Qualitative Inquiry As pointed out earlier in this chapter, quantitative and qualitative methods have complimentary strengths that together allow scientists to get a fuller picture of the complexity of human development as it occurs in context. As Yoshikawa and colleagues state: “the combination of words and numbers can bring us closer to the complexity of developmental change by providing divergent as well as convergent data” (Yoshikawa, Weisner, Kalil & Way, 2008, pg 345). The terms quantitative and qualitative themselves speak to the strengths of each methodology. According to Merriam-Webster’s dictionary, quantitative is defined as “of, relating to, or involving the measurement of quantity or amount.” Qualitative, on the other hand, is defined as “of, relating to, or involving quality or kind (http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary).” Within developmental science, quantitative data and analysis represent the world numerically whereas qualitative data and analysis use non-numeric forms, including words and pictures (Yoshikawa, et al). Certainly no one would argue that the only knowledge needed about the world is of either quantity (numeric) or quality (non-numeric). In fact, in chemistry, typically thought of as a quantitative science, chemists use qualitative analysis to “identify the components of a substance or mixture (Merriam-Webster).” In much this way, qualitative social scientists often draw on qualitative methods to ascertain the components of a social phenomenon or setting. Although mixed methods were first used in psychology in the early twentieth century (Teddlie and Tashakkori, YR), in the field of child development it has historically been all too common for developmental scientists to approach topics from a primarily, and often solely, quantitative standpoint. This may stem from developmental science’s origins in psychology as a lab-based science and following John Stewart Mill’s System of Logic (CITE). Mill positioned the social sciences in relation to the natural sciences, with similar aims of identifying general laws for explanation and prediction of the social world. This view of the social sciences, including developmental science, has its roots in positivism and post-positivism, epistemologies that believe in a singular, objective Truth that can be isolated, measured (either in part or in whole), and either verified (positivism) or falsified (post-positivism; Lincoln & Guba, 2003). As a result, Mill positioned the natural and social sciences as methodologically identical. Yet the social world, including human development, offers a number of complicating factors which call into question the privileging of positivism and post-positivism as the only, or at least dominant, paradigms. For example, whereas in natural science it possible to control the context of a given experiment, even within experimental designs in social science people still exist within their natural environments, which can never be fully controlled. Furthermore, people behave based on their own understandings of a situation, which may or may not match the “objective” reality of a given situation. Much like the movie Rashomon, each individual person has his or her own perspective on the world, complicating the idea of generalization and prediction with the social sciences. Qualitative methods, although they have their beginnings in post-positivism (CITE Lincoln & Guba), have moved increasingly towards interpretive paradigms, with a growing presence of post-modern, post-structural, and critical epistemologies (CITE). These paradigms challenge the traditional role of objectivity and researcher-subject independence (CITE). Qualitative methods recognize the social nature and contextual boundedness of knowledge, positions that can be difficult to integrate with traditional developmental science. Whereas quantitative methodologies believe that subjectivity is a threat to the measurement of the “real world,” qualitative methodologies believe that the objective view of the world is incomplete and that naturalistic inquiry is needed to adequately capture the social world (Shweder, 1996). Despite their historical marginalization in developmental science, qualitative methods are particularly well suited for capturing two important aspects of developmental phenomena that are integral to a developmental systems approach to human development: process and context (CITE). Although qualitative methods have traditionally been eschewed as a means of understanding causal relationships, scholars are now recognizing the strengths of qualitative approaches in identifying the mechanisms behind causal relationships (Yoshikawa, Weisner, Kalil & Way, 2008; Yin CITE). This primarily comes from qualitative methods’ abilities to address questions about “how” and “why” (as opposed to “how many” or “how much”), to identify, as in chemical qualitative analysis, the “components” of human development as it occurs within particular contexts. Charles Ragin identifies seven major goals of social research, some of which he notes as being better suited to quantitative methods and others to qualitative methods: 1) identifying general patterns and relationships; 2) testing and refining theories; 3) making predictions; 4) interpreting culturally or historically significant phenomena; 5) exploring diversity; 6) giving voice, and; 7) advancing new theories. Within qualitative methodology, different methods have different strengths. For example, observations are best suited for understanding actions and behaviors – or what people do. Interview techniques are better suited for understanding the meanings people make of their environments and interactions– or what people believe. The educational researcher Frederick Erikson (CITE) separates out actions from behavior, defining behavior as action plus intention, [CHECK TERMINOLOGY] and identifying behavior (not actions) as the focus of qualitative inquiry. Similarly, Barbara Rogoff (YR) points out in The Cultural Nature of Human Development, that to understand human development we must capture both what happens and what people believe about what happens: To understand development, it is helpful to separate value judgments from observations of events. It is important to examine the meaning and function of events for the local cultural frameworks and goals, conscientiously avoiding the arbitrary imposition of one’s own values on another group. Interpreting the activity of people without regard of their meaning system and goals renders observations meaningless. (Rogoff, pg 17) This dual focus on activity and meaning leads many qualitative researchers to combine methods within studies, drawing on interviews, observations, photographic techniques, and newer approaches such as mapping, to triangulate data in an attempt to get more fully at the phenomenon under study. Qualitative Methods There are many different types of qualitative methods that have strengths for answering different types of research questions. Often, researchers will use multiple modes of data collection to allow for triangulation across data types. Below we provide a brief overview of different types of qualitative data and what types of information they are best for gathering. Observations. Observational techniques in qualitative research stem from those used in ethnography, an anthropologic approach to studying culture. Ethnography is not merely observing a culture, but using what Geertz (CITE) has termed “thick description” to capture the rules and behaviors governing any given culture. To describe a culture, then, is not to recount the events of a society but to specify what one must know to make those events maximally probable. The problem is not to state what someone did but to specify the conditions under which it is culturally appropriate to anticipate that he, or persons occupying his role, will render an equivalent performance. This conception of a cultural description implies that ethnography should be a theory of cultural behavior in a particular behavior, the adequacy of which is to be evaluated by the ability of a stranger to the culture…to use the ethnography’s statements as instructions for appropriately anticipating the scenes of the society…the test of descriptive adequacy must always refer to informant’s interpretations of events not simply to the occurrence of events. (Frake, 1964) Ethnography relies on participant-observation, a method in which the researcher learns about a culture by participating in it while simultaneously observing the culture through detailed field notes. Field notes capture both the being and observing that is part of participant-observation (CITE; Spradley?). The field notes themselves (also called the running record) capture the observing, or what occurs, through thick descriptions of the physical setting, people, and actions (Marshall & Rossman, YR). The analytic notes capture the “being,” or observer’s comments on what is occurring, including subjective feelings as well as questions and analytic insights (Marshall & Rossman, YR). Observational data is best at capturing what people actually do. Yet participantobservation also typically includes interviews (either formal or informal) allowing for simultaneous data about people’s beliefs and meanings. ETC. Interviews Focus Groups Documents, photographs, maps, and other methods Narrative approaches Surveys Kinesics & proxemics Paradigms of Research Some have argued against mixing qualitative and quantitative methods due to the differences in the paradigms that underlie most quantitative and qualitative studies. This approach, called the incompatibility view (Teddlie & Tashakkori, YR), posits that the underlying paradigmatic differences between qualitative and quantitative approaches, in particular the differences in ontology and epistemology, lead to different goals, making it impossible to mix methods within single studies. Yet this view, we would argue, not only confuses methods with methodologies but also risks throwing out the baby with the bath water – leading researchers to miss out on the strengths of combining approaches in the name of paradigmatic rigidity. As others have pointed out, it is possible to quantitatively analyze qualitative data and to qualitatively analyze quantitative data (Yoshikawa et al), indicating that the paradigm split is not between the methods themselves but between typical uses and analytic goals of the methods. In other words, it is not the methods that have paradigmatic meaning, but what we do with them. Teddlie and Tashakkori (CITE) discuss the six different views of how social scientists believe paradigms should inform mixed methods research. Much research appears to be conducted under the umbrella of pragmatism (EXPAND). It may not be surprising, then, that evaluators were early adopters of mixing methods and have used mixed methods for a variety of purposes in their work (see Greene CITE). Others argue that researchers can switch paradigms depending on the needs of a study and still others advocate for intentionally mixing paradigms within a study with the goal of engaging “the tensions that emerge from the juxtaposition of these multiple diverse perspectives” (Teddlie & Tashakkori, pg 18). [NOTE – IF I WERE WRITING THIS ALONE I WOULD PROBABLY ADVOCATE FOR THIS LAST POSITION OR SAY THIS LAST POSITION MOST CLOSELY RESEMBLES HOW WE THINK ABOUT MIXED METHODS – DO YOU BOTH AGREE OR SHOULD I REVISIT THAT IDEA?] Recently, a body of mixed methods work has developed within child development studies. Thomas Weisner’s volume (CITE) on how mixed methods have been used to study children and families brings together a number of prominent researchers from across disciplines who have contributed to the literature in developmental science through using mixed methods to study child development within context. EXAMPLES References Richard Shweder’s “Quanta and qualia: What is the ‘Object’ of ethnographic method?” in Jessor, Colby & Shweder Ethnography and Human Development: Context and Meaning in Social Inquiry (University of Chicago Press, 1996)