Peripheral nerve injury leads to working memory deficits and dysfunction of the hippocampus by up-regulation of TNF-α in rodents Wen-Jie Rena§, Yong Liua§, Li-Jun Zhoua, Wei Lib, Yi Zhonga, Rui-Ping Panga, Wen-Jun Xina, Xu-Hong Weia, Jun Wanga, He-Quan Zhua, Chang-You Wuc, Zhi-Hai Qind Guo-Song Liub* and Xian-Guo Liua* SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURES (1-4) SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE LEGENDS Supplementary Figure 1. No significant difference in locomotor activity was detected in the animals with SNI, intracerebroventricular injection of drugs and deletion of TNFR1 (a-c): There was no significant difference in locomotor activity between SNI and sham groups, used in eight-arm radial maze test (shown in Fig. 1a-i). (d-g): The locomotor activities in the rats with i.c.v. injection of rrTNF and of anti-TNF, intra-hippocampal injection of rrTNF or intraperitoneal injection of thalidomide was not different from vehicle injection groups. (h-i): SNI did not affect the locomotor activity of WT mice and KO mice. (j-o): There was no significant difference in the total number of touching objects in NORT tests among groups. n = 5-20 in each group, Data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Supplementary Figure 2. SNI has no acute effect on synaptic transmission and plasticity in contralateral hippocampus, but produces a delayed LTP inhibition (a): The recordings were made in contralateral side to SNI and sham operation. SNI did not affect the baseline of fEPSPs and LTP induction by HFS (100 Hz, 50 pulses, 4 trains with 15s intervals), which was delivered 1h after nerve injury (n = 5 in each group). (b): Similar with ipsilateral hippocampus, the LTP induction in contralateral hippocampus was also inhibited 23d after SNI (n = 5 in each group). Data are presented as means ± s.e.m. (c-d): The position of recording electrode in the CA1 region and stimulation electrode in the CA3 region are shown. Data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Supplementary Figure 3. TNF-α in plasma increases following SNI The concentrations of TNF-α in plasma 3d and 7d after SNI were significant higher, compared with control (0h) (n = 5 at each time point). ** P<0.01 versus control (0h). Data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Supplementary Figure 4. Intracerebroventricular injection of rrTNF inhibits LTP in CA3-CA1 synapses. (a): HFS was able to induce LTP at CA3-CA1 synapses in aCSF group but not in rrTNF group. The data were recorded in the rats that had been used for eight-arm radial maze test following i.c.v. injection rrTNF (shown in Fig. 5 a and b, n = 8 in each group). (b): Injection of rrTNF (i.c.v., 1 μg/ml, 5 µl) in intact rats did not affect the baseline of fEPSPs but blocked LTP by HFS, which was delivered 1h after rrTNF injection (n = 5 in each group). Data are presented as means ± s.e.m. Supplementary Figure 5. Intracerebroventricular injection but not intra-hippocampal injection of rrTNF induces mechanical allodynia. (a): Paw withdrawal thresholds decreased significantly in the rats with intracerebroventricular injection of rrTNF, compared with those before injection in the same group of rats and with those in aCSF treated group (n=8, P < 0.01, Mann-Whitney U test). (b): Paw withdrawal thresholds were not affected by intra-hippocampal injection of rrTNF or aCSF. The tests were performed on 1d before injection and 3d after last injection. Peripheral nerve injury leads to working memory deficits and dysfunction of the hippocampus by up-regulation of TNF-α in rodents Wen-Jie Rena§, Yong Liua§, Li-Jun Zhoua, Wei Lib, Yi Zhonga, Rui-Ping Panga, Wen-Jun Xina, Xu-Hong Weia, Jun Wanga, He-Quan Zhua, Chang-You Wuc, Zhi-Hai Qin Guo-Song Liub* and Xian-Guo Liua* SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS AND METHODS Immunohistochemistry Animals were terminally anesthetized with urethane and perfused through the ascending aorta with PBS followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer. After perfusion, brains were dissected from the skulls and post-fixed in the same fixative for 24 h and then replaced with 30% sucrose overnight. Frozen brains were cut in 10 μm for synaptophysin staining and 18 μm for TNF-α staining on cryostat (LEICA CM3050 S) and sections were immediately mounted on superfrost slides and left to dry overnight and were blocked with 3% rabbit serum in 0.2% Triton X-100 in PBS for 2 h at 4°C. For synaptophysin staining the sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with mouse anti-synaptophysin (Chemicon, for rat) or goat anti-synaptophysin (Chemicon, for mouse) in blocking solution. On the second day, the sections were rinsed with PBS and incubated with Alexa 488-coupled rabbit anti-mouse or anti-goat IgG in PBS (Invitrogen) for 2 h at room temperature. For TNF-α staining the sections were incubated with goat anti-TNF-α (cell signaling) and treated with Cy3-conjugated secondary antibody. The slices were then coverslipped with fluorescent mounting medium (Vector Laboratories) and left for 48 h at 4°C. To test the specificity of anti-TNF-α used in the present study, a preincubation method was used. In the experiment the anti-TNF-α was preincubated with TNF-α (R&D Systems, Inc) at a concentration of 10 µg/ml, which is 5-fold higher than that of the anti-TNF-α, and then the preincubated antibody was used to detect the immunoreactivity (IR) of TNF-α in brain sections. The results showed that the TNF-α-IR detected by preincubated antibody (Fig. 4f) was significantly lower than that detected by non-preincubated antibody (Fig. 4d). Novel object recognition test (NORT) The test was carried out to access the short-term memory and long-term memory of animals with SNI and sham-operation. The apparatus consisted of a round arena (diameter: 50cm for mouse and 80cm for rat) with black (for rat) or white (for mouse) walls and floor. The box and objects were cleaned between trials to stop the build-up of olfactory cues. Animals received two sessions of 10 minutes each in the empty box to habituate them to the apparatus and test room. Twenty-four hours later, each rat was first placed in the box and exposed to two identical objects for 10 min (sample phase). And then one object was replaced by a new one and the rat was placed back in the box and exposed to the familiar object and to a novel test object for a further 10-min (acquisition phase). The short-term memory was tested 10min after “sample phase” (10-min retention) and long-term memory 24h after sample phase (24h retention) on the same cohorts of animals. The experimenters measured the time spent exploring each object. The recognition index was calculated as the percentage ratio of time spent exploring the novel object over total exploration time during acquisition phase. In mice experiments, we fine-tuned the level of difficulty of the memory test by increasing the number of objects during sample phase to three different objects instead of two identical ones. During the retention interval the most biased object, usually the least explored one, was replaced by a new novel object. The recognition index was calculated as the ratio of time spent exploring the novel object over time spent exploring the most explored object between the two familiar ones. The time spent exploring the least explored familiar object was not considered, to avoid potential bias. All behavioral assays were performed by the experimenters who were blind to the design of the study. Following the NORT test, animals were perfused for the detection of presynaptic terminal puncta or TNF-α staining Measurement of locomotor activity in behavioral test In eight-arm radial maze test, animal activity (locomotion) was determined by a simple calculation based on the amount of time spent in the maze and the number of arms crossed: (no. of arms × 160 cm)/(time (in s) spent in maze), where 160 equals the length from one arm tip to opposite arm tip. In NORT test, the total number of touch all objects were accounted. There was no significant difference in locomotion between SNI and sham groups. The similar results were also observed between drug injection group and vehicle injection group (Supplementary Figure 1). Intracerebroventricular injection of rrTNF or TNF antibody The rat anesthetized with chloral hydrate was placed in a stereotaxic frame and a cannula was implanted in the lateral cerebral ventricle according to the following co-ordinates: 1.2 mm caudal to the bregma, 1.8 mm lateral to the sagittal suture and 3.7-4.3 mm below the surface of the skull. Three days after operation, recombinant rat TNF (rrTNF, R&D Systems, 1 µg/ml) or artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) was injected intracerebroventricularly in a volume of 5 µl in 10 min for 3 times (daily). Three days after last injection the memory tests started. To test the effect of intracerebroventricular injection of TNF-α antibody on memory impairment produced by SNI, cannula was implanted 7 days before SNI and TNF-α antibody (anti-rat TNF-α antibody, R&D Systems, 250 µg/ml) or aCSF (in a volume of 5 µl in 10 min) was injected into lateral ventricle 2h before SNI and in 7 consecutive days after SNI (daily). Three days after cessation of injection, the memory function was first evaluated with NORT, and then with eight-arm radial maze test. The levels of TNF-α in CSF and in hippocampus were accessed following the behavioral tests. In the preliminary experiment, we found that 3h after i.c.v. injection of rrTNF at 1 µg/ml (5 µl in volume in 10min), the concentrations of TNF-α in CSF was 231 ± 10pg/ml. Previous study reported the volume of rat’s CSF is about 2 ml (Grant et al, 1991; Kim et al, 1996). Considering the increase in TNF-α in CSF persisted for at least 20d after SNI and the peak concentration of TNF-α in CSF following SNI was 156.3 ± 2.8 pg/ml (Fig. 4a), the rrTNF at a concentration of 1 µg/ml and the TNF antibody at 250 µg/ml were used for intracerebroventricular injection. Intra-hippocampal injection of rrTNF The cannulae were implanted in bilateral CA1 region of the rostral hippocampus in rats under chloral hydrate anesthesia. The co-ordinates of implantation were following: 3.4 mm caudal to the bregma, 2.5 mm lateral to the sagittal suture and 3.5 mm below the surface of the skull. Seven days after operation rrTNF (200 ng/ml) was injected for 3 times (daily) bilaterally in a volume of 0.5 μl over 1 min period. The injection needles were left in the cannula for 1 min after injection. Three days after cessation of injection, the NORT and eight-arm radial maze tests were successively performed. The levels of TNF-α in CSF and in hippocampus were measured after memory tests. As shown in Fig. 4b, the increase in TNF-α in hippocampus persisted for at least 40d following SNI and the peak concentration reached to 74.8 ± 1.8 pg/100 mg tissue. As hippocampus of is about 60 mg, rrTNF at a concentration of 200ng/ml (0.5 µl in each side) was injected. In control group the same volume of aCSF was injected. TNF-α bioassay To harvest rat cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), the anesthetized animal was placed in the stereotaxic frame and foramen magnum was exposed but dura kept intact. CSF was collected with a tiny needle connected to a syringe (1ml). Animals were sacrificed by decapitation after collecting CSF and the brains were rapidly removed at 4 °C ice-cold PBS. Hippocampus was weighed and homogenized in ice-cold PBS using a Potter homogenizer (1000 rpm, 10 strokes). The homogenates and the CSF were centrifuged for 15min (12000 rpm, 4 °C). Blood was collected from left ventricle of the animals to abstract plasma. After sample standing for 1h in room temperature, raw plasma was drawn off and then centrifuged for 10min (1000rpm, 4 °C). The supernatant was abstracted and stored in -20℃ for TNF-α bioassay. The amount of TNF-α was determined using anti-rat TNF-α ELISA Kits (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) according to the manufacturer’s protocol. REFERENCES Grant R, Condon B, Hart I, Teasdale GM (1991). Changes in intracranial CSF volume after lumbar puncture and their relationship to post-LP headache. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 54: 440-442. Kim YJ, McFarlane C, Warner DS, Baker MT, Choi WW, Dexter F (1996). The effects of plasma and brain magnesium concentrations on lidocaine-induced seizures in the rat. Anesth Analg 83: 1223-1228.