Autoregressive (simplex) models Autoregressive models estimate the stability of a construct over time by regressing later measures of a construct onto earlier measures of the same construct. These constructs or variables can be observed, latent, and even cross-lagged with other variables. In the simple observed example, a variable is an additive function of its immediate predecessor and a random disturbance. Without too much difficulty latent variables can be used to remove measurement error, this is known as a quasi simplex model. These across-time (within construct) predictions are referred to as stability coefficients. The stability coefficients indicate the average standing relative to the group mean from one time to another. So that a large positive stability coefficient would indicate that people reporting above the mean at time 1 tended to report above the mean in time 2. In other papers the coefficients discussed as correlations, which works well since they can range from -1 to 1. The single stability parameter characterizes the stability of each construct over two time points for all subjects in the sample. This is an important distinction from the latent growth curve models which estimate characteristics individual change. So, in the autoregressive model, if the group mean increased 2 units from time 1 to time 2, but an individual reported a the same score for each time period this individual would be making a negative contribution to the average covariance between the two time points. In the simplex models, all time periods are not continuous, but are t-1 discrete time relationships, where t is the number of time points. This allows for different models to act for every time period. So certain factors may influence the first wave, and, depending on the stability coefficients, may affect later scores through the first score. However, we can also examine how different variables can change in their effects across time. So, for example, minority status may contribute to a high score in time one, and therefore may keep scores higher than average due to the high initial score. However, we could also let minority status affect later scores, thus finding that minority status may affect scores at time 2 and 3, but not in time 4. Note these at not time-varying covariates, but rather time invariant variables that effects change over time. We can also add timevarying covariates. Even if set up like the standard simplex model, the quasi simplex model is not identified, and more specifically, the parameter between the first two and last two time periods cannot be identified. To overcome this problem, typically, researchers make two restrictions: first make the first and second errors (the variances) equal and second, make the last and second to last errors equal. Of course, in the minimal three-period model, all the errors would be set to be equal. For a one indicator quasi simplex model there is an additional assumption that is not as easily handled and/or justified. That is, there is an assumption that the errors of the indicators are not correlated over time. This, of course is problematic, since we might expect errors of the same measures to be correlated over time. The only way to adjust for this is to have multiple indicators for the latent construct. Our Current Analysis As is evident from the figure below, our current analysis is a quasi-simplex model. We make all the necessary assumptions about errors to make the model identified. Interestingly our stability coefficients (from time 1 to time 3) for the attendance (.72) religious importance (.82) and the different values (.51 to .81, mean .7) are actually impressively high, especially when it is really a four year time lag. For example, achohol use for kids is around .53-.59 for one year. The highest scores are for intelligence, which are around .87-.9 for one year. The good thing about this is that by knowing the general trend (which I don’t remember discussing in either of our papers) we typically know the trend of all the individuals. As for the controls and college major at time 1 two things are being estimated simultaneously. One is the stability of the construct from one time to another, and the other is the effect of the variable. So, by illustration, after removing the stability from a score, a control with a positive coefficient increases the relative (to the mean) score at that time period. Here we take advantage of the quasi simplex model by allowing the controls and college major to affect multiple time periods. This suggests that we want to allow for different models of change at different times. In the paper we talk about the effects being interpreted as maintenance of attitudes. This may or may not be true since we do not know what the actual score is. In fact, a positive score could be evidence of maintenance, or change because then we are always talking about it in relation to the group average or the average group change. ε0 ε1 ε3 A0 A1 A3 λ0 ζ1 λ1 η0 τ1 τ2 η1 ζ3 λ3 η3 β1 College Major at Time 1 Controls β2 Var ε0 = Var ε1 = Var ε3 λ 0 = λ1 = λ 3 = 1 Latent growth curve models Latent growth curve models allow separate trajectories over time using repeated measures. It requires 3 time periods of measures for a linear trajectory, and more for nonlinear trajectories. It models change for each individual over the time period covered. So instead of comparing time adjacent relations of variables, it uses the repeated measures to estimate an underlying or latent trajectory for each person. Two latent factors, the intercept and the slope, represent the growth over time. Thus it estimates the group level (fixed) means and the random (individual) effects as well. For more details see pages 341-343 of Bollen and Curran 2004, or Curran 2000 (pp. 14-23). As with the simplex models the addition of latent constructs is quite straightforward and commonly done (in psychology). The assumption of continuous change is important as we assume a certain functional relationship over time. We also assume that time-invariant variables can only affect the change once across time. So, if we thought minority status had little effect early, but a major affect later, the latent growth model would not be able to test, or control for this situation. However, we can easily include time-varying covariates. The interpretation would be something along the lines of: after removing the underlying trajectory, the independent variable affected the dependent variable by a _____ units. Clearly an important issue with these models is missing data. As well, people are assumed to be interviewed at roughly equal intervals. However, imputation using available data to complete the trajectories is commonly used. Our Possible Analysis Below is a very rough, but accurate, illustration of what our analysis would probably look like if done using latent growth techniques. Although I added time, two these would not necessarily be requisite (although I think we should). Notice all of the time periods contribute (with regression set to 1) the intercept, and the last three periods contribute to the slope (S1, S2, S3). It is by manipulating S1, S2, and S3 that we would test for nonlinear relationships, as well as specifying time intervals. It is here where we would run into a major decision. Because some students were interviewed on a 0, 1, 3, 5 and others a 0, 2, 4, 6 schedule, we would either need to run two separate models for the two schedules, or we would need to impute the missing years from the observed trajectories, which would put us into having a model with 7 time periods. Although the model would run with the data set like it is now (wave 0, 1 and 3) it really violates the continuous change assumption of the model. I am not sure where my preference lies, but I would try running two separate groups before imputing no matter what the final decision is. No matter our choice we would be estimating trajectories for each individual over the time period. This would lead to some very nice results which would let us talk about change over a very important time. Many studies have used just the unconditional model to talk about different trajectories (increasing, staying the same, decreasing, nonlinear). There is already some work using growth models for religiosity, although ours has different measures, a longer time period, and more controls. However, there is nothing I am aware of like this for the attitudes we talk about in the other paper. In fact these may be very nice corollaries to the current papers. Turing to the different effects let me again remind you that our controls (currently set to be time invariant) would only operate once on the trajectories (Ci, Cs). Since most of our controls (with the exception of the R’s political views) are temporally prior to the respondent’s attitude, we shouldn’t be in too much trouble there. A nice feature of these models is that the controls can not only predict starting levels, but also how the change will occur. So while minority status may affect the intercept, parent SES may affect both the intercept and the slope. I find this particularly interesting, and in some ways I think this is what we are seeing with the current setup of allowing the controls to affect both time 1 and time 3 in autoregressive model. However, again this is outside of the scope of this paper. The inclusion of Major would be a time-varying covariate. We would not need to have multiple measures of major, but as illustrated below we could have it affect both time 2 and time 3 (M2, M3). A positive coefficient for M3, for example, would suggest that having a certain major raises an individual’s religiosity, even after taking out the conditional trajectory of that individual. Interestingly, by doing trajectory groups (i.e. increasing, staying the same, etc) analyses we could essentially do interactions of the major with the trajectory type (assuming we have enough data). ε0 ε1 ε2 ε3 A0 A1 A2 A3 λ0 ζ1 λ1 η0 η1 1 η2 S1 ζ3 λ3 η3 1 M2 College Major at Time 1 M3 1 1 intercept Ci Controls ζ2 λ2 S2 S3 slope Cs Other possible models One model that is relatively new is addressed in Bollen and Curran’s recent book on Latent Growth curve models which uses a method effect for multiple indicators. For both of the quasi simplex and latent growth curve models we have to assume the errors are uncorrelated over time, an assumption that is almost certainly incorrect. However, when there are multiple indicators for the latent constructs a method effect construct can be added for each of the indicators over time. This, in reality, can be done for the autoregressive models as well. Certainly for the family, career and society paper we might want to look at this. There have been some attempts to combine the autoregressive and growth curve models. The most recent, and apparently most successful, attempt is the Autoregressive latent trajectory model (starting on page 345 of Bollen and Curran 2004, see attached). It may be best summarized by looking at one of their figures: As you can see it tries to combine the elements of both, and it seems to do a very good job. However, at this point they have not tried to include time varying covariates. In their recent book, these authors argue that this model acts much like the method effect model, where the stability coefficients are really just getting ride of correlated measurement over time. As much as I like this idea, I have to be honest that I am not completely confident of what each regression or stability coefficient really means. Nevertheless, it is cutting edge, and may be something we want to look into further. By the way, in the Bollen and Curran 2004 article attached they also do a nice job of writing a set of equations that can be used for both autoregressive and latent curve models, thus showing many of the similarities. Conclusion As of right now I think our paper attempts to answer this question (substitute values for religiosity for the other paper): Does being involved in a specific major have an effect on religiosity during young adulthood? To determine what model we need to use I think we need to answer to important questions. 1) Do we want to examine change relative to the group or change relative to the individual? 2a) Do we think the change is continuous or discrete? 2b)Are there different models for different time points? There are good things about each model, but in the end I think it comes down to these two major issues.