Kristen Jones Faye Huie Project Random Coefficient Modeling You hypothesize that Mood and Emotion Regulation are positively related in children. That is, when a child is in a good mood, s/he engages in more emotion regulation than when in a bad mood. You hypothesize further that this is more true for some children than for others. Specifically, whereas mood and regulation are strongly positively related in children with calm temperaments (i.e., kids who score high on temperament), mood and regulation are only weakly related in children who are total spazzes (technical term). Mood and regulation, therefore, vary within child, while temperament is a stable individual difference variable. The rcmproj file has data on these variables for each of 40 children. Mood and Regulation data were collected at each of five time points for each child. Temperament was measured at the outset of the study. Your task is to use rcm to test your hypotheses. 1. What does one of the Level 1 equations look like in its general form? yij = B1jxij + B0j + rij where j symbolizes any single group (i.e., child) and i represents any single occasion within child j We can assume that each of the 40 children have their own separate regression equation. Since the 40 children are randomly sampled from the population, this means our slope and intercept values for each child’s regression equation are now also random effects. The above equation represents the regression of emotional regulation onto mood for child j, case i. 2. What do the two Level 2 equations look like, again in general form? Boj = ϒ00 + u0j The above equation is a representation of a particular intercept value of any given child’s individual regression equation (generated from Level 1) and how it deviates from the population regression intercept. This equation shows how the estimation of any child’s given intercept value is function of the overall population intercept value (fixed effect) and the random deviation of that particular child’s intercept value from the population intercept value. Insofar as clustering influences emotional regulation (i.e., to the degree that group membership explains variance in emotional regulation), this random deviation between a child’s intercept and the population intercept will be greater. B1j = ϒ10 + u1j The above equation is a representation of a particular slope value of any given child’s individual regression equation (generated from Level 1) and how it deviates from the population regression slope. This equation shows how the estimation of any child’s given slope value is function of the overall population slope value (fixed effect) and the random deviation of that particular child’s slope value from the population slope value. Insofar as clustering influences emotional regulation (i.e., to the degree that group membership explains variance in emotional regulation), this random deviation between a child’s slope and the population slope will be greater. 3. Do a simple Variance Components analysis on centered variables to answer the question, What proportion of the variance in regulation can be attributed to between-child differences? What proportion is within-child? Show your work. ICC = (.5812)/(.5812 + 3.0175) = .16 Thus, 16% of the total variance in emotion regulation is attributable to betweenchild differences. (3.0175)/(.5812 + 3.0175) = .84 The remaining percent of the variance (84%) can then be attributable to withinchild differences. 4. Now do another analysis with centered mood as a covariate. What proportion of between subject variance is explained by differences in average mood? What proportion of within subject variance is explained by within person variability in mood? Do significant amounts of each remain? Show your work. (.5812 -.2967)/.5812 = .49 49% of the between-child variance is explained by differences in average mood which leaves an nonsignificant amount of variance in emotion regulation between children left to be explained. (3.0175-2.652)/3.0175 = 12% of the within-child variance in emotion regulation is explained by within person variability in mood leaving a significant amount of within-child variance in emotion regulation left unexplained. 5. Now do a final analysis that also includes Temperament and the Mood* Temperament interaction. What proportion of between subject variance is explained by differences in temperament? What proportion of within subject variance is explained by within person variability in temperament? Do significant amounts of each remain? Show your work. Estimates of Fixed Effectsa 95% Confidence Interval Parameter Estimate Std. Error df t Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound Intercept -.327691 .406888 31.176 -.805 .427 -1.157354 .501972 Cenmood -.466966 .216433 41.453 -2.158 .037 -.903916 -.030016 Temperament .049706 .092522 32.479 .537 .595 -.138647 .238059 Cenmood * Temperament .101174 .046032 34.525 2.198 .035 .007678 .194670 a. Dependent Variable: CenReg. Estimates of Covariance Parametersa 95% Confidence Interval Parameter Estimate Residual Wald Z Sig. Lower Bound Upper Bound 2.647070 .319384 8.288 .000 2.089600 3.353265 .335828 .254901 1.317 .188 .075864 1.486611 UN (2,1) -.083624 .073742 -1.134 .257 -.228156 .060907 UN (2,2) .103287 .057407 1.799 .072 .034749 .307006 Intercept + Cenmood [subject UN (1,1) = Subjnum] Std. Error a. Dependent Variable: CenReg. (.2967 - .3358)/.2967 = -.1318 After the inclusion of temperament, our unexplained between-child variance increases by 13%. We can interpret this as ZERO. Thus, none of the betweenchild variance in emotional regulation can be explained by the addition of temperament. If we look back at the unexplained variance BEFORE the inclusion of temperament, we can see that we no longer had a significant amount of variance in between-child differences in emotion regulation left to be explained. Thus, after the addition of temperament, there is not a significant (p = .24) amount of variance in between-child emotion regulation to explain. (2.652 - 2.647)/2.652 = .0019 After the inclusion of temperament, we not surprisingly don’t explain additional within-child variance in emotion regulation. This is because each child has the same constant temperament value; thus, temperament does not vary across observations within a child. Therefore, a group level predictor will never be able to account for within-group variance, which is why we observe no change in unexplained within-child variance after the inclusion of temperament. 6. Now explain which of your hypotheses were supported and which weren’t. H1: You hypothesize that Mood and Emotion Regulation are positively related in children. That is, when a child is in a good mood, s/he engages in more emotion regulation than when in a bad mood. When we look at the fixed effects after the inclusion of mood, we can observe the weight for mood (-.028) is not statistically significant (p = .73). This tells us that the addition of mood to the equation did not explain a significant amount of variance in emotion regulation. Furthermore, for every single point increase in mood, a slight (albeit) insignificant decrease in emotion regulation was predicted. Therefore, we cannot reject the null hypothesis since our results failed to provide support for a positive relationship between mood and emotion regulation. H2: You hypothesize further that this is more true for some children than for others. Specifically, whereas mood and regulation are strongly positively related in children with calm temperaments (i.e., kids who score high on temperament), mood and regulation are only weakly related in children who are total spazzes (technical term). When we look at the second output (after the inclusion of mood) we get a variance component (tau value) for the variance in slopes across the 40 children (.134) that is significant (p = .038). This tells us that there is a significant amount of variance in the relationship between mood and emotion regulation across children. This indicates that there is some group level (or in this case, child-level) variable accounting for this between-child variance in the mood-emotion regulation slope. In the third set of output (after the inclusion of temperament and the interaction between temperament and mood), we see that our weight for the interaction term is significant (p=.035), meaning the inclusion of the interaction term explains significant variance in emotion regulation. Thus, the weight for the product term (.1012) represents the rate of change in the mood-emotion regulation slope per single point increase in temperament. That is, as children increase in temperament, the relationship between mood and emotion regulation strengthens. This is in line with H2; therefore, we can reject the null hypothesis. 7. Now do an ordinary moderated regression analysis (i.e., a disaggregated analysis ignoring group membership) and explain how your results differ from the RCM analysis. After running this example as an ordinary moderated regression, we can see that our standard errors for the regression coefficients decrease when we ignore group membership. For the RCM analysis, the SE of the intercept = .16, SE of cenmood = .078, SE of centemp = .092, and SE of the interaction = .046 whereas in the disaggregated analysis, the SE of the intercept = .134, SE of cenmood = .06, SE of centemp = .077, and SE of the interaction = .036. This is because in the disaggregated approach, the only error that is accounted for is random variability in Y. Thus, random variability in intercepts and random variability in slopes are ignored, resulting in smaller but less accurate SEs. This means that in OLS, to the degree that clustering matters, we higher alpha inflation, a higher Type I error rate, and an easier time achieving statistical significance. The error term in RCM accounts for random variability in Y, random variability in intercepts, and random variability in slopes which SHOULD be accounted for resulting in larger (but more accurate) SEs of the weights.