The Role of Hot and Cool Executive Functioning in Pre-Readers Story Comprehension and Response to Comprehension Intervention Andrea DeBruin-Parecki Kelly B. Cartwright LRA 2014 Reading comprehension is essential for academic success, yet a significant proportion of children do not achieve basic comprehension proficiency by fourth grade. The NAEP Reading Assessment measures reading and comprehension skills by having students read age-appropriate passages and answer related questions. The cognitive targets being assessed are the understanding of written text, the development and interpretation of meaning, and the appropriate use of meaning in regard to the type of text, purpose and situation, all skills directly related to reading comprehension. The 2013 NAEP Report results reported no improvement in reading for 4th grade students (Anne E. Casey, January 2014). Numerous cognitive skills predict successful reading comprehension, such as executive skills (Cartwright, 2012; Sesma, Mahone, Levine, Eason, & Cutting, 2009), Theory of Mind (ToM) (Cartwright & Guajardo, 2011; Lysaker, Tonge, Gauson, & Miller, 2011), oral language comprehension (Nation, Cocksey, Taylor, & Bishop, 2010; Nation & Snowling, 2004), and vocabulary (Coyne et al., 2010, Muter, Hulme, Snowling, & Stevenson, 2004; Ouellette, 2006). Additionally, explicit instruction in inferential comprehension strategies, such as predicting, inferencing, and making connections to prior knowledge, produces significant gains in reading comprehension in elementary students (Bianco et al., 2012, Duffy et al., 1987; Shanahan, et al., 2010) and narrative comprehension in preschool students (DeBruin-Parecki & Squibb, 2011; 1 Roberts, 2013; Van Kleeck 2008). Literal comprehension instruction often includes explicit discussion of vocabulary during story reading (Zucker, Cabell, Justice, Pentimonti, & Kaderavek, 2013), which produces significant increases in vocabulary in elementary students (Brett, Rothlein, & Hurley, 1996) and in preschool students (Blewitt, Rump, Shealy, & Cook, 2009; Strasser, Larraín, & Lissi, 2013); these are important findings, given the strong predictive relation between reading comprehension and vocabulary (Muter, et al., 2004; Ouellette, 2006). However, comprehension instruction of any type, literal or inferential, occurs infrequently in preschool and early elementary school, when curricula focus predominantly on letter- and wordlevel skills (Duke & Carlisle, 2011; Hawken, Johnston, & McDonnell, 2008; Pressley, 2002). Furthermore, when it occurs, comprehension instruction in preschool classrooms focuses predominantly on literal comprehension rather than on inferential strategies that can be generalized to new situations (Zucker, et al., 2013). In order to address problems with comprehension in later elementary school, researchers must partner with practitioners to better understand how to foster inferential comprehension skills earlier in development (DeBruin & Squibb, 2011; Duke & Carlisle, 2010; Shanahan, et al., 2010). Unfortunately, limited work has been done on teaching comprehension strategies to preschoolers, in examining the role of cognitive contributors to narrative comprehension in preschoolers, or in the effects of cognitive skills on preschoolers’ response to explicit, inferential comprehension interventions. To be able to accomplish this work, many pieces have to be put into place including a comprehension strategy curriculum. The current study used Enhancing Preschoolers in Comprehension (EPIC), a supplementary comprehension strategy curriculum that occurred over a ten-week period twice a week for a weekly total of three hours of whole group instruction and 2 linked small group activities (DeBruin-Parecki, 2013; DeBruin-Parecki & Pribesh, in press; DeBruin-Parecki & Vaughn, 2014; DeBruin-Parecki & Squibb, 2011). The curriculum is designed to have the teacher focus on instruction of four related comprehension skills for prereaders: Connection to Life (activating background knowledge), Predicting (inferencing), Retelling (summarizing), and Increasing vocabulary. There are four units in EPIC: Friendship, Imagination, Other People/Other Places, and Change Over Time. For further information about EPIC including choice of focus strategies and how lesson plans were constructed, see DeBruinParecki & Pribesh, in press; DeBruin-Parecki & Squibb, 2011. To be able to examine the idea that cognitive contributors have a link to narrative comprehension in preschoolers or compare the effects of cognitive skills on preschoolers’ response to a supplementary inferential comprehension strategy curriculum, literal comprehension instruction, and typical comprehension instruction, a study was launched. Researchers were most interested in assessing both the impact of hot and cool executive skills on the effectiveness of a supplementary inferential comprehension strategy curriculum in preschool classrooms, and the differential effects of the types of comprehension instruction. Method Two university researchers partnered with three masters level university lab school preschool teachers who delivered inferential (EPIC), literal, or typical comprehension instruction to their classes for ten weeks. The theme for instruction across all classes was Friendship. The study had a quasi-experimental, pre-test posttest control group design. Children (n = 49; mean age = 4 years, 7 months; 35.8% minority students, 41.5% girls) included 17 3-year-olds, 17 4year-olds, and 15 5-year-olds. Children were pretested on executive skills (cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and working memory), ToM, and receptive and expressive vocabulary, using the 3 Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) (Dunn & Dunn, 2007) and Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT) (Williams, 2007), respectively. Additionally, children’s narrative comprehension skills were assessed with the Early Literacy Skills Assessment (ELSA) (DeBruin-Parecki, 2004). Posttest measures of expressive vocabulary and narrative comprehension were repeated, dependent measures. Pre-Intervention Results One-way ANOVAs by age group (3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and 5-year-olds) indicated 5year-olds scored significantly higher than 3-year-olds on cognitive flexibility, F (2, 45) = 4.57, p < .05; similarly, 5-year-olds scored higher than 3- and 4-year-olds on working memory, F (2, 44) = 5.44, p < .01. No other age differences emerged. All executive skills and ToM were significantly, positively related to narrative comprehension, as expected. Cognitive flexibility predicted significant variance in expressive vocabulary (7%, p < .05) beyond all other pretest variables and age, consistent with comprehension findings in older children and adults (Cartwright, 2002, 2007; Cartwright et al. 2010). ANCOVAs indicated children high on cognitive flexibility or ToM had significantly higher expressive vocabulary than children low on these variables at pretest, F (1, 39) = 5.26, p < .05 and F (1, 36) = 9.56, p < .01, but no similar effects on comprehension emerged at pretest. Post-Intervention Results Repeated measures ANCOVAs revealed inferential comprehension instruction produced significant gains in expressive vocabulary and narrative comprehension, even when all pretest variables were controlled, F (2, 26) = 6.25, p < .01 and F (1, 25) = 5.31, p < .05, respectively (see Figure 1); and literal comprehension instruction produced significant gains in expressive vocabulary over the typical instruction control, F (2, 26) = 6.25, p < .01. Moreover, children in 4 the inferential comprehension condition who were high on cognitive flexibility at pretest made significantly greater gains in comprehension than those low on cognitive flexibility (who made no significant comprehension gains; see Figure 2). These results held, even when expressive vocabulary was controlled, F (2, 24) = 4.99, p < .05. Similar effects on responsiveness to inferential comprehension instruction emerged for ToM. 5 Discussion These data indicate cognitive flexibility and theory of mind are important correlates of expressive vocabulary and precursors to narrative comprehension development; further, these skills appear to enable preschoolers’ successful response to effective inferential comprehension instruction such as the comprehension strategy instruction that composes the content of EPIC. Because cognitive flexibility and ToM can be assessed briefly, they may provide important screening tools to indicate preschoolers’ readiness to respond to comprehension strategy instruction in preschool classrooms. Furthermore, executive skills and ToM can be taught, yielding improvements in reading comprehension in elementary students (Cartwright, 2002; Lysaker, et al., 2011). Thus, future research should explore the possibility of both including supplementary focused comprehension strategy curricula, and training preschoolers on these skills to prepare them for effective comprehension strategy instruction. 6 References Annie E. Casey Foundation. (2014, January). Early Reading Proficiency in the United States. Retrieved November 21, 2014 from http://www.aecf.org/~/media/Pubs/Initiatives/KIDS%20COUNT/E/EarlyReadingProficie ncy/EarlyReadingProficiency2014.pdf Bianco, M., Pellenq, C., Lambert, E., Bressoux, P., Lima, L. & Doyen, A. (2012). Impact of early code-skill and oral comprehension training on reading achievement in the first grade. Journal of Research in Reading, 34 (4), 427-455. Blewitt, P., Rump, K. M., Shealy, S. E., & Cook, S. A. (2009). Shared book reading: When and how questions affect young children’s word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 294-304. Brett, A., Rothlein, L., & Hurley, M. (1996). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories and explanations of target words. The Elementary School Journal, 96, 415-422. Cartwright, K. B. (2002). Cognitive development and reading: The relation of reading-specific multiple classification skill to reading comprehension in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 56-63. Cartwright, K. B. (2007). The contribution of graphophonological-semantic flexibility to reading comprehension in college students: Implications for a less simple view of reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 39, 173-193. Cartwright, K. B. (2012). Insights from cognitive neuroscience: The importance of executive function for early reading development and education. Special Issue on Neuroscience Perspectives - Early Education and Development, 23, 1-13. 7 Cartwright, K. B., & Guajardo, N. R. (2011, March). A longitudinal study of the role of theory of mind in elementary students’ metacognition and reading comprehension. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada. Cartwright, K. B, Marshall, T. R., Dandy, K., & Isaac, M. C. (2010). The development of graphophonological-semantic cognitive flexibility and its contribution to reading comprehension in beginning readers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 61-85. Coyne, M.D., McCoach, D. B., Loftus. S., Zipoli, R., Ruby, M., Creveccouer, Y. C., & Kapp, S. (2010). Direct and extended vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Investigating transfer effects. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 3(2), 93-120. DeBruin-Parecki, A. (2004). The Early Literacy Skills Assessment (ELSA): Violet’s Adventure/La Aventura de Violetta (Carmen Magana, translator). Ypsilanti, Michigan: High/Scope Press. DeBruin-Parecki, A. (April 2013). The value of pre-reader comprehension strategy instruction: Giving young children tools to build understanding. Paper presented at the International Reading Association Conference: San Antonio, Texas. DeBruin-Parecki, A. & Pribesh, S. (in press). Evening the playing field: The importance of teaching all children comprehension strategies. In A. DeBruin, A. Van Kleeck, & Gear, S. Pre-Reader Comprehension: An Essential Building Block to Becoming a Successful Reader. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing. 8 DeBruin-Parecki, A. & Squibb, K. (2011). Promoting at-risk preschool children’s comprehension through research-based strategy instruction. Reading Horizons, 51 (1), 41-62. DeBruin-Parecki, A., & Vaughn, E. S. (April 2014). The Value of Comprehension Strategy Instruction for Head Start Children. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association: Philadelphia. PA. Duffy, G. G., Roehler, L. R., Sivan, E., Rackliffe, G., Book, C., Meloth, M. S….& Bassiri, D. (1987). Effects of explaining the reasoning associated with using reading strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 347-368. Duke, N. K., & Carlisle, J. F. (2011). The development of comprehension. In M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, and P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research, Vol. IV (pp. 199-228). London: Routledge. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M (2007). PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. Hawken, L. S., Johnston, S. S., & McDonnell, A. P. (2005). Emerging literacy views and practices: Results from a national survey of Head Start teachers. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 25, 232-242. Lysaker, J T., Tonge, C., Gauson, D., & Miller, A. (2011). Reading and social imagination: What relationally oriented reading instruction can do for children. Reading Psychology, 32, 520-566. Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665-681. 9 Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S. H., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1031-1039. Nation, K. & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language sklls contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342-356. Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554-566. Pressley, M. (2002). Metacognition and self-regulated comprehension. In A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction (Chapter 13, pp. 291309). Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Roberts, K. L. (2013). Comprehension strategy instruction during parent-child shared reading: An intervention study. Literacy Research and Instruction, 52, 106-129. Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 232-246. Shanahan, T., Callison, K., Carriere, C., Duke, N. K., Pearson, P. D., Schatschneider, C., & Torgesen, J. (2010). Improving reading comprehension in kindergarten through 3rd grade: A practice guide (NCEE 2010-4038). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from whatworks.ed.gov/publications/practiceguides. Strasser, K., Larraín, A., & Lissi, M. R. (2013). Effects of storybook reading style on comprehension: The role of word elaboration and coherence questions. Early Education and Development, 24, 616-639. 10 Van Kleeck, A. (2008). Providing preschool foundations for later reading comprehension: The importance of and ideas for targeting inferencing in storybook-sharing interventions. Psychology in the Schools, 45(7), 627-643. Williams, K. T. (2007). EVT-2: Expressive vocabulary test. Minneapolis, Minn: Pearson Assessments. Zucker, T. A., Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Pentimonti, J. M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2013). The role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1425-1439. 11