Multigrade Instruction Multigrade Instruction Final Exam Betty Krygsheld Seattle Pacific University Edu 6524 School Curriculm Summer On-line 2006 1 Multigrade Instruction 2 Multigrade instruction; is it an option for fostering academic growth in today’s society? Scholars as notable as Piaget (1973) have weighed in on the subject. Studies have been done by Veenman, (1996) Mason &Burns,(1996) Vincent ( 1999 )and Miller.(1991) It seems as through there is not a clear consensus as to the validity of multigrade instruction and academic achievement. Multigrade instruction has a long history in the U, S. and around the world. In the early part of our nation’s history the one-room school house was the norm. In general, the 1800 and early 1900s were characterized by rural living. Towns and settlements were small. Often there were not enough children in an area to set up a school on a singlegrade basis. In 1918 there were 196,037 one-room schoolhouses, representing 70.8 percent of all public schools in the U.S. By 1980 less than 1,000 of these schools remained. (Muse, Smith, &Barker, 1987) Often in the one-room schoolhouse, children moved through school grades based on their mastery of the subjects. As the 1900s progressed, the number of students increased, particularly in urban areas. It simply was easier to organized large numbers of students into age divisions or grades. To a large extent, children progressed through the grades based on their age. In most states today, fewer than three percent of the classes are organized in a multigrade fashion. At this point in our nation’s educational history there has developed a distinction between multiage, and multigrade classrooms. Both multiage and multigrade instruction combine different grade or age levels of students within one classroom. Multiage instruction typically exists because schools deliberately group the students together believing that the range and diversity of the students in the group will benefit the student. Multiage classrooms typically are non-graded and have a focus on individual development. Students progress through the curriculum at their own pace rather than whole class progression. The benefits of multiage instruction lie in the interaction of the younger and older students. Younger students use older students to develop skills and gain knowledge. Older students take responsibility for helping younger students. Students are also able to find others of matching abilities in these classes, creating less alienation of the student within a class. Multigrade classes, on the other hand, are generally formed because of economic necessity. Rural schools simply do not have a large enough population for single-grade classes. Urban schools use multigrade instruction because of fluctuating population. Multigrade classes are defined in research as those classes in which two or more adjacent grades levels are taught in one classroom, by one teacher for most or all of the day. Multigrade classes fall into the graded tradition. Students retain their grade level and are promoted with their grade level. Basic skills, reading, mathematics, and language, are generally taught at separate grade levels, meaning the teacher has to prepare a separate lesson for each grade level. The other subjects are often taught as a whole class. (Veenman1996, Mason & Burns 1996) Multigrade classrooms are regarded with skepticism, especially by parents. The classroom organization is not in keeping with mainstream education and is thought of as Multigrade Instruction 3 a ‘second-best’ solution. The question simply is ‘Are multigrade classrooms as effective, academically, as single grade classrooms?’ This is a straightforward question with several different answers. Research is simply not conclusive one way or another. Veenman (1996) claims that there is no difference academically between single-grade and multigrade class achievement. Miller (1990) also concluded that there was no difference between the achievement levels of the two. Mason and Burn(1996) however, noting the difficult teaching situation that is present in multigrade classes suggests that this finding is simply not logical. Miller’s study compared the outcomes of multiage and multigrade classrooms to the classrooms of single grades. He concluded that multiage/ multigrade teaching did not negatively affect academic outcomes. His work is seriously limited however since it does not make a distinction between multiage and multigrade. With our state policies at present stressing grade level curriculum outcomes, the distinction must be made. If children are required to be working at a specific grade level, then individualization is not in step with current policies. Thus to combine these two categories into one is quite misleading. Veenman avoided Miller’s pitfalls by making a clear distinction between multiage and multigrade instruction. Veenman’s ‘best evidence synthesis’ required ‘locating all research on a given topic, establishing well-specified criteria of methodological adequacy and germaneness to the topic, and then reviewing this best evidence with attention to the substantive and methodological contributions of each study.’ (Veenman 1996) His study included schools from 12 different countries and was carried out mainly in two-grade multigrade classrooms in schools that were predominantly single-grade. Veenman found that, in general, teachers of multigrade classroom tend to 1)teach each grade in their classroom separately with one group receiving instruction while the other group works on seatwork. 2) Collaborative work was not used and peer tutoring was not used to any significant degree. 3) Children have less direct instruction time from their teacher and less time on task. In spite of these drawbacks, Veenman reported no differences between single-graded and multigraded classrooms in cognitive outcomes. Even though Veenman found no difference between multigrade and single-grade classrooms academically, he was critical of the teaching methods in the multigrade classroom. He suggests that the multigrade teachers use the opportunity to use grouping strategies more efficiently, including across-grade teaching, cross-grade tutoring and peer tutoring to make their teaching more effective. Veenman suggest that these aspects are lacking because multigrade teachers lack adequate appropriate training for working with multiage classrooms. Veenman seems to suggest that if multigrade classrooms are needed, they should be taught using multiage methods. This kind of individualization could cause problems for school districts. If testing is linked to accountability, educators are less likely to use individualized methods. Several other researchers agree with the idea of using multiage methods in a multigrade classroom. Maccoby (1992) refers to Piaget’s Social Theory in noting the importance of peers in children’s academic and social development. He points out the connection Multigrade Instruction 4 between the theory of reciprocity and the positive effect on child academic and social behavior when close relationships are established between children and caregivers. Piaget viewed moral development as the result of interpersonal interactions through which individuals work out resolutions. Piaget claimed that a child must work and rework adult commands to construct new and personal norms and achieve moral autonomy. As the child becomes morally autonomous, he is more energized toward intellectual development. Reciprocity in peer relations can provide the psychological foundation that enables a child to consider more than one point of view, and therefore enables the child to move toward moral autonomy. Children, Piaget claims, are more easily able to think and act autonomously with other children than with other adults. Piaget spoke about affectivity in a broad sense as the energetic source on which the functioning of intelligence depends. ((Nucci 2005) The idea of a multiage/multigrade classroom provides consistency over time in relationships between the teachers, children and parents, and thus provides the groundwork for a child’s affective development. Not all researchers agree that multigrade classrooms have no effect on academic achievement. In answer to Veenman’s 1996 article, Simply No Worse, and Simply No Better, Mason and Burns wrote the article “Simply No Worse, and Simply No Better” May Simply Be Wrong: A Critique of Veenman’s Conclusion About Multigrade Classes. As the title suggests, Mason and Burns conclude that multigrade classes do have a slight negative effect on achievement. Further they suggest that confounding variables in Veenman’s research should be included as part of the explanation for his no-difference finding. Mason and Burns point out that the research was flawed on the basis of selection. Veenman used multigrade classes that were part of schools that were predominantly single-grade schools. The reason for this was to control the variable of school atmosphere. It would have been invalid to compare a small, rural, multi-grade school to an urban single-grade school. Thus Veenman makes the choice to use multigrade classes from predominantly single-grade schools. However nearly always when a single-grade school must set up a multigrade classroom, students are purposefully selected for the multigrade classroom. The students who are selected are able students and capable of independent work. Teachers are also selected on the basis of capability. So although Mason and Burns’ findings were similar to Veenman’s, they suggest that there is a slight negative effect on achievement based on their further categorization of studies into purposeful assignment and nonpurposeful assignment groups. Mason and Burns went on to suggest that teaching in the multigrade environment was complex and generally disadvantageous. It required extra effort and time on the teacher’s part as well as adapting the schedule by taking time away from those subjects that were considered less essential. Whereas Veenman suggests that multigrade teachers simply must readjust the teaching style, Mason and Burn seem to be pointing to the idea of teacher efficacy. Teacher efficacy in this sense is defined as a teacher’s perceived expectation of succeeding at a task (the task of affecting student learning) through personal effort. (Lee, Dedrick& Smith 1991) Mason and Burns’ study showed that teachers of multigrade classes felt they were unable to give the individual help the students needed because their time was so limited.. Teachers knew what ought to be done, but they were unable to do it. This also brings about pressure from concerned parents. Under these circumstances, the teacher’s sense of efficacy is likely to be Multigrade Instruction 5 diminished. Diminished teacher efficacy has been linked to negative student achievement. (Ashton &Webb 1986) To sum up this idea of multigrade instruction and teacher efficacy Mason and Burns say: Although progressive teaching practices may indeed make multigrade and multiage classrooms exciting and challenging learning environments, and there are teachers that thrive on such challenges, we think teachers will require considerable support and will need to expend considerable effort to reap rewards from these classrooms. Lacking such support, most teachers find multigrade classes to be difficult classroom environments to manage… (Mason &Burns 1996) The curricular issue I am writing about for my final exam then, is one that faced me in August of 2005. The school in which I teach had 18 fifth graders and 11 fourth graders enrolled. The board was faced with deciding whether to combine the fourth and fifth grade. Although my opinion was not asked, I had a great many reservations about this multigrade class. Mason &Burn as well as Veenman agree on several key issues concerning the formation of a multigrade class. They suggest that if class size grows any larger than 25, negative academic growth will be seen. They also suggest the number of students with learning disabilities should be somewhat limited. Since neither of these factors was limited, I was quite concerned about this combination. However if the former issues had not presented themselves, would combining the classes have been a good idea? I do not believe that combining classes on the basis of fluctuating population is a good idea. I understand the value of multiage classes within a school. If the school board wants to create a multiage class for the purpose of experimenting with the effect of close, long-term relationships between teachers and students, I believe the outcome could be very positive. I think Piaget is correct when he suggests that affectivity is the fuel that makes the motor of intelligence go. (Nucci 2005) Multiage instruction is a wonderful environment for encouraging a child’s affective development. Multigrade decisions made by a school board however, often have little to do with child development. In general the decision is made to combine classes for economic reasons. Since school population fluctuates greatly in small private schools, as soon as the population in those grade levels increases, the grades will again become single-grade classes. The benefits from multiage instruction are simply not realized under these circumstances. It is my belief that classes should be combined only if the school wishes to leave them in combination for at least two years. If multigrade classes are not kept in tact for at least two years, the benefits of multiage instruction will not be realized. If the benefits are not realized, all that is left are the disadvantages. Parents as well as teachers are left unhappy with the situation. Sources Ashton, P.T., & Webb, R. B., (1986). Making a difference: Teachers’ sense of efficacy and student achievement. New York: Longman. Multigrade Instruction 6 Lee, V., Dedick, R., & Smith, J. (1991). The effect of the social organization of schools on teachers’ efficacy and satisfaction. Sociology of Education, 64, 190-208. Maccoby, E.E. (1992). The adequate implication of a multigrade approach. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1006-1017. Mason, D.A. & Burns, R.B. (1996). Simply no worse and simply no better” may simply be wrong : A critique of Veenman’s conclusion about multigrade classes. Review of educational Research, 66 (3), 307-322. Miller B. (1991). Review of qualitative research on multigrade instruction. Research in Rural Education 7 (2), 3-12. Muse, I., Smith R. & Barker, B. (1987). The one-teacher schoolhouse in the 1980s. Las Cruces, NM: ERIC Clearinghouse on Rural Education and Small Schools. (ERIC Document Service No. ED287646. Piaget. (1981). Intelligence and Affection: their Relation During Child Development. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. Veenman, S. (1995). Cognitive and non-cognitive effects of multigrade and multi-age classes: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65(4), 319381. Veenman, S. (1996). Effects of multigrade and multi-age classes reconsidered. Review of Educational Research, 66(3), 323-340. Vincent, S. (1999). Book 1,a Review of research on multigrade instruction. Northwest Educational Laboratory. Portland, OR.