EFFECT OF OLFACTORY STIMULI ON ANIMAL MODELS OF ANXIETY Megha Andrews, Madlyn Kates, Christine Lu, Katherine Miao, Archana Raghunath, Angeli Sharma, Grace Shen, Michael Tai, Mary Tresvalles, Elena Wei, Destiny West, Audrey Zhou Advisor: Dr. Graham Cousens Assistant: Runi Patel ABSTRACT In this study, the effect of predator pheromones and odorant stimuli on phasic fear and immediate or sustained response was examined in the corticomedial amygdala of rats. To identify which stimuli would produce anxiety-like symptoms, behavioral studies such as an Acoustic Startle Response (ASR) experiment and an Open Maze experiment were conducted. The Acoustic Startle Response, when conducted in conjunction with an exposure to predator pheromones, served as a measure of phasic response to sudden stimuli. The Open Maze test, based on thigmotaxis and aversion to open spaces, is used as an assay in anxiety-related behavior. To see if neurons in the amygdala are responsive to sensory stimuli, an Electrophysiology experiment was conducted. This measured electrical activity in the neurons of the corticomedial amygdala, their firing patterns in relation to odorant triggers and subsequent anxiety responses. Understanding the circuitry of the amygdaloidal complex and how sensory stimuli are processed can provide insight into predator-odor induced anxiety responses, and aid in further research on anxiety disorders in humans. INTRODUCTION Anxiety has provided humans with a means to react to potential threats and act accordingly to ensure survival. However, anxiety as an evolutionary advantage has evolved into pervasive disorders that negatively impact both physical and mental health. Human beings exhibit anxiety disorders which range from phobias to panic disorders. Characterized by feelings of dread over future events, anxiety disorders are commonly associated with general fear and uneasiness, as well as chronic symptoms of heart and respiratory disease. Anxiety disorders and its multiple variants have become one of the most prevalent forms of psychological illness. Thus, a heightened scientific interest in its anatomical causes and neural mechanisms has followed suit. However, as human experimentation of anxiety and fear necessitates stress and displeasure, a standardized animal model is implemented as a practicable alternative. Although many researchers believe that anxiety and fear are discrete emotions, there is still behavioral similarity that allows scientists to look to phasic and sustained fear in animal models to understand the role fear and anxiety play within the corticomedial amygdala and other neural pathways. The phasic fear is a sudden response to specific stimuli that dissipates quickly once the threat is eliminated, akin to specific phobias, while a sustained fear which is caused by apprehension to a less specific and unpredictable threat, akin to anxiety disorders (3). Anatomically, the processing of phasic and sustained fear is located in discrete areas of the amygdala with evidence that the corticomedial nuclei is involved with sustained anxiety-like responses. The use of pheromones on rat models examines the phasic fear and sustained fear which cause anxiety responses and behavior. [4-1] Olfaction perception is processed within a plethora of neural pathways. The olfactory portion of a rat's brain is known to have five subsections: the main olfactory system (MOS), the accessory olfactory system (AOS), the trigeminal system, the Grueneberg ganglion, and the septal organ of Masera (8). Pheromones are detected using the vomeronasal organ (VNO) (8). Species may evince signs of fear and stress such as freezing or running due to activation of the sympathetic nervous system from detecting predator odors. However, would a pheromone from a natural predator cause a Sprague-Dawley domesticated rat, which has never been exposed to the odor, to become aversive due to innate anxiety responses within the corticomedial amygdala? If a prey species is exposed to the pheromone for the first time, the reluctance to stay in the same vicinity as a pheromone may be seen due to innate behaviors passed on through breeding. Although there has already been research indicating the role of the entorhinal cortex of a rat brain in processing olfactory stimuli and pheromones, it is believed that the amygdala in the temporal lobes also play a seminal part in response to pheromones (10). There are three subregions of the amygdala: the basolateral (BLA), central, and corticomedial amygdala (CMA). While all three regions aid in olfactory perception, only the CMA receives input from both the MOS and the AOS. Consequently, it is likely that the corticomedial amygdala is not only able to receive signals generated by pheromones, but also regular, nonvolatile odors. The objective of this study is to examine the effect of olfactory stimulation on animal models of anxiety. Three experiments were conducted to evaluate potential contributions of the corticomedial amygdala to processing olfactory information relevant to a predator odor model of anxiety. Sustained fear response, tested with the Acoustic Startle Test, and aversion, tested with the Open Maze, were measured after exposure to predator odors, while electrophysiology quantified the neural activity when exposed to pheromones. These tests were then repeated with the control variables of neutral odorants and a non-predator pheromone. Open Maze Open Maze Behavioral Response To determine if predator pheromones would stimulate aversive behaviors, the rats were placed within a tub and introduced to predator pheromones, cat urine and fox urine, which were placed in specified zones on gauze. The rats explored the tub and the aversion due to the exposure of pheromones was analyzed. In nature, due to sympathetic nervous system activation, prey species elicit signs of fear and stress such as aversion or running, the fight or flight response, as an innate survival mechanism. The prediction was that domesticated rats which come in contact with a predator pheromone would become aversive to the area where the odor is present even if exposed to that pheromone for the first time. Rats have innate behaviors which must be taken into account within this study. Within an open maze, rats may tend to avoid open spaces and stay around the perimeter of the maze, a phenomenon known as thigmotaxis (4). The thigmotaxis that rats exhibit is due to innate stress and anxiety behavior, which in nature enables avoidance of predator species to maintain survival. This aversive behavior may lead to less exploration of the maze and the pheromones. In order to avoid this behavior affecting the study, the rats were placed in the middle of the maze in order to begin exploration and given consequent trials to acclimate to the maze. [4-2] Acoustic Startle Response In order to determine the effects of the odorants on the subjects, the rats were placed in startle chambers and observed for their acoustic startle response. When exposed to sound pulses of approximately 85 decibels (dBA) or higher, the rats startle in response due to an inherent reflex. In nature, such responses to sudden, loud stimuli may protect them from the attack of predators. Modulation of Acoustic Startle Response While the aforementioned pontine reticular nucleus is pivotal in generating motor output in the acoustic startle response, it also receives input from other nuclei that can alter the intensity of the response due to factors such as habituation, prepulse inhibition (PPI), and fear (5). Since it is known that fear can enhance the startle response, studying the activity of rats in the presence of pheromones known to elicit fear is a potential way of understanding whether the corticomedial amygdala has a role in processing fear and the related emotion, anxiety. The Effect of Neophobia on Acoustic Startle Response One factor that had to be taken into account in the startle chamber portion of the study was neophobia, which is defined as the fear of new experiences. In order to avoid the nebulous situation of whether an enhanced startle response was caused by the pheromones or the fact that the rodents were enclosed in a box they have never been in before, the rats were given a brief acclimation period of one to five minutes in the chamber before any sound pulses and/or pheromones were released. Electrophysiology Channels and Variables Unit Recordings of Odor Specific Neural Ensembles Single-unit electric recordings of individual or ensembles of neurons provide valuable insight into the function of specific brain regions in processing external information and signals. The ability to monitor single-neuron activity allows for high spatial and temporal resolution in the assessment of interneuron communication and flow. Such recordings enable easy measurement of action potentials or enhanced neural activity via extracellular spike discharge without damage to the cell. Subsequently, receptive field analysis is performed in which a stimulus configuration activates neuron recording into an electrophysiology diagram. In vitro single-unit extracellular recording of the anesthetized rat involving a Tungsten microelectrode was performed to measure the electrical activity of cells in the corticomedial amygdaloidal complex- thought to orchestrate a behavioral and physiological response to predator stimuli. Recording of CMA neuronal activity in the rat when exposed to both volatile odors and pheromones evinces the olfactory pathways that elicit anxiety or fear. Given that this recording technique allows for the detection of neighboring neurons around the electrode tip, neurons in the CMA may even generate a synchronous response or experience similar neuronal tuning. This may provide additional insight into the interactions between neurons in processing predator odors followed by an anxiety-like response. Measure of Respiratory Rate using Nasal Thermocouple [4-3] Respiration is crucial to the olfactory perception of the rats, as the action potentials of the neurons in the corticomedial amygdala of the brain fire in conjunction with every breath. Entrained cells are usually found deep in the amygdala, and cells that are entrained with the sniff cycle will fire together and regulate their activity. A nasal thermocouple was used to measure sniffing behavior and respiration rate, and after being implanted into one nostril of the rats, was able to record breathing patterns and their relation to neural activity. By measuring the temperature differences between inspired and expired air as well as fluctuations of pressure in the nasal cavity, the thermocouple was able to match the respiration rate with the action potentials of the firing neurons. Odorants and Triggers In order to better understand the pathways of anxiety cortical processing, particularly in relation to olfactory detection leading to predator odor fear, an array of pheromones and chemical stimuli was presented to the rats. The 4 monomolecular odorants administered were as follows: limonene, heptanone, isoamyl acetate, propyl butyrate, as well as the one pheromone, fox urine- containing 2,3,4 dihydro 2,5 trimethyl azoline (TMT) - which possesses predatorial properties that may elicit changes in neuronal activity. In line with prior research (8), TMT, a remarkably volatile compound found in red fox urine, has been shown to generate drastic behavioral and physiological effects in both rats and mice. Exposure to this predator odor in rats induces an increase in c-fos expression, an indirect marker of heightened neural firing, in several layers of the main olfactory bulb as well as increased vigilance via freezing behavior and elevated EEG and neck muscle EMG recordings. In accordance with previous studies, in which the rat was exposed to a wide range of emotional triggers, the 4 volatile stimuli were utilized to elicit neutral and positive responses. As the electrical recordings are focused on the waveforms engendered by the introduction of fox urine, using negative odors provide an articulate comparison to the enhanced effect of fox urine, while a positive odor would cause a paucity of activity in the brain regions associated with olfaction-induced fear. Volatile odors were chosen as control stimuli as they elicit a presumably neutral rat response and do not indicate possible threat. One of such areas of the olfactory system that modulates predator odor conditioned fear is the corticomedial amygdala (CMA). The CMA serves as the transmission center of direct and indirect input from the main and accessory olfactory systems and relays the information to the hypothalamic nuclei where hormone secretion is modulated to produce a defensive response. With a key role in facilitating innate emotional behaviors, unit electrical recordings of this region would shed considerable light on the neural circuitry that accompanies unconditioned pheromone - evoked anxiety. MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects The subjects of the Startle Response study were eight male Sprague-Dawley rats. Each subject was housed in a clear, plastic cage within a climate-controlled room and was kept on a 12-hour light cycle. Each subject was put through the same number of baseline tests and startle tests, but four of the subjects were put through one extra startle test session. However, the data [4-4] from that session is not included in this study due to technical errors (see experimental error section). The subjects of the Open Maze study were six male Sprague-Dawley rats. Prior to the experiment each subject was handled three times. Each time, they were handled by a different person and were held for three minutes each before being returned to their cages. The subjects of the Electrophysiology study were two male Sprague-Dawley rats. Each subject experienced the same craniotomy and electrophysiology recording procedures on different days of the week; however, the electrode was inserted in different coordinates in each of the rats’ brains as described in the procedure. Open Maze Experiment Apparatus The Maze test was conducted in an empty tub, which was put directly under a camera that detected the rat's’ movement and location and convey data and a video onto the Any-Maze software. The tub was divided into 12 sections in order to collect more accurate and precise data. A small circle divided the inner side of the tub from the edge and six sections dividing the tub into wedges. Within each wedge, an X was placed on which either an odorless gauze or gauze with the odor was placed. The ANY-Maze computer software program was used to record and analyze data. The ANY-Maze software consists of a camera that records and tracks the movement of the rat in the tub. Each section was programmed as a zone in the Any-Maze software. Each pheromone position was programmed as a point in the software. By dividing the tub in such a manner, data could be collected on how long the rat spent in each section and compared with the amount of time the rat spent in the section which contained the pheromone. Within the tub, the amount of time the rat spent in each section and also the average distance from the point was measured. Variables and Procedure Four different odors and their impact on the behavior of rats were tested. In this experiment, cat urine was used as the weak predator pheromone and TMT from fox urine was used as the strong predator pheromone. A positive pheromone, the urine of sexually active female rats, was used as a counterbalance for the predator pheromones. The neutral smell of propyl butyrate was used to compare both the positive and negative pheromones with. In order to better facilitate diffusion of the odor, 5-6 drops of the odor were placed on cotton gauze. The rest of the five cotton gauzes had 5-6 drops of water on them. Gauzes were taped down on the Xs in the tub. Baseline trials were run in order to get the rats acquainted with the tub and the gauzes, with each gauze containing drops of water instead of odorants. The data from the baseline testing was used to gain a better understanding of the way the rats interacted with their environment. All the rats were kept outside of the testing room with the door closed prior to each new odor. Changing the location of the odor each trial blocks the possibility that the rats avoided or gravitated towards a certain location despite the odor that is placed there. The lights inside the room were turned off to ensure that an unbalanced distribution of light did not affect the location of the rat. There was one red light in the room to allow for the camera to detect the rat's [4-5] movement. Five to six drops of cat urine was placed on one gauze, drops of water were put on the other five gauzes, and the gauzes were taped on the Xs in the tub. The first subject was brought into the room, held for 30 seconds, and then once it was placed in the center of the tub, the three minute test on the Any-Maze program began and the software measured the previously set parameters. Once the three minutes was complete, the rat was taken out of the tub and put back in its cage. The tub was cleaned with ethanol solution and the process continued until all the rats had finished the first trial. Once all the rats went through the first trial, the location of the odor was moved, more drops of the odor was added to the gauze with odor, and the process was repeated. When all three trials for this odor was completed, the process was repeated for the propyl butyrate, fox urine, and female rat urine. Acoustic Startle Response Apparatus The four apparatuses used in the Acoustic Startle Response test were Med Associates sound attenuating enclosures. Each chamber had a platform for the subjects to step on, as well as a load cell that recorded the force with which the subjects exerted on the platform when startled, a red light, a speaker, an automatic fan, and an amplifier. For the entirety of the testing, the speakers were emitting white noise at 65 decibels (dBA). An olfactometer was used to disperse odorants into the apparatuses. These odorants were fox urine (which contained 2,4,5 dihydro 2,5 trimethylthiazoline (TMT)), cat urine, female rat urine, and propyl butyrate (PB). Procedure Three days prior to beginning the study, all subjects were handled by humans for 3 minutes a day to minimize any stress that may be caused by handling. Before starting the Acoustic Startle Test sessions, each subject was put through three baseline tests in which their individual startle responses to varied sound pulses were measured. Subjects N50-N53 (Group A) were tested first and received a 2 minute acclimation period inside the chambers before the session began. During the session, sound pulses of 75, 85, 95, and 105 dBA were emitted in a pseudo-random sequence (the sequence was kept constant for each subject). The pulses had a rising time of 15 milliseconds and were played in 15 second intervals. Each baseline session was 10 minutes long. Approximately 5 minutes later, subjects N54-N59 (Group B) were tested using the same procedures as Group A. The chambers were cleaned with ethanol between every session. After the Baseline Test, vials containing 1 mL of each odorant were equipped with tubing that connected the vials to the olfactometer and the chambers. Group A was the first group to be tested. All subjects were placed in the four chambers in numerical order. Once placed in the chambers, the subjects were given a 5 minute acclimation period. Afterward, 30 acoustic pulses with an intensity of 95 dBA and rising time of 15 milliseconds were released in 15 second intervals. Following those 30 pulses (on the 31st pulse), the olfactometer released odorants into their respective chambers. During this portion of the test, the fan was running automatically to filter the odorants outside of the chamber (this took approx. 30 seconds). Afterward, odorants were released every fourth pulse (35th pulse, 39th pulse, 43rd pulse, etc.). Upon completing the session, Group A’s subjects were taken out of the chambers, placed into their respective cages, [4-6] and given 20 minutes rest. Group B was then placed into the chambers in and were then administered the test in the same fashion as Group A. Note that in between Sessions 2 and 3, a fourth baseline test was administered to both groups. The order of the odors presented across the sessions was counterbalanced to remove any possible confound of repeated testing. Craniotomy and Electrophysiology Recording Apparatus A stereotax apparatus was used to perform the electrophysiology recording. It contained ear and incisor bars to fix the rat’s head in place and dials that could be used to precisely move and lower an electrode in an x-y-z-coordinate system. A 12 mega ohm tungsten recording electrode from A-M system (Plano, Texas) was used. The electrode was connected to an A-M Systems model 1800 microelectrode amplifier; the signal was amplified 10,000 times and it was filtered between 500 Hz and 3000 Hz. The amplifier had 2 channels; 1 channel was used to detect neuronal activity and the second channel received input from the thermocouple to record respiration. The output of the amplifier was sent in parallel to the oscilloscope and a National Instruments digital acquisition PCI card, which allowed the signal to be digitized at 20 kHz. Procedure The craniotomy was performed on two rats over two days, following the same procedure each day. The rats were anesthetized with 3 grams per kilogram of urethane and isoflurane was used as a supplemental anesthetic. Ear bars and an incisor bar were used to fix the rat’s head in place in the sterotax and the top of the head was shaved. A scalpel was used to make an incision along the back of the head and the skin was clipped to the side to expose the skull. The connective tissue covering the skull was scraped aside. A window was drilled in the right parietal bone and a hole for the electrode was drilled in the left parietal bone. Electrophysiology recording was done over two days and the same procedure was followed on both days. First, the rat was positioned in the stereotax and grounded by connecting its tail to the faraday cage with wire in order to reduce electrical noise. A thermocouple was connected to observe and record the respiration of the rat. Differential recording of the brain was conducted by connecting 2 electrodes to the brain. A curved reference electrode was used to record extraneous electrical noise while the straight unit electrode was used to record electrical signals from neurons and noise. The unit electrode was positioned at bregma, the point where the frontal and parietal bones of the skull come together, and the coordinates of this point were measured. The target was then found and the electrode was lowered to the brain surface. On the first day of recording, the target was 3 mm posterior and 2 mm lateral to bregma. For the second day of recording, the target was changed to 3 mm posterior and 3.4 mm lateral to bregma in order to reach the amygdala. After the oscilloscope settings were set to 1 ms per division and 50 V per division, the electrode was lowered into the brain. On the first day, the electrode was only lowered until the multiple cells were found at 5.96 mm below the surface of the brain. On the second day of the recording, the electrode was lowered to 8.45 mm below the surface until neurons were observed. On the second day, researchers could hear breathing related neural activity, which is typically only observed in ventral sites that receive olfactory input; this was not [4-7] noticed in initial recordings. This would suggest that on the second day, the electrode was in a region that receives olfactory input. Odor tubes were connected to present odors to the rat. Six odors were presented to the rat on the first day, following the sequence: limonene, heptanol, isoamyl acetate, heptanone, propyl butyrate, and cat urine. On the second day, the cat urine was replaced by fox urine, but the rest of the odors presented were same and the sequence also remained the same. Odors were presented for 2 seconds with a 1 minute odor onset latency and each odor was presented 10 times. Once the recording was complete, the rat was decapitated and the rongeurs were used to remove the brain from the skull. The brain was placed in a 10% formalin and 30% sucrose solution for a week to preserve and harden it. After a week, the brain was sliced and sectioned with a vibratome and mounted onto microscope slides. The slides were stained with neutral red nissl staining and observed under a light microscope. RESULTS Open Maze The objective of the Open Maze experiment was to measure any aversion the rats may have to the various odors. This was done by placing the rats in an empty pool that acted as an open environment for the rats to freely explore. The movements of the rats were recorded through a camera placed over the maze that then analyzed the pattern of rat behavior through the ANY-Maze program. At the conclusion of the experiment, the amount of time each rat spent in each zone of the maze, the rats’ average speeds in each zone, and the average distance the rats spent away from the points of odor placement were measured and recorded. The data was then analyzed with two-tail T-tests that calculated the certainty of significant difference (t), the degrees of freedom (df), and the probability of getting the specific t-value for the given degree of freedom (p) in a (t, df, p) format. In order to analyze the data recorded for the experiment, two sets of inferential statistical analyses were performed. One two-tail T-test analysis was performed to compare the behavioral patterns of the rats in the zones containing the odors, indicated as “target,” and those zones opposite of them. As can be seen in Table I, only the pvalues comparing the target and opposite zones for all three types of the data recorded were less than 0.05. This suggests, with a 95% confidence, that there was a statistically significant difference in the time, speed, and distance away from points of odor placement the rats spent in each zone. The p-value for female rat urine the values was greater than 0.05, indicating that there was no real statistical significant difference between the time spent in the target and opposite zones. Table I: Two-tail T-test results comparing odorants to control trial Odorants T DF P Cat urine vs. propyl butyrate 3.0031 5 Time in zone [4-8] .0300 Fox urine vs. propyl butyrate 1.280 5 .2775 Rat urine vs. propyl butyrate 1.8673 5 .1208 Cat urine vs. propyl butyrate 8.9576 5 .0003 Fox urine vs. propyl butyrate 3.2546 5 .0226 Rat urine vs. propyl butyrate 2.7240 5 .0416 Cat urine vs. propyl butyrate 3.8940 5 .0115 Fox urine vs. propyl butyrate 4.3691 5 .0072 Rat urine vs. propyl butyrate .0115 Speed in zone Table 1: The table displays the (t, df, p) values calculated from the two-tail T-Test analysis for comparing the rats’ behaviors in the target and opposite zones for each odor. The general trend of the table indicates that mainly the rat behavior exhibited in the cat urine and fox urine trial showed a significant difference between their target and opposite zones. Distance from point 3.8880 5 The other type of inferential statistical analysis was used to analyze the data comparing the three odors: Cat Urine, Fox Urine, Female Rat Urine against the control: Propyl Butyrate. In general it was found that there was little difference in the time the rats spent in target zones in the odor trials than those in the control trial. For example, as shown in Table II , only the trial with cat urine has p-values less than 0.05, suggesting that it is the only odor that can be said with a 95% confidence to have shown a statistically significant increase in the time the rats spent exploring the cat odor zones compared to the propyl butyrate zones. Both the fox urine and rat urine trials showed no statistically significant difference from the control trial, indicating that any observed differences between time the rats spent in the fox and rat odor trials compared to the controlled trial were the result of chance rather than an innate aversion or attraction to the pheromones. When analyzing the Average Speed of the Rats in the Target Zones containing the odors compared to the Target Zones containing the control scent, it can be seen that there was a statistically significant difference between the speed the rats spent in the odors compared to the control. As shown in Table II, the p-values for cat urine, fox urine, and rat urine were 0.0002, 0.0149, 0.0269, respectively. All of these p- values are less than 0.05 which indicates at least a 95% confidence that the difference in rats’ speeds in the zones containing the odors compared to Propyl Butyrate is statistically significant, and not a result of chance. Finally when analyzing the data for the Average Distance the Rats kept away from the points in the target zones in the odor trials compared to those in the control trial, it was found that there was generally a statistically significant difference in the distance the rats stayed away from the points in the odor trials compared to the points in the control trial. For example, the p[4-9] values for Cat Urine and Fox Urine compared to Propyl Butyrate were 0.0418 and 0.0049, respectively. T-test results for both odors suggest with at least a 95% confidence that the rats on average stayed closer to the target points in the odor trials than those in the control trial. Table II: Two-tail T-test results comparing odorants to control trial Odorants T DF P Cat urine vs. propyl butyrate 3.0031 5 Time in zone Fox urine vs. propyl butyrate 1.280 .0300 5 .2775 Rat urine vs. propyl butyrate 1.8673 5 .1208 Cat urine vs. propyl butyrate 8.9576 5 .0003 Fox urine vs. propyl butyrate 3.2546 5 .0226 Rat urine vs. propyl butyrate 2.7240 5 .0416 Cat urine vs. propyl butyrate 3.8940 5 .0115 Fox urine vs. propyl butyrate 4.3691 5 .0072 Rat urine vs. propyl butyrate .0115 Speed in zone Table II: The table displays the (t, df, p) values calculated from the two-tail T-Test analysis for comparing the rats’ behaviors in the target zones from each odor trial: Cat Urine, Fox Urine, and Female Rat Urine to a control trial: Propyl Butyrate. In general, mainly the rat behaviors in the Cat and Fox Urine target zones showed a statistically significant difference from the rat behaviors exhibited in the target zones of the control trial. Distance from point 3.8880 5 1a) 1b) 1c) Figure 1: The data from the open maze tests with error bars representing 10% standard error. Comparison of the average time the rats spent in the target zone (zone with odorant) and the zone [4-10] opposite of it expressed in seconds for each odorant, comparison of the average speed in the target zone and the zone opposite of it expressed in meters per second for each odorant, and comparison of the average distance the rats traveled in the target point and the point opposite of it expressed in meters for each odorant. Acoustic Startle Chamber To monitor behavioral response in response to anxiety-inducing odors, rats were held in startle chambers to measure and record its startles. The timing and amount of force was recorded using the Startle Reflex program, which depicted the movements in waveforms. These waveforms varied in amplitude and wavelength depending on the variations of the startle. If the rat pushed off the bars of the chamber with greater force, the program would record a larger amplitude. Along the same lines, if the rat startled quicker, the wavelength would be shorter. The results are then entered into an ANOVA calculator for an analysis of variance using inferential statistics. Table III: Inferential Statistics and Analysis of Variance for Startle Test Results F P Baseline 10.08 <0.001 Habituation 17.82 <0.001 Cat urine 0.15 0.929 Fox urine 0.07 0.975 Female rat urine 0.09 0.965 Propyl butyrate 0.02 0.996 Foremost, the data collected from the baseline sessions conducted at the commencement of the experiment is represented in Figure 2a. One-Way ANOVA was used as a method of inferential statistics in order to analyze variability, and sphericity was assumed that all groups had equal variability. The results, seen in Table III, gives us above a 99% confidence that there is a significant main effect of pulse intensity and that a greater sound pulse intensity produces a greater startle reflex in the rats. Thus, reasonable confidence can be taken in the reliability of the rats and equipment. Additionally, the baseline trials give evidence that habituation takes place as well. As the mice are repeatedly exposed to the sound pulse, they appear to startle less as they become accustomed to the noise. In Figure 2a, at 95 db, the rats appear to startle relatively more [4-11] during the first baseline session; as the sessions progress, the startle response decreases. Inferential statistics indicates that this observation is significantly significant, as shown in Table III. This indicates that there is more variance than expected by chance. 2a) 2b) 2c) 2d) 2e) 2f) Figure 2: Startle amplitude of Olfactory Startle Tests Expressed in Arbitrary Units of Force. [4-12] The startle amplitude during the five baseline sessions, the thirty pre-odor baseline trials of the cat urine session, and the olfactory sessions of cat urine, fox urine, female rat urine, and propyl butyrate respectively. Included are error bars that represent the ±SEM. Data is recorded for each rat, which underwent four sessions of different odorants. Each rat was to be exposed to each odorant in a different order to counterbalance any lingering effects of an odor. Despite this intention, a glitch occurred in which half the rats received exposure to one odorant without proper startle measurement recording such that the data began recording 400 milliseconds after the pulse was emitted, effectively missing the rat’s startle reaction in the data collected. Nevertheless, each rat was exposed to each odorant and the session was redone to successfully record the startle peak value. In each session, the first thirty trials are baseline trials with the sound pulse given when no odorant is present. These trial results are averaged together in groups of ten, an example of which is shown in Figure 2b. The last forty trials of every session were in the presence of an odorant. By representing the data using graphs, one is able to see the average effects of various odors on the startle of the rats. The graph in Figure 2c represents the average startles of the eight rats in the presence of cat urine. Each bar is made up of interspersed trials, representing every fourth sound pulse trial. The first bar is the average of the peak value of the startle at the moment the odor is released, represented as the percentage of the average baseline value taken before each session. The following bars represent the average of the startles 15, 30, and 45 seconds after the odor release. The startles in the presence of cat urine odorant is higher than during the baselines without the odorant present; each bar is above the average baseline peak value. However; there is not much variance shown for the startles during the time between the odor releases. As shown in Table III, there is only 1% confidence. Based on the data represented in Figure 2d, fox urine appears to elicit the greatest behavioral response in a temporary, transient fashion. At the moment the odorant is released, the rat startle is dramatically greater than the startles seen during the baselines and in the period after the odor is released. Table III shows that these results are also inconclusive. Female rat urine, represented in Figure 2e, elicited a higher response than baseline though not much variance in the startle response. Analysis of variance in Table III shows that there is little confidence in effect of female rat urine. Propyl butyrate appears have the least variance in startle, as shown in Figure 2f. The startle in the presence of the odorant is greater than baseline, but there is little variability between the trials. Inferential statistics in Table III gives little confidence in the significance of any variance, showing any variance is likely due to chance. Electrophysiology Recording Information from the electrodes were processed amplified, then displayed on a monitor (Fig. 2). The waveforms displayed are the difference of the input from a reference electrode and the signals picked up by the test electrode. This reduces electrical noise in the data, allowing for the clear comparison of the morphologies between different electrical spikes of potential nearby action potentials. The recording of the data began after the electrode had reached a region where the amplitude of firing neurons reached around 2-3 times the ambient electrical noise. One cell could easily be observed to consistently fire with a similar amplitude and frequency throughout the experiment, regardless of the presence of an odorant. [4-13] After the recording of electrical input was complete, the data was analyzed by the program Offline Sorter. Every electrical signal that had an amplitude that exceeded a specified threshold was recorded as a point on a graph with the peak-valley value (an indicator of amplitude) on the x-axis and the time of the experiment on the y-axis (Fig. 3). The points were then sorted into two groups, two clumps of points most likely to have originated from the firing of a neuron near the inserted electrode. They were labeled cell 1a (region on the left) and cell 1b (region on the right). At the beginning of the recording, the difference in amplitude between the two cells is evident, but the amplitude of cell 1b decreases over the course of the experiment. About ⅗ through the experiment, the electrode began to record either less frequent or weaker action potentials, but began again to detect greater numbers of higher amplitude action potentials after around ⅘ of the recording had progressed. Figure 3: Electrical information displayed as multiple cells fire action potentials with amplitudes exceeding the preset threshold. Figure 4: Graph of the electrical spikes that exceed a set threshold with peak-valley values on the x-axis and time on the right, separated into two separate units. Figure 5: Histogram (periform raster) of the number of action potentials at specific times relative to the release of 5 neutral odors (heptanol, isoamyl acetate, propyl butyrate, heptanone, limonene) and one experimental odor (fox urine) at time 0 seconds to time 2 seconds (experimental cell 1b). The data was then processed by the Neuroexplorer software. It first generated periform rasters, histograms of the frequency of action potentials at certain times relative to the [4-14] presentation of an odorant from time 0 seconds to 2 seconds (Fig. 5). The first five odorants were presented as neutral control odors to the rat while the sixth, the fox urine, was the pheromone targeted by the experiment. The data was expected to match the results gathered from a different experiment conducted on a rat brain exposed to the same 5 neutral control odors as well as octadiene (Fig. 5). These cells demonstrated consistent and immediate spikes of activity during the odorant presentation, especially during the presentation of the heptanol, isoamyl acetate and octadiene.. It is clear that the firing of the experimental cells are far more random and less clustered around the two-second odor presentation window than predicted across all odorants. The bars over the histograms, marking the firing of neurons at specific times during each trial, also demonstrate the decrease in amplitude seen over time in Figure 2. Figure 6: Histogram (periform raster) of the number of action potentials at specific times relative to the release of 6 neutral odors (heptanol, isoamyl acetate, propyl butyrate, heptanone, limonene, and octadiene) at time 0 seconds to time 2 seconds (model cell 1c). Figure 7: Cross correlogram of the two experimental neurons detected by the inserted electrode displaying the frequency of firing of cell 1b at a specific time relative to the firing of cell 1a at time 0 seconds. Figure 8: Cross correlogram of two of the model neurons displaying the frequency of firing of cell 1c (model) at a specific time relative to the firing of cell 1a (model) at time 0 seconds. A cross correlogram was also generated by the program to indicate whether the proximity of the neurons was reflected in connected activity. This would be demonstrated if a neuron consistently fires at a specific time before or after another cell fires. A cross correlogram was generated for the two cells identified by the experimental electrode data (Fig. 6). The time of firing of cell 1b when compared to the firing of cell 1a (at time 0) seems random when compared to the near synchronization seen in the two cells recorded in a separate experiment (Fig. 7). Their spatial association is reflected in the close temporal relationship in their activity. [4-15] DISCUSSION This experiment focused on the relationship between predator pheromones and the behavioral responses and responses on the corticomedial amygdala of rats to these odorant stimuli. The three parts to the experiment analyzed anxiety in rats in different ways. The open maze experiment found that while rats do show apparent recognition of odors from rat and fox urine, there is not conclusive evidence to show that rats exhibit an innate fear or aversion response to these odorants. The acoustic startle experiment found that the rats exhibited a phasic fear response to the fox urine odorant, but had no definitive conclusions for any of the odorants presented due to a lack of statistically significant difference in startle reaction for all of the odorants. The data from the electrophysiology experiment did not support the hypothesis that neurons would respond differently to the presentation of a predator odor. Open Maze Several predictions were made on the reactions the rats would have to the various odorants. The first odorant tested was cat urine. Researchers predicted that the rats would show at least a slight aversion to the cat urine due to an innate fear within the rats that would cause aversion to the pheromones present in the odorant. However, the data (Fig. 1a) shows results that contrasted with the expected results. The inferential statistical analysis indicates that there was a significant difference in the amount of time the rat spent in the target zone and the zone opposite, but unlike expected, it spent a greater amount of time in the opposite zone than the target zone. These findings do not support that rats have an innate aversion to cat urine. This does, however, indicate that the rats recognized the cat urine odor as something more than a regular odor such as propyl butyrate (Fig. 1a). The researchers observed that the rat spent a great amount of time smelling the odorant, as indicated by the results. Although the design of the maze does not allow for a confident conclusion to be drawn with these results that there is an aversion, it can be noted that the rats do in fact have an innate recognition to cat urine. Since none of these rats had previously been exposed to a cat, they may not have immediately recognized that the odor belonged to a predator and may not have sensed any apparent danger. Furthermore, fox urine was also tested, and it was predicted that the fox urine would also cause an aversion as strong as or even stronger than the cat urine. Similar to with cat urine, the rats spent a significantly higher amount of time in the zone with the odorant than the opposite zone. Once again, this is peculiar because previous studies have shown that rats have an aversion to the pheromones released from fox urine (8). Conclusive evidence that the fox urine caused anxiety cannot be drawn from the maze. The neutral propyl butyrate did not show a significant difference in time spent in target and opposite zones, but both the cat urine and fox urine did, indicating recognition of the odors. The results suggest that an open environment may not be ideal for testing anxiety levels in rats because innate recognition of the odorants, although it may have caused some anxiety, was all that could be directly measured rather than an innate fear. The third odorant that was tested was female rat urine. Since all the rats used in the experiment were male rats, it was expected that the rats would spend the greatest amount of time in the zone with the female rat urine. It was found that for most of the rats (Fig. 1a), the rats did spend more time in the zone with the female rat urine than the opposite zone. The results were not statistically significant, but if more rats and more trials were used, the trend shows there might have been a statistical significance. The excitement of the rats may account for the lack of [4-16] statistically significant evidence even though the rats seemed sexually aroused. These qualitative measures indicate that there may have been more than simply recognition of the odorant and that there was some sexual excitement. Error Analysis The study was designed to test the effect olfactory senses have on innate fears in rats and the behavioral reactions rats have to pheromones from various animals. A factor of concern when evaluating the results is the potential effects of habituation in the rats in which repeated exposure to the environment lessens the response to the stimulus. After repeated exposure to the scents, realization that there was no apparent danger present may have affected the rats’ behaviors. Habituation to the maze itself is an important factor that may have affected rat behavior. The experimental design did not fully accommodate for changes in rat behavior that may have been due primarily to repeated exposure to the maze environment. This confounding variable was not accounted for since baseline testing was not done each day of experimentation due to time constraints. Daily measurement of a baseline would have blocked the effects of habituation to the maze environment and limited the amount of variables in the experiment. In future studies, varying the order of the odorants for each rat would also be beneficial as another precaution in order to block the effects of increased exposure to the maze environment. Acoustic Startle Chamber The first portion of testing in the startle response experiment measured a baseline reaction. In these sessions, two of which occurred on the first day of testing and one at the start of each day of testing with odors, no odor was introduced and the pulse sequence was a pseudorandom sequence of pulses ranging from 75 to 105 decibels in magnitude. Researchers expected that as the baseline testing progressed, the rats would demonstrate habituation i.e., the lessening of response to a stimulus as the subject becomes familiar with, and learns the harmlessness of, said stimulus. As predicted, a decrease in startle reaction was identified in the progression of the baseline sessions seen by the statistically significant difference between the first and fifth (last) baseline sessions, shown in figure 2a. This data indicates that the rats grew familiar with the chamber such that their neophobia (fear of new things) subsided and that the equipment is acting how it should in order to have confidence for the later results. Aside from the separate sessions for baseline testing, each session in which an odorant was presented commenced with a 30 baseline trials of 95 decibels 15 seconds apart. Researchers expected to observe a decrease in startle magnitude, as evidence of additional habituation, however, the results showed quite the opposite. Figure 2b shows that as the baseline trials progress, the average magnitude actually increases. While unexpected, this can be easily explained in terms of the study. As 3 out of the 4 sessions with odorant rats experienced succeeded other sessions (and the rats were given the scents in all different orders) it is quite likely that the rats had some recollection of the previous sessions, which may have included odorants like fox urine and cat urine with their predator pheromones, and vague memory that the odorants were only introduced after a while. Thus, the increasing startle response results from anxiety from the rats anticipating the odorants’, which they might expect to be a negative stimulus like the predatory pheromones, introduction coming nearer and nearer. [4-17] In the actual experimental portion of this test, i.e. the sessions in which odorant was presented, researchers had several predictions about what the data would show. Concerning the cat urine, researchers predicted that the rats may not show full-blown anxiety-induced behaviors as a result of presentation of the odorant. The data shown in Figure 2c demonstrated that while there was an increase in the startle response as compared to the baseline, this was not a significant increase. This finding indicates little confidence that such a reaction is from innate fear and instead points to a heightened response as a result of neophobia or some other confounding variable. The next odor researchers used was female rat urine which was intended to be a positive stimulus to the rats (as the urine of the females would contain female sex pheromones)and as such, it was predicted that rats would not show a significantly elevated startle response to the odor. The data, presented in figure 2e, showed that there was an elevation in startle response from the baseline trials, though, this difference was not statistically significant. Once again this indicates that there is little confidence that the increased response is a result of anxiety from exposure to the female rat pheromones. The observed heightened response can be mostly likely explained by the fact that the rats are sexually inexperienced, such that this exposure would have been their first encounter of such sex pheromones so that the rats were likely quite sexually aroused, possibly resulting in a hypersensitivity to startle. Otherwise the heightened response could be the result of neophobia, similar to the reaction to cat urine. The third odorant presented was the propyl butyrate, or the pineapple oil, which acted as a control odor in the experiment as it was the only odorant presented which contained no pheromones. Because it was a control, researchers expected no enhanced startle response to the odorant. The data, summarized in figure 2f, shows that the rats actually had the greatest overall increase in startle response relative to its baseline (even though it technically was not statistically significant) which was quite unexpected. It seems plausible that the elevated response was the result of irritability from the rats having been kept in the small chamber for an extended period of time and for more sessions than they would customarily experience. Finally, researchers predicted that the rats would show an elevated, and likely sustained, startle response in the presence of fox urine. The data shown in figure 2d however confirms most of this prediction as it shows an increased response to the sound pulse when the odorant was presented, but the increased response is only apparent at the sound pulse right when the odorant is presented. Data such as this suggests that the rats definitely experienced innate anxiety to the pheromones present in the fox urine, but that they experienced more of a phasic fear response, characteristic of phobias, than the sustained anxiety, characteristic of generalized anxiety disorder, which researchers expected to observe. Despite the apparent trends however, the inferential analysis determined this test to be inconclusive, preventing the establishment of definitive conclusion. Even still, the fox urine, as implied by the data, is the only odorant which shows an anxiety response and hence the only odorant viable for use in further research on anxiety. Error Analysis This experiment was designed to examine rat’s innate anxiety (or lack thereof) to different odorants as measured by their tendency to startle (a common anxiety response) upon hearing a 95 decibel sound pulse. Although researchers made extensive efforts to avoid confounding variables in the data collection, technology errors and timing related inconsistencies [4-18] have inadvertently done so. A major error in the testing was based around coding problem which caused a delay in data collection such that the recording to miss the actual startle response and showed data that falsely indicated little to no reaction to any of the odorants presented. While the data was collected a second time at a later date, this error caused what should have been a first exposure to odors, therefore definitely observing innate fear, to become a second exposure, which introduces new confounds such as habituation or conditioned fear from previous experience of being startled. Electrophysiology Recording One cell was recorded from the first rat. It was not located in the corticomedial amygdala so it was not surprising that no relationship was found between odor presentation and frequency of action potentials. Two cells were recorded from the second rat; it was expected that they were located in the medial nucleus of the amygdala because background neural activity related to breathing was heard. However, when the action potentials were compared with the breathing data from the thermocouple in Figure 9, there was no clear relationship between the timing of respiration and the timing of the firing. This indicates that although the electrode was recording from the correct region of the brain, the cells that were recorded did not process olfactory input. Action potentials from the two different cells were divided into separate units during data analysis. However, neither cell showed activity that suggested a relationship between odor onset and the frequency of action potentials. Experimental cell 1b in Figure 5 did not show a clear increase in firing in response to any of the odors. Therefore, it can be concluded that the recorded cells were not responding to odors. However, it is still possible that they were located in the medial nucleus of the amygdala, because only 10-15% of cells in this area of the brain are typically responsive to odors. It was expected that during the recordings, the cells would increase firing during the 2 seconds of odor presentation. Sample data that was recorded prior to the current experiment has been included in Figure 6 as an example of the expected cell responses. The cells in this figure show a distinct increase in firing with the presentation of odors. It was hoped that the cells recorded during the experiment would show the largest increase in firing in response to the presentation of fox and cat urine. Such a response in the corticomedial amygdala would indicate that this region processes anxiety related odors and pheromones. Future Implications This study was an investigation into the effect of olfactory stimulation on phasic and sustained fear in the neurons of the corticomedial amygdala. Future researchers may want to work to examine the effects of stronger predator pheromones, such as TMT in fox urine. The experiment conducted involving fox urine showed the greatest potential for larger response in the amygdala. As the neuronal circuitry of a rat and human are strikingly similar, the application of rats in such studies would provide scientists with a more lucid understanding of the role discrete areas of the brain play in anxiety disorders and behaviors. [4-19] REFERENCES 1. Bind RH. The role of pheromonal responses in rodent behavior: future directions for the development of laboratory protocols. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2013: 124–129. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3624779/ 2. Curzon P, Zhang M, Radek RJ. The behavioral assessment of sensorimotor processes in the mouse: acoustic startle, sensory gating, locomotor activity, rotarod, and beam walking. Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. 2009 [Internet]. [cited 2015 Jul 27] 2nd edition. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press; 2009. Chapter 8. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK5236/ 3. Davis M, Walker D, Miles L, Grillon C. Phasic vs. sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs. anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009: 105-135. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2795099/ 4. Harris A, D'eath R, Healy S. Environmental enrichment enhances spatial cognition in rats by reducing thigmotaxis (wall hugging) during testing. Animal Behaviour: 1459-1464. 5. Koch M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol. 1999 Oct; 59(2): 107-28. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347209001183 6. Lowry R. One-way ANOVA. VassarStats. 2015; [Internet]. [cited 27 July 2015]. Available from: http://vassarstats.net/anova1u.html 7. Stoelting ANY-maze flexible video tracking for neuroscience experiments. [updated 2011]. [cited 2015 Jul 26]. Available from: http://www.anymaze.com/ 8. Takahashi L. Olfactory systems and neural circuits that modulate predator odor fear. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014; [Internet]. [cited 2015 Jul 28]: 1-13. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3949219/ 9. Valsamis B, Schmid S. Habituation and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rodents. Journal of Visualized Experiments. 2011; [Internet]. [cited 2015 Jul 27]: (55): 3446. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3217252/ 10. Xu, Wenjin, Donald A. Wilson. Odor-evoked activity in the mouse lateral entorhinal cortex. Neuroscience. 2012; [Internet]. [cited 2015 Jul 29]: 12–20 p. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0306452212008159 11. [updated 2015]. Silver Spring (MD): Understanding the Facts | Anxiety and Depression Association of America, ADAA.: [cited 2015 Jul 29] Available from: http://www.adaa.org/understanding-anxiety [4-20]