Understanding intuition from the human resource practitioners’ perspective Eugene Sadler-Smith, Surrey Business School, University of Surrey1 Abstract This research aimed to understand intuition from the perspective of the Human Resources’ practitioner. We used a novel linguistic methodology based on de-nominalization to elicit participants’ experiences of intuition (N = 124). Based on our analysis we: refined the construct of intuition through redefinition; interpreted the subjective experience of intuition as comprising three dimensions (‘intuiting’, ‘intuition’, and ‘enacting’); discovered that intuitive affect has two facets (‘bodily awareness’ and ‘cognitive awareness’). We outline the theoretical implications and the relevance of our findings for Human Resources practice and make suggestions for further qualitative and phenomenological studies of intuition. Keywords: decision making; intuition; Human Resources Introduction It is well –established that human resource practitioners, in common with other organizational actors, make us of intuition in decision making and problem solving (Miles & Sadler-Smith, 2014). Although there has been a surge of interest in intuition (e.g. Dane & Pratt, 2007; Dörfler & Akermann, 2013; Hodgkinson & Clarke, 2007; Miller & Ireland, 2005; Sadler-Smith & Shefy, 2004; Salas, Rosen & DiazGranados, 2010; Sinclair & Ashkanasy, 2005) there is a scarcity of qualitative field work and little intuition research in the area of Human Resources (Akinci & Sadler-Smith, 2012; Miles & Sadler-Smith, 2014). Given that intuition is especially relevant in the social and expert judgements that are involved in Human Resource practice the objective this research was to elicit first-hand accounts from Human Resource practitioners of their subjective experiences of intuition. Background Taking the ‘intuitive judgment (the outcome)-versus-intuiting (the process)’ distinction (Dane and Pratt, 2007) as a starting point, we sought a novel way to access subjects’ experiences of intuition; in doing so we turned to linguistics. In English grammar the term ‘nominalization’ means ‘noun-like’, therefore to ‘denominalize’ a noun is to “make it less noun-like, or turn it into a verb, adjective, or some other grammatical category” (Payne, 1997: 94), e.g. ‘intuition’ renders ‘intuit’. Through denominalizing we argue that using the verb (intuit) as the basis for empirical inquiry could render intuition more transparent. This allows research participants to retrieve and reflect on direct personal occurrences of the action instantiated in the verb (intuiting), rather than undertake de-contextualized musings on the meaning of the noun (intuition). So, rather than asking Human Resources practitioners the somewhat dull and de-personalized question ‘what is intuition?’ or ‘what does intuition mean to you?’ we instead de-nominalized ‘intuition’ and asked: ‘what happens when you intuit?’ formulated as a sentence-completion task: “when I intuit...’. But why did we choose Human Resources as our research context? One reason is that intuition is relevant for and used in the decision making processes in Human Resources 1 Email for correspondence: e.sadler-smith@surrey.ac.uk practice (Miles & Sadler-Smith, 2014). However, a problem is that Human Resources organizational decision makers often overestimate the validity of their intuition whilst simultaneously underestimating the validity of paper-and-pencil tests. As Highhouse (2008) noted: one of the greatest achievements of IO psychology has been the development of selection decision aids, whilst one of its greatest failures has been an inability to convince employers to use them. Highhouse also lays some of the blame at the door of popular books which “extol the virtues of intuitive decision making” (2008: 334) such as Malcolm Gladwell’s 2005 best-seller Blink: The power of thinking without thinking. However, although there is a plethora of knowledge regarding applicant reactions to employee selection procedures (Diab, Pui, Yankelevich and Highhouse, 2011) there is a dearth of understanding about why Human Resources practitioners find intuition so appealing and how they use it in employee selection decisions (Miles & Sadler-Smith, 2014). Many Human Resources practitioners are generalists involved in recruitment, selection, reward and learning and development, use intuition in selection decisions (as research suggests) then it is also likely that they use in in Human Resource Development (HRD) decision making as well, hence understanding intuition is relevant to HRD research and practice. Method, Sample and Procedure We asked our Human Resources’ participants ‘what happens when you intuit?’ We did so in the convenient setting of professional development seminars on the topic of intuition. Our participants elected to attend a seminar for personal and professional development purposes. The seminars (five in total, lasting an hour-and-half each) were conducted by the author and held at various locations across the south east of the UK. The total number of participants was 124. At the beginning of each seminar, following a brief introduction, participants were requested to recollect an occasion (or occasions) during which they had experienced an intuition. They were allowed a short time during which to reflect on this experience, and from their recollections and reflections were asked to construct a general answer the question ‘what happens when you intuit?’ They did this by completing, on a pre-printed form, the firstperson statement ‘when I intuit…’ Participants were also invited to indicate their gender (58 per cent female, 33 per cent male, and nine percent undeclared). Completed forms were collected-in at the end of the seminar, and responses were typed-up verbatim in MS Word and transferred to MS Excel for further coding, sorting, and cross-tabulating. Participation was voluntary and anonymity was guaranteed. Data Analysis The data set was 124 individual responses; these varied in length from six words (e.g. “when I intuit I generate ideas”) to 44 words (i.e. “when I intuit my gut tells me I need to seek more information and ask more questions to better understand a situation. My head starts to question what I am seeing or hearing and I feel I need to do something or take action”). There were 528 different words and 2082 words in total in the data set. We decomposed participants’ responses into ‘units of text’ which were then analysed and coded. The final coding scheme consisted of 16 1st order concepts which were labelled according to their content, for example ‘Experiences’, ‘Gut reactions’, ‘Anticipating’, etc. The labelling of the concepts was participant-based (Langley & Abdallah, 2011) reflecting as closely as possible the words participants used. We then moved from participant-based concepts to researcher-based themes representing six superordinate categories as follows: ‘Antecedents’; ‘Processes’; ‘Bodily awareness’; ‘Cognitive awareness’; ‘Outcomes’; ‘Behaviours’. We then stepped-up the level of abstraction in the analysis one more time and classified the categories into three superordinate aggregate dimensions, i.e. ‘Intuiting’, ‘Intuition’, and ‘Enacting’. Discussion of Findings The overall analytical approach and the derived data structure is consistent with the method recommended by Gioia, Corley and Hamilton (2013) for inductive research, as implemented in, for example, Nag, Corley and Gioia (2007) and Pratt, Rockmann, and Kaufmann (2006). The data structure is shown in Figure 1 and sample units of text for each of the 1st order concepts are shown in Table 1. On the basis of the analysis of our findings we define intuition, as follows: positively- or negatively-valenced feeling states, manifesting cognitively and somatically, arising rapidly and subconsciously, informed by prior learning and experiences, affording proximate evaluations which guide subsequent behaviours. Figure 1. Data structure (dimensions, themes and concepts) Experiences Antecedents Patterns Intuiting Automatic Processes Fast Subconscious Feelings Bodily Awareness Gut reactions Intuition Sense Cognitive Awareness Mental images Insight Outcomes Negative signal Positive signal Enacting Anticipating Deciding Behaviours Judging Questioning Table 1. Data structure and sample units of text (numerals in brackets indicate which participant is being quoted) Dimension Theme 1st Order Concept Sample unit of text Intuiting Antecedents Experiences “Elements of my experience come together to shape my thoughts and actions” (19) Patterns “I recognise cues” (31) Automatic “Feel something to be true without analysing it” (22) Fast “Make decision quickly” (33) Subconscious “Feels as though answer/response appears from nowhere” (77) Feelings “Whole body feeling” (113) Gut reactions “Feelings in my stomach” (10) Sense “Get a sense” (2) Mental images “Listen to that small voice in my head” (28) Insight “Make productive connections between previous unconnected or apparently unrelated ideas” (105) Negative signal “I know something is wrong” (56) Positive signal “Something clicks inside that I recognize as clearly right” (5) Anticipating “Sense that someone is going to say something” (17) Deciding “Guides me towards concrete direction, decision or action” (20) Judging “Cannot rationalize an absolute decision so I make a casting vote based on intuition” (38) Questioning “Ask myself what's really going on here” (50) Processes Intuition Bodily Awareness Cognitive Awareness Enacting Outcomes Behaviours What happens then you intuit? One of contributions of this research is that we are able to arrive empirically at a phenomenological, subjective experience-based definition of intuition. Further contributions are as follows: (i) as noted above, previous research distinguishes between ‘intuiting’ (process) and ‘intuitive judgement’ (outcome) (Dane & Pratt, 2007), see Figure 2a, but we identify three phases in the process (Figure 2b); (ii) we found that Human Resources practitioners’ subjective experience of intuition has two facets: ‘bodily [somatic] awareness’ and ‘cognitive awareness’; (iii) This research bridges intuition’s antecedents and its behavioural outcomes, and the subjective experience of intuition is at the mid-point of the process (Figure 2b); (iv) our inductively-derived term ‘enacting’ subsumes Dane and Pratt’s (2007) intuitive judgment and captures a wider range of potential outcomes. Figure 2a. ‘Intuiting-intuitive judgement’ model (see Dane & Pratt, 2007) Intuiting •Intuition in its input state: a non-conscious, fast, holsitic processing mode Intuitive judgement •Intuition in its outcome state: an affectively charged judgement Figure 2b. ‘Intuiting-intuition-enacting’ model Intuiting • Rapid, automatic, subconscious processing; response to recognized patterns, supported/enabled by informational substrates from past experiences/prior learnings Intuition • Experienced as: (1) bodily awareness: viscerally (‘gut reactions’) and less specific ‘feelings’; (2) cognitive awareness: general ‘sense’ and mental images (visual, auditory) Enacting • Intuitions provide positive/negative evaluations and insights enacted in approachversus-avoid behaviours, decision making, problem solving, and creativity 5 What happens then you intuit? Our model supports existing research; for example, the way intuition is interpreted in our analysis is fully consistent with the view of ‘intuition-as-expertise’, ‘intuitive expertise’ and ‘expert intuition’ (Sadler-Smith & Shefy, 2004; Kahneman & Klein, 2009; Salas et al., 2010) and Klein’s recognition-primed decision model, namely: ‘intuiting’ entails rapid, nonconscious processing based on past experiences and prior learnings. In the recognition-primed decision (RPD) model (Klein, 1998; 2003; Klein, Calderwood & Clinton-Cirocco, 1988) decision makers recognize salient cues (as one of our participants wrote: “I recognize cues” [Participant 31]) and match these to extant patterns and prototypes of similar situations or people (for example, “what’s likely to happen, what’s likely to work” [Participant 48]). In the RPD model behavioural responses, in the form of ‘actions scripts’, are executed on the basis of a matching process (Klein, 2003). The process, as far as the intuitor is concerned, appears to be largely automatic involving little in the way of effortful cognition (see Stanovich, 2009). Subjects “understand instinctively without the need for conscious reasoning” (Participant 66), so much so that the experience of intuiting provides evaluations arrived at with minimal conscious cognition (“it is just right” [Participant 68]). Furthermore, the process is perceived to be fast. Whether or not the intuitive response turns out to be effective will depend on the intuitor’s situational awareness (Klein et al., 1988), her expertise (Ericsson, Prietula & Cokely, 2007) and the validity of the decision making task or environment (i.e. how predictable the actual outcomes are on the basis of the perceived cues, see: Kahneman & Klein, 2009). Implications Our research addresses a significant shortcoming in current theorizations by shedding light on the nature and role of not just intuition but also, and specifically, ‘intuitive affect’ in Human Resources practice. Since intuition “operates at the nexus of thinking and feeling” (Hodgkinson et al.,2009: 278) viewing cognitions and affect as distinct (e.g. Panksepp, 2003) is potentially unhelpful, whereas conceiving the boundary as being more permeable opens the way to a potentially richer conceptualization of the phenomenon of intuition. Clore (1992) is helpful in this regard: he suggests that feelings can be grouped into three categories: affective feelings: valenced subjective experiences encompassing moods and emotions; bodily feelings: reflections of physical processes such as hunger or pain; cognitive feelings: experiential states that accompany cognitive processes such as feelings of familiarity (Clore, 1992; Greifeneder, Bless, & Pham, 2010). The experiential state of ‘intuition’ was found to have two elements ‘bodily awareness’ (‘gut feel’) and ‘cognitive awareness’ (‘hunch’); these can be mapped broadly on to Clore’s (1992) distinction between bodily feelings and cognitive feelings (although this was not Clore’s original intention). There is supporting evidence for this assertion since cognitive feelings have been found to be related to intuition in that greater reliance on cognitive feelings is associated with higher levels of faith in intuition (Keller & Bless, 2008). Cognitive feelings may be one way in which the products of subconscious processes become registered, articulated, and interpreted (Greifeneder, et al., 2010). As noted in the Introduction, intuition research in management is characterized by a scarcity of empirical work. Our research is one of a small handful of qualitative studies in this area (e.g. Burke & Miller, 1999; Clarke & Mackaness, 2001; Sadler-Smith & Shefy, 2007). It has several implications for intuition theory and hints at several new directions for intuition research as follows: 6 What happens then you intuit? (1) The research sharpens and elaborates the definition of intuition, but in contrast with other recent research it has done so directly from the perspective of data and experience rather than indirectly from theory; (2) Researchers have previously equated intuitions with ‘gut feelings’ (e.g. Burke & Miller, 1999; Hayashi, 2001) however our analysis shows this is insufficient. A more helpful framing is a general category of ‘intuitive affect’ within which bodily feelings (referred to colloquially as ‘gut feel’) and cognitive feelings (referred to colloquially as ‘hunch’ or ‘vibe’) comprise two separate facets of the subjective experience of intuition; (3) We expand Dane and Pratt’s (2007) ‘intuiting’ and ‘intuitive judgment’ distinction into three elements, ‘intuiting’, ‘intuition’, and ‘enacting’ (Figure 2b) which offers a tentative three-phase process model of intuition. As far a future directions go, we need to know more about the detailed nature of intuitive affect over-and-above mere attributions to ‘gut feelings’, and also explore whether different feeling states arise under different conditions, the possibility of individual differences in the subjective experience of intuition, individuals’ sensitivities to their bodily state (Dunn et al., 2010), variability in the modalities in which intuition presents itself (Miller, 1992; Vaughan, 1979), and the neural correlates of the various facets of intuitive affect (Segalowitz, 2007) Conclusion This research has sought to get closer empirically to human resource practitioners’ experiences of intuition by applying a novel qualitative methodology based on denominalization. By interpreting what Human Resources’ practitioners wrote about their experiences of intuition we have proposed a three phase model: intuiting, intuition, and enacting. Through replication and extension with different samples in different contexts, by developing and elaborating the method and generating further research questions and hypotheses this research could have important implications for how we understand the subjective experience of a phenomenon which is seen increasingly as pivotal in Human Resources and organizational practices more generally. References Agor WH (1986) The logic of intuition: How top executives make important decisions. Organizational Dynamics 14(3): 5-18. Akinci C and Sadler-Smith E (2012) Intuition in management research: A historical review. International Journal of Management Reviews 14(1): 104-122. Burke LA and Miller MK (1999) Taking the mystery out of intuitive decision making. Academy of Management Executive 13(4): 91-99. Clarke I and Mackaness W (2001) Management ‘intuition’: an interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies 38(2): 147-172. Clore GL (1992) Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 133-163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. London: Vintage. Dane E and Pratt MG (2007) exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Academy of Management Review 32(1): 33-54. 7 What happens then you intuit? Dane E and Pratt MG (2009) conceptualizing and measuring intuition: A review of recent trends. In Hodgkinson, G. P. and Ford, J. K. (Eds.) International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 24 (pp. 1-40). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Dane E, Rockmann KW and Pratt MG. (2012) When should I trust my gut? Linking domain expertise to intuitive decision-making effectiveness. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 119: 187-194. Diab, D.L., Pui, S-H., Yankelevich, M. and Highhouse, S. (2011) Lay perceptions of selection decision aids in US and non-US samples. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, Vol.19, No.2, pp.209-216. Dunn BD, Galton HC, Morgan R, Evans D, Oliver C, Meyer, M and Dalgleish T (2010) Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science 21(12): 1835-1844. Epstein S, Pacini R, Denes-Raj V and Heier H (1996) Individual differences in intuitiveexperiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71(2): 390-405. Ericsson KA, Prietula MJ and Cokely ET (2007) The making of an expert. Harvard Business Review July-August: 115-121. Forgas JP (1995) Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin 117(1): 39-66 Gioia D, Corley KG and Hamilton AL (2013) Seeking qualitative rigor in inductive research: Notes on the Gioia methodology. Organizational Research Methods 16(1): 15-31. Greifeneder R, Bless H and Pham MT (2010). When do people rely on affective and cognitive feelings in judgment? A review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2): 107-141. Hayashi AM (2001) When to trust your gut. Harvard Business Review 79(2): 59–65. Highhouse, S. (2008) ‘Stubborn reliance on intuition and subjectivity in employee selection’. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1(3): 333-342. Hodgkinson GP and Clarke I (2007) Exploring the cognitive significance of organizational strategizing: A dual-process framework and research agenda. Human Relations 60(1): 243255. Hodgkinson GP, Sadler-Smith E, Burke LA, Claxton G and Sparrow PR (2009) Intuition in organizations: Implications for strategic management. Long Range Planning 42(3): 277-297. Hodgkinson GP, Langan-Fox J and Sadler-Smith E (2008) Intuition: A fundamental bridging construct in the behavioural sciences. British Journal of Psychology 99(1): 1-27. Kahneman D and Klein GA (2009) conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. The American Psychologist 64(6): 515-526. Keller J and Bless H (2008) Predicting future affective states: How ease-of-retrieval and faith in intuition moderate the impact of activated content. European Journal of Social Psychology 38: 1-10. Khatri N and Ng HA (2000) The role of intuition in strategic decision making. Human Relations 53(1): 57-86. Klein GA (1998) Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge: MIT Press. Klein GA (2003) Intuition at work. New York: Currency Doubleday. Klein GA, Calderwood R and Clinton-Cirocco A (1988) Rapid decision making on the fireground. Alexandria: U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioural and Social Sciences. Langley A and Abdallah C (2011) Templates and turns in qualitative studies of strategy and management. Research Methodology in Strategy and Management 6: 201-235. Lieberman MD (2007) Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology 58: 259-289. 8 What happens then you intuit? Lieberman MD, Jarcho JM and Satpute AB (2004) Evidence-based and intuition-based selfknowledge: An fMRI study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87: 421-435. Miles, A., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2014) “With recruitment I always feel I need to listen to my gut”: the role of intuition in employee selection. Personnel Review, 43(4): 606-627. Miller CC and Ireland RD (2005) Intuition in strategic decision making: Friend or foe in the fast-paced 21st century? Academy of Management Executive 19(1): 19-30. Miller AI (1992) Imagery and intuition in creative scientific thinking: Albert Einstein’s invention of the special theory of relativity. In D. B., Wallace, & H. E. Gruber, (Eds.). Creative people at work: Twelve cognitive case studies, pp. 171-187. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Nag R, Corley KG and Gioia DA (2007) The intersection of organizational identity, knowledge, and practice: Attempting strategic change via knowledge grafting. Academy of Management Journal 50(4): 821-847. Panksepp J (2003) At the interface of the affective, behavioural, and cognitive neurosciences: Decoding the emotional feelings of the brain. Brain and Cognition 52(1): 4-14. Payne TE (1997) Describing morphosyntax: A guide for field linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pratt MG, Rockmann KW and Kaufmann J.B (2006) Constructing professional identity: The role of work and identity learning cycles in the customization of identity among medical residents. Academy of Management Journal 49(2): 235-262. Sadler-Smith E and Shefy E (2004) The intuitive executive: Understanding and applying ‘gut feel’ in decision-making. Academy of Management Executive 18(4): 76-91. Sadler-Smith E and Shefy E (2007) Developing intuitive awareness in management education. Academy of Management Learning & Education 6(2): 186-205. Salas E, Rosen MA and DiazGranados D (2010) Expertise-based intuition and decision making in organizations. Journal of Management 36(4): 941-973. Segalowitz SJ (2007) Knowing before we know: Conscious versus preconscious top–down processing and a neuroscience of intuition. Brain and Cognition 65(2): 143-144. Simon HA (1987) Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion. The Academy of Management Executive 1(1): 57-64. Sinclair M and Ashkanasy NM (2005) Intuition myth or a decision-making tool? Management Learning 36(3): 353-370. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E and MacGregor DG (2004) Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis 24(2): 311322. Stanovich KE (2009) What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational thought. New Haven: Yale University Press. Vaughan FE (1979) Awakening Intuition. New York: Doubleday. 9