The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children Advertisements directed at children contain strong stereotypes attached to the labels “boy” and “girl”. The nature of this problem is that gendered language both reflects and contributes to stereotypes about males and females. Male and female youth represent an increasingly profitable market for advertisers. Historically, this trend has grown considerably over the last few decades, partly due to the increasingly large number of cable channels offered on television (Childs & Maher, 2003, p. 408). Childs and Maher (2003) say that researchers started to show a strong focus on this issue around the mid- to late-1990s. During this time it was estimated that children aged 2-14 “directly influenced $188 billion USD of parental expenditures...” (Childs & Maher, 2003, p. 408). Childs and Maher (2003) point out that annual spending on advertising for children amounted to approximately $3 billion USD in 2003 (Childs & Maher, 2003, p. 408). Even though children’s advertising had received scholarly attention prior to the 1990s, it was this sort of unprecedented growth that lead to an increase in studies (Childs & Maher, 2003, p. 409). Childs and Maher (2003) suggest that many researchers wanted to know if this increase in children’s television and advertising lead to a direct increase in exposure to gender bias in everyday life. Gender stereotypes in advertisements lead to behaviours that reflect gender attitudes by associating gender-specific behaviour with personal rewards and fulfilment. Gender specific behavior is a reflection of the popular idea that males and females are “vastly different psychologically” (Hyde, 2005, p. 581). Hyde’s argument is that males and females, including youth and adults, “are similar on most, but not all psychological variables” (p. 581). This supports the idea that advertising continues to 1 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children present strong stereotypes partly because many consumers, for a wide variety of reasons, have proven to be quite receptive of gender stereotypes, as illustrated by continued sales. Nelson and Vilela (2012) discuss how assumptions about gender differences have also been part of scholarly work on this subject. For example, these researchers question whether the Selectivity Hypothesis is still appropriate for scholars considering the fact that this theory relies heavily on supposed differences between males and females. The central principle behind this hypothesis is that in order to be successful, advertisers have to market to men and women in different ways. Nelson and Vilela (2012) discuss this theory as an example of how gender is not a “fact” but rather “an ideology that originates from social, historical, and cultural constructs of ideas and beliefs” (Nelson & Vilela, 2012) . (Toys ‘R’ Us, 2013) Hot Wheels Retrieved from http://www.toysrus.ca/category/index.jsp?categoryId=3730382&foreSeeBrowseSampli ng=15&foreSeeBrowseLoyalty=1&foreSeeEnabled=true The social construction of gender is a major component of prescribed behavior in elementary schools. Baker-Sperry (2006) says that children negotiate ideas about gender 2 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children while interacting with peers in an elementary school setting. Baker-Sperry (2006) refers to today’s children as “active agents” who take an authoritative part in filtering messages and perceiving meaning on their own terms, even within settings such as elementary schools that are controlled by adults (p. 38). As children work out what it means to be a “boy” or “girl” for themselves, they often establish their own unique rules for gendered behavior. Baker-Sperry (2006) says that as a result, there are sometimes social repercussions to “crossing gender lines,” including humiliation and chastisement (p. 41). If gendered advertisements promote this sort of division along gender lines, the reward and fulfillment promised by such ads is the sense of belonging and peer acceptance that goes along with following socially-accepted ideas about gender categories. Miller et al. (2009) support this point by saying that children often respond more harshly to boys “who violate gender appearance norms” than they do to girls (p. 877). Thus, for boys the reward for conforming to gender norms is the avoidance of social exclusion. The same research also suggests that girls receive considerable “social sanctions” for not conforming to physical appearance norms (Miller et al., 2009, p. 877). Bakir and Palan (2010) argue that creating such clear and strong separations between the genders takes place partly because “stereotyping functions as a way to categorize information about what it means to be male and female...” (p. 35). As children continually rely on gender stereotypes to categorize themselves and others, they “develop cognitive structure to help them make sense of gender-related information” (Bakir & Palan, 2010, p. 35). Bakir and Palan (2010) say that as a result, gender categorization becomes a natural cognitive process. 3 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children The idea that gender categorization becomes a natural cognitive process is supported by the fact that advertising provides one of the most frequent sources of gender categorization. Bakir and Palan (2010) say that the average child in the United States views approximately 40,000 commercials per year. These commercials appear predominantly on television, but also on billboards, public transportation ads, the Internet, newspapers, movie theatres and many other locations in our media environment (Johnson & Young, 2002, p. 461). Research studies suggest that the vast majority of these advertisements contain gender stereotyping. Johnson and Young (2002) convey this point by investigating televised ads for toys in order to determine how advertisers script language differently for males and females. These researchers ask; “How is gender used as a discourse code to link products to gender roles?” (Johnson & Young, 2002, p. 461). Johnson and Young’s (2002) answer to this question is that products are linked to gender roles by way of portraying certain actions as being performed exclusively by one or the other gender. Some researchers have argued that this has a significant impact on children because being exposed only to traditional sex roles limits the “range of experiences” for children to experiment with (Johnson & Young, 2002, p. 465). The gendered nature of advertising goes beyond the more obvious gender roles portrayed in television commercials. In their study, Childs and Maher (2003) recorded 215 commercials directed at children. 46% of this sample contained commercials advertising food or candy and 33% advertised for toys (Childs & Maher, 2003, p. 411). Childs and Maher (2003) focus on food advertisements because they are often thought to be gender neutral. Childs and Maher (2003) challenge this view by measuring some key variables that illustrate the degree and type of gender categorization. These variables 4 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children include the gender roles of characters in advertisements, the gender of voice-overs, and the gender of the main user of the product. Childs and Maher’s (2003) show that within their sample, gender preferences were “significantly” present in most of the measures they examined. While this was not surprising for advertisements promoting toys, Childs and Maher (2003) discovered that supposedly “gender-neutral” food advertisements shows strong gender preferences for voice-overs, dominant products users, and main character roles. This subtle form of gender preference illustrates that gender bias can reach children at a more subconscious level, which may have an even stronger impact on their views of gender, since it can be more difficult to notice and challenge the messages sent through subtle advertising. Added to these assessments of media and the frequency of gendered advertisements, there are also a number of psychological theories that assess the impact of stereotypes on children. For example, the concept “stereotype activation” refers to stereotypes being established in the individual’s memory after frequent exposure (Miller et al., 2009, p. 871). These stereotypes are then retrieved from memory when people make social judgements and behavioral decisions (Miller et al., 2009, p. 871). As mentioned above, children are frequently exposed to gender stereotypes, suggesting that such stereotypes are regularly stored their memories. Thus, the “stereotype activation” hypothesis suggests that gendered advertisements have a direct impact on how children act and feel toward one another and toward adults. Miller et al. (2009) develop this point by examining how boys and girls describe their respective gender traits. After interviewing 256 children aged 3 to 10, Miller et al. (2009) conclude that “appearance stereotypes were particularly prevalent in descriptions of girls and activity/trait 5 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children stereotypes were more prevalent in descriptions of boys”. Miller et al. (2009) focus on relatively new concepts related to gender stereotyping, such as the different ways that boys and girls perceive messages about gender and what they focus on when looking at ads. Miller et al. (2009) found that when girls were asked to respond to advertisements, the most frequent responses targeted appearance (31% of participants). This was followed by traits (19%) and activities (14%) (Miller et al., 2009, p. 874). Boys showed a very different focus for their responses. 27% of boys focused on character traits, followed by 19% focusing on activities, and 13% focusing on appearance (Miller et al., 2009, p. 874). Miller et al. (2009) say that these findings indicate that some gender stereotypes are “differentially accessible when children think about boys and girls”. In other words, some stereotype categories (for example, appearance) are applied more often to girls than they are to boys. This also raises the possibility that certain stereotype categories are stronger than others because of the ways they are exposed to children. For example, appearance may be the strongest focus among girls because they are most frequently exposed to visual advertisements that have something to do with one’s appearance and behavior. The following advertisement from the brand “Just Like Home” clearly illustrates this tendency for products intended for girls: (Toys ‘R’ Us, 2013) Just Like Home Retrieved from http://www.toysrus.ca/family/index.jsp?categoryId=4016318 6 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children Research on the impacts of media on people’s appearance and behavior has traditionally focused on television and print media, but new research is emerging on gender differences that appear on the Internet (such as the example provided above). As McQuillan and O’Neill (2009) point out, Internet usage continues to rise as computers become cheaper and more advanced. Children are becoming increasingly computer literate as these devices become easier to use. McQuillan and O’Neill (2009) discuss the optimistic view that because Internet usage is now “more participatory and equitable than traditional media,” it may not have the same negative impact that television has on gender perceptions (p. 366). Further research is needed on this topic, but an initial position taken by McQuillan and O’Neill (2009) is that while the gender gap may not be as strong on the Internet, “more subtle gender differences in online activities are emerging, particularly among children”. This raises the need to consider the character of new media while trying to resolve the issue of gender stereotyping. The first possibility for helping resolve the issue of gender stereotyping in children’s advertising is to conduct more research on the motivations and experiences of young Internet users (McQuillan & O’Neill, 2009, p. 375). Some assume that youth’s experiences with the Internet are largely similar, but according to McQuillan and O’Neill (2009), patterns of technology development and use also depend on gender. For example, McQuillan and O’Neill (2009) claim that gender determines how young people use the Internet because their identities are shaped by “multiple communities of masculinity and femininity practice”. The gender stereotypes promoted within these communities should be studied based on how they impact Internet usage among youth, or how young boys and girls use the internet in difference ways. Another more hands-on approach to 7 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children resolving this issue is to make advertisers aware of how their practices impact young consumers. Advertisers are not always aware of the degree to which they are perpetuating gender stereotypes, as they are concentrating on pleasing the public with new and exciting ideas. Bakir and Palan (2010) do not provide a specific strategy for advertisers (nor would this be an easy and straightforward process), but these researchers do recommend that parents take on a greater role in helping their children understand gender content in advertising. Baker-Sperry’s (2006) study implies that teachers should also take an active role in helping children understand gender relations. This recommendation is based on the fact that children spend a great deal of time with one another at school, during which time they often construct their own meanings about gender. Baker-Sperry (2006) proposes that excessive competition, which is a common feature of many schools, creates social hierarchies which in turn create peer pressure to conform to specific gender norms. Teachers might address this issue by informing students that there are multiple other rewards and gratifications to be experienced by not conforming to gender norms, such as making new friends across gender boundaries and experimenting with products (e.g., toys and clothes) that do not fit traditional notions of “male” and “female”. One of the most effective ways to resolve the issue of gender advertising in children’s advertisements is to make a case to advertisers for the benefits of creating gender-neutral ads. Baker-Sperry (2006) suggests the benefit of gender-neutral advertising by mentioning previous research that shows how children play with toys in many different ways and with toys that are not necessarily exclusive to their gender. For example, Baker-Sperry (2006) says that many young females commonly use items that are identified as being for the opposite gender. In another research experiment, children 8 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children played in mixed sex groups. Within these groups “children consistently played with masculine or neutral toys and games, rarely engaging in play that could be identified as ‘feminine’”. Baker-Sperry (2006) claims that many advertisers have already become aware of this type of behaviour pattern and that they responded by including a mixture of both boys and girls in their advertisements. Baker-Sperry (2006) also states that advertisers have done this to maximise the selling potential. This strongly suggests that there is already a viable market for gender- neutral advertising and toys. 9 The Gendered Language of Advertisements Directed at Children References Baker-Sperry, L. (2006). Gendered Agency: Power in the Elementary Classroom. Women and Language, 29(2), 38-46. Bakir, A., & Palan, K.M. (2010). How are Children’s Attitudes Toward Ads and Brands Affected by Gender-Related Content in Advertising? Journal of Advertising, 39(1), 35-48. Childs, N.M., & Maher, J.K. (2003). Gender in food advertising: boys eat first. British Food Journal, 105(7), 408-419. Hyde, J.S. (2005). The Gender Similarities Hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581-592. Johnson, F., & Young, K. (2002). Gendered Voices in Children’s Television Advertising. Critical Studies in Media Communication, 19(4), 461-480. McQuillan, H., & O’Neill, B. (2009). Gender Differences in Children’s Internet Use. Journal of Children and Media, 3(4), 366-378. Miller, C.F., Lurye, L.E., Zosuls, K.M., & Ruble, D.N. (2009). Accessibility of Gender Stereotyping Domains: Developmental and Gender Differences in Children. Sex Roles, 60, 870-881. Nelson, M.R., & Vilela, A.M. (2012). Is the Selectivity Hypothesis Still Relevant? A Review of Gendered Persuasion and Processing of Advertising Messages. In C.C. Otnes & L.T. Zayer (Eds.), Gender, Culture, and Consumer Behavior (pp. 111133). New York, NY: Routledge. 10