1 Gentry Hanks SOCY 921 Fall Term Paper December 5, 2011 Affect, Emotion, and Technology I will provide a brief overview and discussion of how emotion and affect are theoretically conceptualized1, defined, and studied in affective computing, information sciences, neurosciences, and sociology (and intersections thereof2) as well as some examples of how this research is being applied. I will address how the discipline of geography has contributed to the study emotion and affect and suggest my future contributions to geography in affective, emotional studies. Lastly, I will discuss how emotion and affect relate to my particular research interests in regard to digital and material hoarding. The topics of affect and emotion have seen a surge, as of late, in many disciplines, particularly science and technology studies. While affect and emotion have long been studied in psychology, cognitive and behavioral sciences, as well as business and economics for the purpose of marketing and advertising, there seems to have been a surge in interest in regard to technology. As new and different types Given the long temporal attention to affect and emotion, I have only chosen a few more recent studies to discuss and will opt to not address the deep pile of work surrounding Freud, Derrida and Foucault on this matter in this paper. 2 Because of the collaborative efforts and overlapping work, it was difficult to arrange some of the studies in neat categories of disciplines, so the arrangement might seem circular at times. 1 2 of technology are generated, there is a push to connect what is perceived as artificial and natural. While some biologists seek to clone ‘natural’ humans, other scientists, in a sense, seek to (re)produce an artificial human through robotics. Of course this becomes all the more complicated when we question, “what kinds of bodies are being imagined, and what limits and possibilities does the robot embody in turn” (Casatneda and Suchman, 2005, p. 2), particularly in regard to humans, machines, and animals3. This has highlighted to a great extent the perceived divide between mind and body and as Elizabeth Wilson (1998) notes, “there is very little written about psychology and in a contemporary scientific context that does not in some way invoke either the computer or the brain” (103). The very approaches taken to bridge the divide between artificial and natural are based on the existence of the separation of the two. Wilson (1998) also points out “emerging from the…field of cognitive science, the computational approach has eschewed the idea of neurological plausibility, aiming instead to produce a theory of cognition that exists independently of biological constraints” (pp. 103-104). Mechanical bodies are worked on as such to reproduce natural human movements, while other studies and projects attempt to capture human cognitive processes and replicate them with technology. Neurosciences and Affective Computing Many avenues of emotional and affectual research lead to an interest in autism. Rosalind Picard (1997) explores and investigates human-computer interactions so that computers might better assist autistic people through affective 3 See Panksepp (2004) for a discussion of animals and emotions. 3 computing (see Picard, 1997 and Lisetti, 1998). Picard (2009) notes the multifariousness of inference, “when people infer an emotion such as ‘exuberance’, they combine multiple channels of information in a complex way: smiles, shrieks, upward bodily bouncing or arm gestures, and perhaps even tears of joy” (p. 3577). Therefore Picard (2009) surmises, “measuring emotion is not as simple as measuring a few bodily parameters” (p. 3577). The messiness of emotion is hard to quantify and “no algorithms yet exist that describe how to precisely combine the many contributing channels into a full space of emotions” (Picard, 2009, p. 3577). This statement presupposes that indeed there exists a whole or complete emotion space and denies that emotion might be wholly fractured and fragmented. Locating affectual and emotive activity in particular parts of the brain is a fastidious concern of neuroscientific research. According to Picard (2009), the autonomic nervous system (ANS) is part of the peripheral nervous system, which is responsible for functions at a pre-cognitive level. Picard (2009) describes the autonomic nervous system as bimodal (see Gunes and Piccardi, 2007), comprised of the sympathetic and parasympathetic portions (Picard, 2009). According to Picard (2009), “the parasympathetic nervous system promotes restoration and conservation of bodily energy, ‘rest and digest’, the sympathetic nervous system stimulates increased metabolic output to deal with external challenges, the socalled, ‘flight or flight’” (p. 3577). This understanding compartmentalizes internal and external processes of affect and emotion as dealt with separately. These understandings are typically arrived at through observing the brain through brain scans and measuring activity levels in different portions of the brain when 4 performing a spontaneous or given task. This becomes problematic when heightened activity comes to stand for “location” of certain activities within the brain (see Cordelia Fine, 2011), which may communicate a lack of activity in other areas of the brain. By geographically locating affect and emotion in the brain, neurologists are able to label emotion and affect as cognitive or precognitive. Picard (1997) discusses the limbic system, which she locates “between the brain stem and the two hemispheres of the cortex” (p. 2) and asserts that it is responsible for “memory, attention, and emotion” (p.2). Neuroscientist Antonio Damasio (1994) describes two categories of locations of emotions—those occurring primarily and those as secondary. For Damasio (1994), primary emotions occur at a pre- or meta-cognitive level and secondary emotions occur at a cognitive level. Primary emotions relate back to the ANS and the limbic system, whereas secondary emotions (and therefore cognitive) are located within prefrontal somatosensory cortices (Damasio, 1994). Once emotion and affect are located and locatable within the brain, many potentialities and projections become possible. The fields of affective computing, machine learning, and robotics have seen a peaked interest in emotion and affect. Creating robots and machines (see Schroeder, 2010) that can harness natural speech patterns, natural body and facial movements have come to be the golden goose of some robotics research. The emulation of humans, yet not recognizable as such, serves as a large focus in technological aspirations (although not all). These projects typically deal with the development of prosthetics for amputees or to aid children and adults with autism (see Lacava et al., 5 2007) or sensory perception disorders. For example, capturing emotion in the human voice for natural speech patterns and storytelling was attempted through machine learning to help children with autism experience and recognize emotion. Machine learning has the potential to be applied in a variety of fields and types of research. According to Ian Witten and Eibe Frank (2005), “machine learning…is used to extract information from the raw data in databases” (p.xxiii) and the extraction process “is one of abstraction: taking the data, warts and all, and inferring whatever structure underlies it” (p. xxiii). For the purposes of quantifying emotion and affect, machine learning is used to extract raw data (collected using any number of quantitative methods) from facial expression recognition, neural activity, prosody and pattern of voice and speech, body movements4, and eye movements. Cecilia Alm, Don Roth, and Richard Sproat (2005), combining forces from linguistics, computer science and electrical engineering, empirically explored “textbased emotion prediction” (p.579) through machine learning5. Their approach involved classifying emotional contents of narrative in children’s fairytales using a tripartite model that considered emotional valence, prosody, and natural language processing (Alm, Roth, Sproat, 2005). They found that “despite the schematic narrative plots of children’s stories, tales still differ in their overall affective orientation, which increases the data complexity” (p. 583) and that “emotions are not discrete objects; rather they have transitional nature, and blend and overlap See Camurri et al (2000) and Zimmerman (2003) for a discussion on affect, emotion, dance and music. 5 They used SNoW (Sparse network of Winnows) learning architecture, which is a framework of linear functions commonly used in large-scale tasks in bioinformatics, visual processing, and natural language processing (Alm, Roth, and Sproat, 2005). 4 6 along the temporal dimension” (p.585). This transitional and overlapping nature make it all the more difficult to assess and estimate emotions (see Matsumoto et al., 2007 and Kay and Loverock, 2008). Neural Computational researchers Marian Bartlett et al. (2005) used machine learning to recognize “spontaneous [facial] expressions in an interview setting” (p.568) extracted from video. They relied on “objective coding standards” (p. 570) known as FACS (Facial Action Coding System) to “objectively capture the richness and complexity of facial expressions” (p.568). A limitation to the study using FACS arises when there are head movements (ranging beyond the plane) and movements of the mouth during speech (Bartlett et al., 2005). This is another example of the dynamic and rich processes at work in emotion and affect that complicate attempts to quantify and “objectively” understand emotion. Within information studies, affect is defined by Ping Zhang and Na Li (2005) as “a term that encompasses mood, emotions, and feelings” (p. 105) and is “a fundamental aspect of human beings, one that influences reflex, perception, cognition, and behavior (p. 105). Zhang and Li distinguish between affect and affective quality, defining the latter as “the ability of an object or stimulus to cause changes in one’s affect” (p. 105). Zhang and Li’s (2005) article “The Importance of Affective Quality” opens with “Users aren’t always rational logical beings—emotion plays an often overlooked role in user acceptance of technology,” which again places rationality and logic as outside of emotion. Additionally, they define it as a fundamental aspect of human beings. This definition marginalizes humans without 7 abilities to recognize, process or express emotion as well as conveying that emotion is perhaps a uniquely human phenomenon. Information studies researchers Ping Zhang and Na Li (2005) position their empirical investigation of affective reaction to technology use within a sea of studies that they claim “are based on the assumption that human beings are rational and behave based on logical information-based thinking” (pp. 106-107). Zhang and Li center their research on situating emotion, affect and mood as separate from logic and reason. This approach allows them to assert that users of technology base their perception and potential adoption of technology on perceived affective qualities and behavioral intention in regard to a positive valence of perceived ease of use and usefulness (2005). Again, their research relies on the notion that human (pre)cognition compartmentalizes emotion and affect in geographically or spatially different parts of the brain than logic and reason as well as viewing them as mutually exclusive processes of (pre)cognition. Social Sciences and Humanities: Geography, Affect and Emotion Within the humanities and social sciences6, there has been what Patricia Clough termed an “Affective Turn.” According to Clough (2007) there are “two primary precursors to the affective turn…the focus on the body, which has been most extensively advanced in feminist theory and the explorations of emotions, conducted predominantly in queer theory” (p. x). The parameters of this turn are potentially more limiting than liberating, because as sociologist Myra Hird (2003) suggests, “when feminists study materiality, it tends to be in terms of how humans 6 See Beder (2005) for work done in social work regarding technology and emotion. 8 (such as scientists) interact with materiality, as though there is no outside of, or beyond, the cultural context” (p. 448). Clough (2007) poses, as many scholars dealing with affect do, that affective labor “engages at once with rational intelligence and with the passions or feeling” (p. xi). This presupposes that emotion and affect are dichotomous. Elizabeth Wilson puts forth that this is a false dichotomy and Picard (2009) points out that “people are quick to polarize thoughts and feelings as if they were opposites (p.2). Davidson and Milligan review the literature dealing with emotion, organizing and contextualizing it according to scale and sphere (e.g. domestic, public) within geography and acknowledge parallel emotional, affectual research in sociology (see Bendelow and Williams 1998). Davidson and Milligan’s (2004) approach to emotion requires a reexamination of emotional embodiment and embodiment of emotion as they suggest, “recognition of the inherently emotional nature of embodiment has, thus, led many to the conclusion that we need to explore how we feel—as well as think—through ‘the body’” (p. 523). I hope to explore thinking through the body in a combination of bio/neurological ways as well as socio-cultural. Davidson and Milligan’s (2004) suggestion that “emotions, then, might be seen as a form of connective tissue that links experiential geographies of the human psyche and physique with(in) broader social geographies of place” (p. 524) might be helpful in addressing mind/body dualities and dichotomies. Geographer Ben Anderson (2007) explores hope and anticipatory knowledge in relation to affect, in particular “how the disclosure of the nanoscale as a place subject to intervention, the act that is taken to define nanotechnology, can be 9 understood in the context of anticipatory knowledge practices that create futures” (p 156). Anderson (2007) argues that “affect is transversal to both nanotechnoscience and anticipatory governance… that the ground that enables hope to be placed in nanotechnology is the event that defines nano – to simultaneously reduce ‘life’ to matter and to multiply ‘life’ into a limitless set of materialities.” (p. 156). I hope to expand these understandings to include unbounded bodies in cyberspace and the reduction of hoarding (a matter of scale), yet further grounding my work in materiality of bodies concurrently. I will build upon geographical understanding of identity in regard to extensions of the self, using geographer Paul Adams’ (2005) boundless self and other work in geography, such as Davidson’s research in the Spatiality of Identity (2003) by drawing from science and technology studies, particularly neuro and cognitive sciences. Feminist geographies have renewed interests in bodies and embodiment, yet still often neglect biological bodies, mainly focusing on social/cultural experiences of embodiment. Feminist geographies, like other feminist research is often centered around and carried out through critical cultural discourse which, according to Wilson (1998) “misrepresents the complex relation between neurology and its outside, but also, by locating malleability, politics, and difference only in the domain of culture and environment, it abandons neurology to the very biologism it claims to be contesting” (p.16). I aspire to in a way traverse “feminism’s own naturalized antiessentialism” (Wilson, 1998, p. 15) in my work because as Wilson astutely questions, “how can a critical habit nurtured on an antibiologism produce anything but the most cursory and negating critique of 10 biology” (p. 16). Therefore, I will attempt to engage biological bodies in my future work in addition to socio-cultural, discursive bodies7. I aim to do a comparative study of spatial and emotional hoarding behaviors in animals and humans and discuss how we, through neuroscience and technology have come to understand the difference between hoarding (see Pertusa et al., 2008) and collecting, particularly as is pertains to public (archives) and domestic spheres. I want to delimit8 hoarding as a purely material physical amassing and extend it to the virtual and digital realms of things. Additionally, I will acknowledge and discuss the controversy of categorizing hoarding as a syndrome in and of itself as opposed to its categorization as a symptom of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). There are implications of how hoarding is categorized through a social process within cognitive and behavioral sciences for future clinical and neurological research. Geography is situated in both physical and social sciences, which provides an opportunity to study this cognitive and behavioral phenomenon within a social context as well as social and behavioral within a scientific context. I understand that this dichotomy is largely linguistic and not necessarily ontological. 8 The term deconstruct is purposefully not used here because as Wilson (1998) suggests, “deconstruction never offers the possibility of a move beyond or outside of what it interrogates” (p.164). I want to expand and move beyond contemporary understandings of psycho and cognitive theories surrounding hoarding, not merely deconstruct them. 7 11 Adams, P. C. (2005). The Boundless Self: Communication In Physical And Virtual Spaces. Syracuse Univ Pr (Sd). Alaimo, S. (2008). Material feminisms. Indiana University Press. Alm, C. O., Roth, D., & Sproat, R. (2005). Emotions from text: machine learning for text-based emotion prediction. Proceedings of the conference on Human Language Technology and Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, HLT ’05 (pp. 579–586). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics. doi:10.3115/1220575.1220648 Anderson, B. (2007). Hope for nanotechnology: anticipatory knowledge and the governance of affect. Area, 39(2), 156-165. doi:10.1111/j.1475-4762.2007.00743.x Bartlett, M. S., Littlewort, G., Frank, M., Lainscsek, C., Fasel, I., & Movellan, J. (2005). Recognizing Facial Expression: Machine Learning and Application to Spontaneous Behavior. Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, IEEE Computer Society Conference on (Vol. 2, pp. 568-573). Los Alamitos, CA, USA: IEEE Computer Society. doi:http://doi.ieeecomputersociety.org/10.1109/CVPR.2005.297 Beder, J. (2005). Cybersolace: Technology Built on Emotion. Social Work, 50(4), 355-358. Bendelow, G., & Williams, S. J. (1998). Emotions in social life: critical themes and contemporary issues. Routledge. Camurri, Antonio, Hashimoto, S., Ricchetti, M., Ricci, A., Suzuki, K., Trocca, R., & Volpe, G. (2000). EyesWeb: Toward Gesture and Affect Recognition in Interactive Dance and Music Systems. Computer Music Journal, 24(1), 57-69. Cowie, R., Douglas-Cowie, E., Tsapatsoulis, N., Votsis, G., Kollias, S., Fellenz, W., & Taylor, J. G. (2001). Emotion recognition in human-computer interaction. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine, 18(1), 32-80. doi:10.1109/79.911197 Damasio, A. R. (2000). The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Davidson, J. (2003). Phobic geographies: the phenomenology and spatiality of identity. Ashgate Publishing, Ltd. Davidson, J., & Milligan, C. (2004). Embodying emotion sensing space: introducing emotional geographies. Social & Cultural Geography, 5, 523-532. doi:10.1080/1464936042000317677 DeKoven, M. (2001). Feminist locations: global and local, theory and practice. Rutgers University Press. Dennis, C., Newman, A., Michon, R., Josko Brakus, J., & Tiu Wright, L. (2010). The mediating effects of perception and emotion: Digital signage in mall atmospherics. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 17(3), 205-215. doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2010.03.009 Devillers, L., Vidrascu, L., & Lamel, L. (2005). Challenges in real-life emotion annotation and machine learning based detection. Neural Networks, 18(4), 407-422. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2005.03.007 Fine, C. (2011). Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference (Reprint.). W. W. Norton & Company. Gunes, H., & Piccardi, M. (2007). Bi-modal emotion recognition from expressive face and body gestures. Journal of Network and Computer Applications, 30(4), 1334-1345. doi:10.1016/j.jnca.2006.09.007 Hatano, G., Okada, N., & Tanabe, H. (2000). Affective minds. Elsevier Health Sciences. Hird, M. J. (2003). New Feminist Sociological Directions. The Canadian Journal of Sociology / Cahiers canadiens de sociologie, 28(4), 447-462. doi:10.2307/3341837 Kay, R. H., & Loverock, S. (2008). Assessing emotions related to learning new software: The computer emotion scale. Comput. Hum. Behav., 24(4), 1605–1623. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2007.06.002 Lacava, P. G., Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Smith Myles, B. (2007). Using Assistive Technology to Teach Emotion Recognition to Students With Asperger Syndrome. Remedial and Special Education, 28(3), 174 181. doi:10.1177/07419325070280030601 Lisetti, C. L. (1998). Affective computing. Pattern Analysis and Applications, 1, 71-73. doi:10.1007/BF01238028 Marcus, A. (2003). The emotion commotion. interactions, 10(6), 28–34. doi:10.1145/947226.947239 Matsumoto, K., Ren, F., Kuroiwa, S., & Tsuchiya, S. (2007). Emotion estimation algorithm based on interpersonal emotion included in emotional dialogue sentences. Proceedings of the artificial intelligence 6th Mexican international conference on Advances in artificial intelligence, MICAI’07 (pp. 1035–1045). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. Retrieved from http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1775967.1776074 12 Nasoz, F., Alvarez, K., Lisetti, C. L., & Finkelstein, N. (2004). Emotion recognition from physiological signals using wireless sensors for presence technologies. Cognition, Technology & Work, 6, 4-14. doi:10.1007/s10111-003-0143-x Panksepp, J. (2004). Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press. Pertusa, A., Fullana, M. A., Singh, S., Alonso, P., Menchon, J. M., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2008). Compulsive Hoarding: OCD Symptom, Distinct Clinical Syndrome, or Both? American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1289-1298. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111730 Peter, C., & Herbon, A. (2006). Emotion representation and physiology assignments in digital systems. Interacting with Computers, 18(2), 139-170. doi:10.1016/j.intcom.2005.10.006 Picard, R. W. (2000). Affective Computing. MIT Press. Picard, R. W. (2009). Future affective technology for autism and emotion communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1535), 3575 -3584. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0143 Rachman, S., Elliott, C. M., Shafran, R., & Radomsky, A. S. (2009). Separating hoarding from OCD. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(6), 520-522. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.014 Ramsey, W. (2011). Eliminative Materialism. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (2011st ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2011/entries/materialism-eliminative/ Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145-172. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145 Schröder, M. (2010). The SEMAINE API: towards a standards-based framework for building emotion-oriented systems. Adv. in Hum.-Comp. Int., 2010. doi:10.1155/2010/319406 Suzuki, K., Camurri, A., Ferrentino, P., & Hashimoto, S. (n.d.). Intelligent agent system for human-robot interaction through artificial emotion (Vol. 2, pp. 1055-1060). IEEE. doi:10.1109/ICSMC.1998.727828 Sykes, J., & Brown, S. (2003). Affective gaming: measuring emotion through the gamepad. CHI ’03 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems, CHI EA ’03 (pp. 732–733). New York, NY, USA: ACM. doi:10.1145/765891.765957 Wajcman, J. (2010). Feminist theories of technology. Cambridge Journal of Economics, 34(1), 143 -152. doi:10.1093/cje/ben057 Witten, I. H., & Frank, E. (2005). Data mining: practical machine learning tools and techniques. Morgan Kaufmann. Zhang, P., & Li, N. (2005). The importance of affective quality. Commun. ACM, 48(9), 105–108. doi:10.1145/1081992.1081997 Zimmerman, J. (2003). Exploring the role of emotion in the interaction design of digital music players. Proceedings of the 2003 international conference on Designing pleasurable products and interfaces, DPPI ’03 (pp. 152–153). New York, NY, USA: ACM. doi:10.1145/782896.782939