The effects of a female audience on the aggression of male Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens, based upon size of male. Bayan Yazdi and Farhad Esmaeilzadeh Department of Biological Sciences, Saddleback College, Mission Viejo, CA 92692 Betta splendens (Siamese fighting fish) are a naturally aggressive species that have been bred for larger body sizes and hyper-aggressive temperament for commercial use. It was hypothesized that male Betta splendens are more aggressive when a female is visibly present, and that larger males are more aggressive than smaller males. Nine males and one female B. splendens were used in this study. Randomly determined pairs of males were allowed to view each other through a clear divider for five minutes per trial. Based upon this data, size of male is not correlated to aggression levels (p= 0.902), however males are significantly more aggressive when a female is present (p= 0.001). INTRODUCTION: Male Betta splendens typically have a high propensity towards aggression, which has arisen from male competition and mating rituals (King, 1973), and has been made more prominent by selective breeding for larger body sizes and hyper-aggressive temperaments (Monvises et al., 2009). Upon visual contact, male B. splendens display specific aggression reactions such as approaching, tail beating, biting attempts, and extending gill covers (Verbeek et al., 2005; Braud and Weibel, 1969). By utilizing these aggressive displays, each male B. splendens attempts to create the illusion that it is larger than its opponent. The gill cover display involves the attacking fish facing its opponent head-on and flaring its opercular gill covers, increasing its size relative to its opponent. Each aggressive display typically lasts for a few seconds, and each “fight” usually ends in the retreat or death of one fish (Bronstein, 1983). Scale coloration has been shown to have no significant impact on male Betta splendens aggression (Bucknam et al., 2013). It was hypothesized that a larger male B. splendens would have a more aggressive nature, and males regardless of size would be more aggressive when in the presence of a female. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Nine male and one female Betta splendens were purchased from PetSmart (San Juan Capistrano, CA, and Aliso Viejo, CA). Subjects were measured from the base of their tail to their mouth and were placed on a size spectrum to determine if they fit into the large or small size category. The subjects were held in PetSmart standard Betta holding containers (Petsmart, Phoenix, AZ) approximately 20 cm apart and were kept isolated in this way for at least three days to acclimate to laboratory conditions. Subjects were fed twice daily at 8:00 am and 5:00 pm with Top Fin Betta Bits (Petsmart, Phoenix, AZ). All testing took place between 8:00am and 11:30am under controlled and standardized lighting, temperature (25°C), and pH. For the experiment, all pairings for the trials were randomized by Random.org: True Random Number Service (Random.org, Dublin, Ireland). For each trial, two male fish were held in an Aqueon Betta Bowl (Aqueon Aquarium Products, Franklin, WI) on separate sides of the divider. The testing tank was equipped with a clear and darkened divider. Subjects were acclimated to the test tank for five minutes prior to each trial, with the darkened divider in place. After five minutes, the darkened divider was removed and subjects were allowed to view one another for five minutes. During trials that involved the female, a separate Aqueon Betta Bowl (Aqueon Aquarium Products, Franklin, WI) containing the female was attached to the first bowl, so that the males would be able to view the female for the entire ten minutes of the experiment. During the trial time, the number of gill cover flares, tail beats, approaches, and biting attempts of the male were counted and documented. Figure 1. Diagram depicting tank trials were conducted in. Both transparent and darkened dividers in place between the two male fish during first five minutes. Only transparent dividers in place during the following five experimental minutes. Transparent divider in place between female and males for entire duration of experiment. RESULTS: Figure 2. Comparison of aggressive behavior in male Betta splendens with and without female Betta splendens. Chi-square =58.9. Probability = 0.001. Male fish are significantly more aggressive when female is present as opposed to when female is absent. Figure 3. Comparison of aggressive behavior in male Betta splendens based upon size. Chi-square= 2.19. Probability = 0.902. There is no correlation between size and aggression levels in male Betta splendens. Without Female Audience With Female Audience Gill flare 371 Approach 244 Tail beat 51 Bite attempt 68 479 304 106 250 Table 1: Total aggressive displays of all male Betta splendens tested with and without female audience. Small/large Small/small Large/large Gill flare 28 37 23 Approach 16 25 16 Tail beat 5 5 5 Bite attempt 9 19 8 Table 2: Mean aggressive displays of all male Betta splendens tested, categorized by size. Figure 4. Profile of subject C displaying gill flare. Figure one reflects the tank all subjects were tested in. Size and color pairing categories were compared using a chi-squared test. Figure two depicts bar graph of total number of gill flares, approaches, tail beats, and biting attempts displayed by male Betta splendens, based upon whether or not female was present. Fish with a female audience were significantly more aggressive than fish without a female audience (p=0.001, chisquared test). Table two is the mean aggressive displays of all male Betta splendens tested, categorized by size. Figure three reflects table two as a bar graph; mean aggressive displays based upon size of male subjects. There was no correlation between size and aggression in male Betta splendens (p= 0.902 chi-squared test). Figure four is a picture of subject C displaying a gill flare, an example of an aggressive display. DISCUSSION: There is no correlation between size and male B. splendens aggression levels. Male B. splendens are more aggressive when a female is visibly present. Further research may be done on the effects of the size of the female on male B. splendens aggression. More research could also take into account the effect of the coloration of the male on its aggression. Bucknam, Esmaeilzadeh, Yazdi, and Yuen found that color and size do not affect male B. splendens aggression when there is no female audience. Our results indicated that size is not a factor in aggressive behavior even if a female is visibly present. Therefore, another study could be conducted to evaluate if color plays a role in aggressive behavior when a female is visibly present. LITERATURE CITED: Braud, W. G. & Weibel, J. E. (1969). Acquired Stimulus Control of Drug-Induced Changes in Aggressive Display in Betta Splendens. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 12 (5): 773-777. Bucknam, K., Esmaeilzadeh, F., Yazdi, B., Yuen, N. (2013). The Effects of Body Size and Coloration on Aggression in Male Siamese Fighting Fish (Betta splendens). Saddleback Journal of Biology. King, J. A. (1973). The ecology of aggressive behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 4: 117-138. Monvises, A., Nuangsaeng, B., Sriwattanarothai, N., Panijpan, B. (2009). The Siamese fighting fish: well-known generally but little-known scientifically. Science Asia, 35: 8-16. Thompson, T. (1966). Operant and classically-conditioned aggressive behavior in Siamese fighting fish. American Zoologist, 6 (4): 629-641. Verbeek, P., Toshitaka I., & Noboru M. (2005). Differences in Aggression Between WildType and Domesticated Fighting Fish are Context Dependent. Animal Behaviour, 73: 75-83.