nine conventional numbers, using equations like (1a) and (1b), but replacing the ordinal values oi with the true numeric values. The linear model this time failed to produce a significant effect (F(1,7) = 1.58, p = .24, R2 = .18), but the logarithmic model did (F(1,7) = 197.87, p < .001, R2 = .96). This finding coincides with recent results by Landy et al. (in press). In that study, college students located the position of numbers on a number line whose ends were labeled “1 thousand” and “1 billion.” A substantial percentage of participants (about 35%) positioned numbers near one million at about the midpoint of the scale, despite the fact that the difference between one billion and one million is 1000 times greater than the difference between one million and one thousand. These participants apparently believed that one thousand, one million, and one billion are about equally spaced on the number line. Note, though, that the distribution of the means for the conventional numbers appears to be loglike, even when these means are plotted against their ordinal positions, as in Figure 2b. If participants knew the true ordering of these numbers (and took them to be less than the fictitious numbers), then the means should instead plot as a straight line in the figure. This is true even if participants incorrectly believed the numbers were equally spaced. Figure 2a also shows that the mean positions of these items were not especially close to the upper endpoint. So truncation may not suffice to explain their log-like shape. Although several reasons for this finding are possible, one simple explanation is that participants sometimes used the middle of the number line as a kind of “don’t know” response when dealing with big How Many Is a Zillion? / 10 conventional numbers like eighteen sextillion and forty-five, about whose value they were uncertain. (For similar tendencies with other response scales, see Schwarz & Hippler, 1987.) Except for three, each numeral had the form x-y-number word-z-w (e.g., seventy-five million and thirty-five). Although these numbers were randomly assigned, the prefix number (e.g., seventy-five) or the suffix number (e.g., thirty-five) might have affected the positioning of the numerals. However, correlations of these values with the mean position on the number line were slightly negative and nonsignificant (for the prefixes, r(17) = -.11, and for the suffixes, r(17) = -.23, p > .10 for both). Experiment 2: From Zero to a Google The results from Experiment 1 show that college students place numerals in a log-like pattern on a number line if the larger numbers are unfamiliar. That experiment ensured unfamiliarity by using fictitious numbers, but the same prediction should emerge if the numbers are conventional but sufficiently large. The present study repeats the procedure of Experiment 1, but replaces the fictitious numbers with conventional ones, such as eleven decillion and seventy-one, which the participants have probably not often encountered. One advantage is that each numeral has a true value, so we can assess observed versus correct positions over the full range of stimuli. Table 2 lists the stimulus numerals, along with their numeric values. Participants placed each numeral on a number line that was labeled with zero on the left and one google on the right. The expectation is that participants’ placement of these numerals will continue to display log-like structure. Method The procedure in this experiment duplicated that of Experiment 1. The only change concerned the materials. First, the number word stimuli were the natural-language terms in Table 2 (see the dictionary entry for “Number,” 1961). Second, the number lines were anchored at zero and one google. One google (= 10100) was taken as the upper anchor because it is larger than the Table 2 numbers but not morphologically related to them. Of course, uncertainty about the meaning of the anchor, as well as How Many Is a Zillion? / 11 uncertainty about the meaning of the numerals, can contribute to log-like placement (Matthews & Chesney, 2011). The line itself was 118 mm. Participants were 25 college students, all of whom had taken at least a high school calculus class. One participant made a mark that fell off the number line, and this point is treated as missing in the analyses. Results and Discussion The number placements from this study show much the same crowding at the top as in the first experiment. The mean of the first seven numerals are in correct order, but the remaining numerals are compressed and often incorrectly sequenced. Figure 3a plots these trends, with labels corresponding to the items in Table 2. As in Experiment 1, the standard errors of the means typically increase from left to right, suggesting greater uncertainty about the positions of the larger numbers. Participants may have based some of their decisions on the English word forms, since the numerals containing quintillion and quindecillion and those containing sextillion and sexdecillion appear (incorrectly) in adjacent positions. However, this was not always the case: Septillion and septdecillion are not especially close. The main question about these data is whether a linear or a logarithmic function gives a better account of the numerals’ spacing. Regressions based on Equations (1a) and (1b) show that both functions significantly predict the means, but that the log function produces the better fit. For the linear function, F(1,18) = 74.52, p < .001, R2 = .80. For the log function, F(1,18) = 323.65, p < .001, R2 = .95. A paired ttest of the absolute values of the residuals shows smaller deviations from the logarithmic model, t(19) = 5.85, p < .001, d = 1.85. Figure 3b displays the predicted values from the two equations, and the results appear quite similar to those from Experiment 1, despite the new, conventional values for the larger numbers. We can also examine the relative accuracy of linear and log equations based on the numerals’ true numeric values, this time using all the stimulus items. As in Experiment 1, the linear model does not significantly fit the data (F(1,18) = 1.17, p = .29, R2 = .06), but the log model is highly significant (F(1,18) = 44.14, p < .001, R2 = .71). How Many Is a Zillion? / 12 General Discussion Despite the fact that college students know the structure of the natural numbers, their placement of numerals on a number line is not always linear. When the set of numerals includes both large items and fictitious ones, as in Experiment 1, they assigned positions to the numbers that are better fit by a log than by a linear function. Similarly, when all the stimulus numerals are conventional, but some are very large, placement was again log-like rather than linear. These conclusions hold whether the functions are based on the obtained ordinal positions of the numerals or on the numerals’ true values. The data thus agree with the statistical simulation: Uncertainty about the meaning of unfamiliar numerals, together with a bounded scale, can produce log-like positioning on a number line. These statistical and experimental findings imply that the log-like placements aren’t conclusive evidence for an internal, logarithmically-calibrated representation of the natural numbers. They therefore provide a skeptical challenge to conclusions from earlier studies. However, a proponent of log-calibration might question the basis of this challenge. Although college students know the organization of the numbers, they may nevertheless resort to a logarithmic representation when faced with unfamiliar numerals, such as those in the present experiments. Under speeded conditions, adults show size effects in comparing the values of single-digit numbers (e.g., Moyer & Landauer, 1967)—for example, they are faster at deciding that 3 is less than 4 than that 7 is less than 8. These results have been taken to support the existence of an approximate number system that coexists with the linear representation people need for official arithmetic. College students might likewise fall back on a logarithmic representation because of their lack of knowledge of the numbers. The present results, however, do not question the idea that people have both an approximate number system and an official arithmetic system. The issue is whether the number-line placement data provide evidence for a log-calibrated internal representation (one particular instantiation of the approximate system). What the simulations highlight are other ways to explain the same data (see, also, Cantlon et al., 2009; Cohen & Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011; Landy et al., in press; Matthews & Chesney, How Many Is a Zillion? / 13 2011). Moreover, fictitious numbers presumably don’t have positions on an internal line, and the same may well be true of rarely encountered conventional numbers. Participants might temporarily assign these numbers to points at the high end of an internal logarithmic line, but it is unclear how or why they would do so. A stronger argument in favor of a mental log-calibrated number line might show that the conditions leading to log-like functions in the simulations don’t apply to previous studies. One of these conditions—the boundedness of the number line—is built in to such experiments, but the other condition—greater uncertainty associated with the position of the larger numerals—is more vulnerable. Variability clearly increased in the present experiments (see Figures 2a and 3a), but other studies may not exhibit this relation. Most published studies do not report variability as a function of the size of the number, but Siegler and Booth (2004) note that variability failed to correlate with the size of numerals between 0 and 100. Caution is required, however, in interpreting this finding (beyond the usual caution associated with null results). The statistical simulation reported earlier assumes increasing variability in the underlying distributions of the numbers, but this increase need not translate directly into increasing variability in the observed truncated distributions. The boundaries of the number line impose restrictions on variability, just as they impose restrictions on the means. Thus, if the standard deviations of the underlying distributions increase rapidly, the resulting standard deviations of the truncated distributions quickly reach an asymptote, resulting in minimal change. It is possible to find values of the distributions’ parameters that produce both log-like positions of the means and quite small increases in standard deviations (e.g., less than 12% over the range 1-9 in simulations like those reported earlier). The effect of truncation also predicts that if participants received an unbounded number line—one in which no upper bound exists—increasing standard deviations would reappear. This prediction is confirmed by Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer (2011). In sum, statistical and experimental simulations suggest that people’s logarithmic placement of numerals is not necessarily the result of a log-calibrated mental representation. It could instead reflect How Many Is a Zillion? / 14 uncertainty about the meanings of the larger number words, together with limits imposed by the boundaries of the number line. This suffices to explain children’s shift from a log to a linear pattern as they learn the meanings of the larger words. These assumptions also explain how the same child could show a linear pattern for 0-100 but a log pattern for 0-1000, since the child may know the meanings for the larger numbers in the former range but not in the latter. The central goal for research in this area is to determine what people know about the organization of the integers. Particular tasks, such as number-line placement, may be helpful in this respect, but we need to inspect carefully the bridge they provide between data and theory. How Many Is a Zillion? / 15 Acknowledgments Thanks to Julie Booth, David Landy, Robert Siegler, members of my lab group, and audiences at New York University, Northwestern University, and the University of Notre Dame for their feedback. For their help with the experiments, I thank Samantha Thompson and Antonia Yang. This research was supported by IES grant R305A080341. How Many Is a Zillion? / 16 References Barth, H. C., & Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a representational shift. Developmental Science, 14, 125-135. doi: 10.1111/j.14677687.2010.00962.x Barth, H., Slusser, E., Cohen, D., & Paladino, A. (2011). A sense of proportion: Commentary on Opfer, Siegler and Young. Developmental Science, 14, 1205-1206. Berteletti, I., Lucangeli, D., Piazza, M., Dehaene, S., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Numerical estimation in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 46, 545-551. doi: 10.1037/a0017887 Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 42, 189-201. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.189 Cantlon, J. F., Cordes, S., Libertus, M. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Comment on "Log or Linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in western and amazonian indigene cultures". Science, 323, 38. doi: 10.1126/science.1164773 Cohen, D. J., & Blanc-Goldhammer, D. (2011). Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 331-338. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0059-z Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind constructs mathematics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Spelke, E., & Pica, P. (2008). Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science, 320, 1217-1220. doi: 10.1126/science.1156540 Dehaene, S., & Mehler, J. (1992). Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words. Cognition, 43, 1-29. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90030-l Ebersbach, M., Luwel, K., Frick, A., Onghena, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2008). The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year old children: How Many Is a Zillion? / 17 Evidence for a segmented linear model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 1-17. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.006 Feigenson, L. (2007). The equality of quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 185-187. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.006 Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 4374. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90050-r Gallistel, C. R., Gelman, R., & Cordes, S. (2006). The cultural and evolutionary history of the real numbers. In S. C. Levinson & J. Pierre (Eds.), Evolution and culture: A Fyssen Foundation symposium. (pp. 247-274). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2008). Development of number line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 277-299. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982361 Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52-77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L., & Prohaska, V. (1988). Hierarchical organization in ordered domains: Estimating the dates of events. Psychological Review, 95, 471-484. doi: 10.1037/0033295x.95.4.471 Indefinite and fictitious numbers (2012). In Wikipedia: The free encyclopedia. Retrieved from http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Indefinite_and_fictitious_numbers Landy, D., Silbert, N., & Goldin, A. (in press). Estimating large numbers. Cognitive Science. Leslie, A. M., Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (2008). The generative basis of natural number concepts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 213-218. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.004 Marks, L. E., & Algom, D. (1998). Psychophysical scaling. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Measurement, judgment, and decision making (pp. 81-178). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Matthews, P. G., & Chesney, D. (2011). Straightening up: Number line estimates shift from log to linear with additional information. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher & T. F. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of How Many Is a Zillion? / 18 the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1936-1941). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2009). Children's early mental number line: Logarithmic or decomposed linear? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 503-515. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.006 Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519-1520. doi: 10.1038/2151519a0 Number (1961). In P. B. Gove (Ed.), Webster’s third new international dictionary of the English language (p. 1549). Springfield, MA: Merriam. Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2007). Representational change and children's numerical estimation. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 169-195. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.09.002 Opfer, J. E., Siegler, R. S., & Young, C. J. (2011). The powers of noise-fitting: Reply to Barth and Paladino. Developmental Science, 14, 1194-1204. Rips, L. J., Bloomfield, A., & Asmuth, J. (2008). From numerical concepts to concepts of number. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 623-642. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x08005566 Schwarz, N., & Hippler, H.-J. (1987). What response scales may tell your respondents: Informative functions of response alternatives. In H.-J. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social information processing and survey methodology (pp. 163-178). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development, 75, 428-444. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14, 237-243. doi: 10.1111/14679280.02438 Wynn, K. (1992). Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220-251. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90008-p How Many Is a Zillion? / 19 Table 1. Stimulus Numerals Used in the Number Placement Task of Experiment 1 a. Three b Seventy-one hundred and ninety-three c. Twenty-eight thousand and thirty-two d. Seventy-five million and thirty-five e. Twenty-nine billion and ninety-one f. Forty-seven trillion and sixty-four g. Sixty-five quadrillion and sixty-nine h. Sixty quintillion and ten i. Eighteen sextillion and forty-five j. Thirty dillion and forty-twoa k. Eighty-three robillion and seventy-sixa l. Twenty-one jillion and fifty-onea m. Eleven bajillion and seventy-onea n. Twenty-one skillion and thirty-twoa o. Thirty katrillion and fifty-fivea p. Twenty-eight fantillion and fifty-foura q. Forty-two bazillion and forty-ninea r. Fifteen kabillion and seventy-twoa s. Forty-three umptillion and twenty-fivea t. Ninety-two zillion and fiftya a Fictitious numbers How Many Is a Zillion? / 20 Table 2. Stimulus Numerals Used in the Number Placement Task of Experiment 2 a. Three 3 b Seventy-one hundred and ninety-three 7,193 c. Twenty-eight thousand and thirty-two 28 103 + 32 d. Seventy-five million and thirty-five 75 106 + 35 e. Twenty-nine billion and ninety-one 29 109 + 91 f. Forty-seven trillion and sixty-four 47 1012 + 64 g. Sixty-five quadrillion and sixty-nine 65 1015 + 69 h. Sixty quintillion and ten 60 1018 + 10 i. Eighteen sextillion and forty-five 18 1021 + 45 j. Ninety-two septillion and fifty 92 1024 + 50 k. Twenty-one octillion and fifty-one 21 1027 + 51 l. Forty-three nonillion and twenty-five 43 1030 + 25 m. Eleven decillion and seventy-one 11 1033 + 71 n. Fifteen undecillion and seventy-two 15 1036 + 72 o. Twenty-one duodecillion and thirty-two 21 1039 + 32 p. Forty-two tredecillion and forty-nine 42 1042 + 49 q. Twenty-eight quattuordecillion and fifty-four 28 1045 + 54 r. Thirty quindecillion and forty-two 30 1048 + 42 s. Thirty sexdecillion and fifty-five 30 1051 + 55 t. Eight-three septendecillion and seventy-six 83 1054 + 76 a. 0 100 b. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Position on Number Line Figure 1. (a) A number line of the type used in experiments on children’s placement of numerals. (b) The results of a simulation of number-line placement, as described in the text. Each distribution is a smoothed curve, based on a sample of 106 randomly generated values. Standard Error of Mean (mm) a. 14 12 10 8 a bc d e f j kg l i mno hp q r s t 6 4 2 Zero One Gazillion 0 0 20 40 60 80 100 Mean Position on Number Line (mm) b. Mean Position on Number Line (mm) 100 80 60 40 20 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Ordinal Position of Numeral Figure 2. (a) Mean placement of numerals on the number line. Blue circles denote conventional numbers; red squares, fictitious numbers. Labels on the points correspond to the listing in Table 1. The error bars (1 SEM) appear in a vertical orientation for legibility. (b) Best-fitting logarithmic (solid line) and linear function (dashed line) to the mean number positions, Experiment 1. Standard Error of Mean (mm) a. 14 12 10 a bc d e f g rhi spnkjlqm o t 8 6 4 2 One Google Zero 0 0 20 40 60 80 100 Mean Position on Number Line (mm) Mean Position on Number Line (mm) b. 100 80 60 40 20 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Ordinal Position of Numeral Figure 3. (a) Mean placement of numerals on the number line. Labels on the points correspond to the listing in Table 2. (b) Best-fitting logarithmic (solid line) and linear function (dashed line) to the mean number positions, Experiment 2.