Shanghai International Studies University 5 November 2010 Categories in the Brain Prototypicality, Subcategorization, Thinking Sydney Lamb Rice University lamb@rice.edu “to know is to categorize” Jeffrey Ellis Topics in this presentation • • • • Phenomena associated with categories Information in the brain Six Hypotheses Explaining the phenomena associated with categories Topics • • • • Phenomena associated with categories Information in the brain Six Hypotheses Explaining the phenomena associated with categories Phenomena associated with categories 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways Phenomena associated with categories: 1 1. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) – The feature-attribute model fails • Works for some mathematical objects, but doesn’t apply to the way people’s cognitive systems apprehend most things • Example: CUP Phenomena associated with categories: 2 1. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) 2. Fuzzy boundaries – Example: VEHICLE • Car, truck, bus • Airplane? • Boat? • Toy car, model airplane? • Raft? • Roller skate? • Snowboard? Fuzzy Categories • No fixed boundaries • Membership comes in degrees – Prototypical – Less prototypical – Peripheral – Metaphorical • The property of fuzziness relates closely to the phenomenon of prototypicality Phenomena associated with categories: 3 1. 2. 3. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members – – Prototypical • CAR, TRUCK, BUS Peripheral: • – AIRPLANE, TOY CAR, RAFT, ROLLER SKATE, etc. Varying degrees of peripherality Prototypicality phenomena • The category BIRD – Some members are prototypical • ROBIN, SPARROW – Others are peripheral • EMU, PENGUIN • The category VEHICLE – Prototypical: CAR, TRUCK, BUS – Peripheral: ROLLER SKATE, HANG GLIDER Phenomena associated with categories: 4 1. 2. 3. 4. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains – ANIMAL – MAMMAL – CARNIVORE – CANINE – DOG –TERRIER – JACK RUSSELL TERRIER – EDDIE – Each subcategory has the properties of the category plus additional properties Smallest subcategory has the most properties – Phenomena associated with categories: 5 1. 2. 3. 4. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains 5. Categories are in the mind, not in the real world – – In the world, everything • is unique • lacks clear boundaries • changes from day to day (even moment to moment) Whorf: “kaleidoscopic flux” Phenomena associated with categories: 6 1. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another English: bell French: cloche (of a church) clochette (on a cow) sonnette (of a door) grelot (of a sleigh) timbre (on a desk) glas (to announce a death) Phenomena associated with categories - 7 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways (B.L. Whorf) Why? 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways Why? Answer: Because of the structure of the brain 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways Topics • • • • Phenomena associated with categories Information in the brain Six Hypotheses Explaining the phenomena associated with categories How to explain? • We have to examine how our information about categories is represented in the brain • The brain is where our linguistic and cultural knowledge is represented • This recommendation is in line with a suggestion first made to linguists by Norman Geschwind in 1964 – Geschwind: a great neurologist – Said that linguists should consider brain struc\ture Sources of information about the brain • Aphasiology – Research findings during a century-and-a-half • Brain imaging • Neuroanatomy • Other research in neuroscience – E.g., Mountcastle, Perceptual Neuroscience (1998) Some things that are now well established • The brain is a network – Composed, ultimately, of neurons • Neurons are interconnected – Axons (with branches) – Dendrites (with branches) • Activity travels along neural pathways – Cortical neurons are clustered in columns • Columns come in different sizes – The smallest: minicolumn – 70-110 neurons • Each minicolumn acts as a unit – When it becomes active all its neurons are active • Locations of various kinds of “information” – Visual, auditory, tactile, motor, … Deductions from known facts • Everything represented in the brain has the form of a network – (the “human information system”) • Therefore a person’s linguistic and conceptual system is a network – (part of the information system) • Every lexeme and every concept is a sub-network – Term: functional web (Pulvermüller 2002) Concepts and percepts: Cortical representation • • Percept: one sensory modality – Locations are known • Auditory: temporal lobe • Visual: occipital lobe • Somatosensory: parietal lobe Concept: more than one sensory modality – Higher level – Angular gyrus, (?)temporal lobe, (?)SMG Example: The concept DOG • We know what a dog looks like – Visual information, in occipital lobe • We know what its bark sounds like – Auditory information, in temporal lobe • We know what its fur feels like – Somatosensory information, in parietal lobe • All of the above.. – constitute perceptual information – are subwebs with many nodes each – have to be interconnected into a larger web – along with further web structure for conceptual information Topics • • • • Phenomena associated with categories Information in the brain Six Hypotheses Explaining the phenomena associated with categories Topic 3 • Six Hypotheses – Functional webs – Cortical Columns – Nodal specificity • Adjacency – Extrapolation to humans • And to linguistic and conceptual structure – Hierarchy in functional webs – Cardinal nodes Hypothesis I: Functional Webs • • A concept is represented as a functional web Spread over a wide area of cortex – Includes perceptual information – As well as specifically conceptual information • For nominal concepts, mainly in – Angular gyrus – (?) For some, middle temporal gyrus – (?) For some, supramarginal gyrus Building a model of a functional web: First steps Each node in this diagram represents the cardinal node* of a subweb of properties T C For example M V C – Conceptual M – Motor T – Tactile V – Visual *to be defined in a moment! Add phonological recognition For example, FORK C T M P V These are all cardinal nodes – each is supported by a subweb Labels for Properties: C – Conceptual M – Motor P – Phonological image T – Tactile V – Visual The phonological image of the spoken form [fork] (in Wernicke’s area) Add node in primary auditory area For example, FORK C T M P PA V Labels for Properties: C – Conceptual M – Motor P – Phonological image PA – Primary Auditory T – Tactile V – Visual Primary Auditory: the cortical structures in the primary auditory cortex that are activated when the ears receive the vibrations of the spoken form [fork] Add node for phonological production For example, FORK C T M P PP PA Arcuate fasciculus V Labels for Properties: C – Conceptual M – Motor P – Phonological image PA – Primary Auditory PP – Phonological Production T – Tactile V – Visual Part of the functional web for FORK (showing cardinal nodes only) Each node shown here is the cardinal node of a subweb T M PP C P PA V For example, the cardinal node of the visual subweb An activated functional web (with two subwebs partly shown) T C PP PR PA M C – Cardinal concept node M – Memories PA – Primary auditory PP – Phonological production PR – Phonological recognition T – Tactile V – Visual V Visual features Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Ignition of a functional web from visual input T C PR Art PA M V Speaking as a response to ignition of a web T C PR Art PA M V Speaking as a response to ignition of a web T C PR Art PA M V Speaking as a response to ignition of a web T C PR Art PA M From here (via subcortical structures) to the muscles that control the organs of articulation V An MEG study from Max Planck Institute Hypothesis II: Nodes as Cortical Columns • Information is represented in the cortex in the form of functional webs (Hypothesis I) – A functional web is a network within the cortical network as a whole • consisting of nodes and their interconnections – connections represented in graphs as lines • Nodes are implemented as cortical columns • The interconnections are represented by inter-columnar neural connections and synapses – Axonal fibers – Dendritic fibers The node as a cortical column • The properties of the cortical column are approximately those described by Vernon Mountcastle – Mountcastle, Perceptual Neuroscience, 1998 • Additional properties of columns and functional webs can be derived from Mountcastle’s treatment together with neurolinguistic findings – Method: “connecting the dots” • Hypothesis IV: (Coming Soon!) “[T]he effective unit of operation…is not the single neuron and its axon, but bundles or groups of cells and their axons with similar functional properties and anatomical connections.” Vernon Mountcastle, Perceptual Neuroscience (1998), p. 192 The Cerebral Cortex – coronal section Grey matter • Columns of neurons White matter • Inter-column connections Evidence for columns • Experiments on living cats, monkeys, rats • Microelectrode penetrations in cortex • If perpendicular to cortical surface – Neurons all of same response properties • If not perpendicular – Neurons of different response properties • Conclusion: All neurons of a single column respond to stimuli – alike – and differently from those of adjacent columns Microelectrode penetrations in the paw area of a cat’s cortex Columns for orientation of lines (visual cortex) Microelectrode penetrations K. Obermayer & G.G. Blasdell, 1993 The (Mini)Column • Width is about (or just larger than) the diameter of a single pyramidal cell – About 30–50 m in diameter • Extends thru the six cortical layers – Three to six mm in length – The entire thickness of the cortex is accounted for by the columns • Roughly cylindrical in shape • If expanded by a factor of 100, the dimensions would correspond to a tube with diameter of 1/8 inch and length of one foot Cortical Column Structure • Minicolumn 30-50 microns diameter • Recurrent axon collaterals of pyramidal neurons activate other neurons in same column • Inhibitory neurons can inhibit neurons of neighboring columns – Function: contrast • Excitatory connections can activate neighboring columns – In this case we get a bundle of contiguous columns acting as a unit Cortical minicolumns: Quantities • • • • • • Diameter of minicolumn: 30 microns Neurons per minicolumn: 70-110 (avg. 75-80) Minicolumns/mm2 of cortical surface: 1460 Minicolumns/cm2 of cortical surface: 146,000 Neurons under 1 sq mm of cortical surface: 110,000 Approximate number of minicolumns in Wernicke’s area: 2,920,000 (at 20 sq cm for Wernicke’s area) Adapted from Mountcastle 1998: 96 Topological essence of cortical structure (known facts from neuroanatomy) • The thickness of the cortex is entirely accounted for by the columns • Hence, the cortex is an array of nodes – A two-dimensional structure of interconnected nodes (columns) • Third dimension for – Internal structure of the nodes (columns) – Cortico-cortical connections (white matter) Nodal interconnections (known facts from neuroanatomy) • Nodes (columns) are connected to – Nearby nodes – Distant nodes • Connections to nearby nodes are either excitatory or inhibitory – Via horizontal axons (through gray matter) • Connections to distant nodes are excitatory only – Via long (myelinated) axons of pyramidal neurons Simplified model of minicolumn I: Activation of neurons in a column Other cortical locations Cell Types II III Pyramidal Spiny Stellate Thalamus IV Inhibitory Connections to neighboring columns not shown V VI Subcortical locations Simplified model of minicolumn II: Inhibition of competitors Other cortical locations Cell Types II III Pyramidal Spiny Stellate Thalamus IV Inhibitory V VI Cells in neighboring columns Local and distal connections excitatory inhibitory Findings relating to columns (Mountcastle, Perceptual Neuroscience, 1998) • The column is the fundamental module of perceptual systems – probably also of motor systems • This columnar structure is found in all mammals that have been investigated • The theory is confirmed by detailed studies of visual, auditory, and somatosensory perception in living cat and monkey brains Hypothesis III: Nodal Specificity in functional webs • Every node in a functional web has a specific function • The nodes in each area of a functional web – Constitute a subweb – Their function fits the portion of cortex in which they are located • For example, – Phonological recognition in Wernicke’s area – Visual subweb in occipital and lower temporal lobe – Tactile subweb in parietal lobe – Each node of a subweb also has a specific function within that of the subweb Support for Nodal Specificity: the paw area of a cat’s cortex Column (node) represents specific location on paw Support for Nodal Specificity: Columns for orientation of lines (visual cortex) Microelectrode penetrations K. Obermayer & G.G. Blasdell, 1993 Hypothesis III(a): Adjacency • Nodes of related function are in adjacent locations – More closely related function, more closely adjacent • Examples: – Adjacent locations on cat’s paw represented by adjacent cortical locations – Similar line orientations represented by adjacent cortical locations Support for Nodal adjacency: the paw area of a cat’s cortex Adjacent column in cortex for adjacent location on paw Extrapolation to Language? • Our knowledge of cortical columns comes mostly from studies of perception in cats, monkeys, and rats • Such studies haven’t been done for language – Cats and monkeys don’t have language – That kind of neurosurgical experiment isn’t done on human beings • Are they relevant to language anyway? – Relevant if language uses similar cortical structures – Relevant if linguistic functions are like perceptual functions Hypothesis IV: Extrapolation to Humans • Hypothesis: The findings about cortical structure and function from experiments on cats, monkeys, and rats can be extrapolated to human cortical structure and function • In fact, this hypothesis is simply assumed to be valid by neuroscientists • Why? We know from neuroanatomy that, locally, – Cortical structure is relatively uniform across mammals – Cortical function is relatively uniform across mammals Hypothesis IV(a): Linguistic and conceptual structure • Hypothesis IV(a): The extrapolation can be extended to linguistic and conceptual structures and functions • Why? – Local uniformity of cortical structure and function across all human cortical areas except for primary areas • Primary visual and primary auditory are known to have specialized structures, across mammals • Higher level areas are – locally – highly uniform Objection • Cats and monkeys don’t have language • Therefore language must have unique properties of its structural representation in the cortex • Answer: Yes, language is different, but – The differences are a consequence not of different (local) structure but differences of connectivity – The network does not have different kinds of structure for different kinds of information • Rather, different connectivities Hypothesis V: Hierarchy in functional webs • A functional web is hierarchically organized – Bottom levels in primary areas – Lower levels closer to primary areas – Higher (more abstract) levels in • Associative areas – e.g., angular gyrus • Executive areas – prefrontal • These higher areas are much larger in humans than in other mammals • Hypothesis V(a): Each subweb is likewise hierarchically organized Properties of Hierachy • Each level has fewer nodes than lower levels, more than higher levels – Compare the organization of management of a corporation • Top level has just one node – Compare the “CEO” Hypothesis VI: Cardinal nodes • Every functional web has a cardinal node – At the top of the entire functional web – Unique to that concept – For example, C/cat/ at “top” of the web for CAT • Hypothesis VI(a): – Each subweb likewise has a cardinal node • At the top level of the subweb • Unique to that subweb • For example, V/cat/ – At the top of the visual subweb Cardinal nodes of a functional web Some of the cortical structure relating to fork Each node shown here is the cardinal node of a subweb Cardinal node of the whole web T M PP C P PA V Cardinal node of the visual subweb (Part of) the functional web for CAT The cardinal node for the entire functional web T C P A M V Cardinal nodes of the subwebs Support for the cardinal node hypothesis - 1 It follows from the hypotheses of nodal specificity and hierarchy – A hierarchy must have a highest level – The node at this level must have a specific function 2. It is needed for ignition of the whole web from activation of part of it – For example, to activate the phonological representation from the visual 3. It is automatically recruited in learning anyway, according to the Hebbian learning hypothesis 1. More support for cardinal nodes Example: FORK • The web as a whole represents the concept – For example, the concept FORK • The whole can evidently be activated by any part of • the network – From seeing a fork – From eating with a fork – Etc. The cardinal node provides the coordinated organization that makes such reactivation possible Reactivating the functional web • When the cardinal node (the integrating node) is activated, it can activate the whole (distributed) functional web – Without it, how would that be possible? – E.g., activating conceptual and perceptual properties of cat upon hearing the word cat – From phonological recognition to concepts – From visual image to phonological representation Cardinal nodes and the linguistic sign • • Connection of conceptual to phonological representation Consider two possibilities 1. A cardinal node for the concept connected to a cardinal node for the phonological image 2. No cardinal nodes: multiple connections between concept representation and phonological image • supported by Pulvermüller (2002) Implications of possibility 2 • • • • No cardinal nodes: multiple connections between concept representation and phonological image I.e., different parts of meaning connected to different parts of phonological image Consider fork – Maybe /f-/ connects to the shape? – Maybe /-or-/ connects to the feeling of holding a fork in the hand? – Maybe /-k/ connects to the knowledge that fork is related to knife? Conclusion: Possibility 2 must be rejected Topics • • • • Phenomena associated with categories Information in the brain Six Hypotheses Explaining the phenomena associated with categories REVIEW Phenomena associated with categories 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways REVIEW How to explain? • Description is fine, but its only a start • Next step: Explanation • How to explain? – By answering the question of how categories are represented in the brain Phenomena associated with categories: 1-3 No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) – Example: CUP – More realistic alternative: radial categories 2. Fuzzy boundaries – Example: VEHICLE 3. Prototypical members and peripheral members 1. – VEHICLE • Prototypical: – • • CAR, TRUCK, BUS Peripheral: – AIRPLANE, TOY CAR, RAFT, ROLLER SKATE, etc. – Varying degrees of peripherality These three phenomena are interdependent How do radial categories work? • Different connections have different strengths (weights) • More important properties have greater strengths • For CUP, – Important (but not necessary!) properties: • Short (as compared with a glass) • Ceramic • Having a handle • Cups with these properties are more prototypical The properties of a category have different weights The cardinal node CUP T MADE OF GLASS SHORT CERAMIC The properties are represented by nodes which are connected to lower-level nodes HAS HANDLE Nodes have activation thresholds • The node will be activated by any of many different combinations of properties • The key word is enough – it takes enough activation from enough properties to satisfy the threshold • The node will be activated to different degrees by different combinations of properties – When strongly activated, it transmits stronger activation to its downstream nodes. Prototypical exemplars provide stronger and more rapid activation The cardinal node Activation threshold (can be satisfied to varying degrees) CUP T MADE OF GLASS SHORT CERAMIC Stronger connections carry more activation HAS HANDLE Explaining Prototypicality • Cardinal category nodes get more activation from the prototypical exemplars – More heavily weighted property nodes • E.g., FLYING is strongly connected to BIRD – Property nodes more strongly activated • Peripheral items (e.g. EMU) provide only weak activation, weakly satisfying the threshold (emus can’t fly) • Borderline items may or may not produce enough activation to satisfy threshold Activation of different sets of properties produces greater or lesser satisfaction of the activation threshold of the cardinal node CUP Inhibitory connection MADE OF GLASS SHORT CERAMIC HAS HANDLE More important properties have stronger connections, indicated here by thickness of lines Explaining prototypicality: Summary • • • • Variation in strength of connections Many connecting properties of varying strength Varying degrees of activation Prototypical members receive stronger activation from more associated properties • BIRD is strongly connected to the property FLYING – Emus and ostriches don’t fly – But they have some properties connected with BIRD – Sparrows and robins do fly • And as commonly occurring birds they have been experienced often, leading to entrenchment – stronger connections Phenomena associated with categories: 4 1. 2. 3. 4. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains – ANIMAL – MAMMAL – CARNIVORE – CANINE – DOG – TERRIER – JACK RUSSELL TERRIER – EDDIE – Each subcategory has the properties of the category plus additional properties Smallest subcategory has the most properties – How to explain? Perceptual Neuroscience • Hypothesis IV: Extrapolation • Hypothesis IV(a): Extrapolation can be extended to linguistic and conceptual structures • Why? Cortical structure, viewed locally, is – Uniform across mammalian species – Uniform across different cortical regions • Exceptions in primary visual and primary auditory areas • Different cortical regions have different functions – because of differences in connectivity – not because of differences in structure REVIEW In particular.. • Cortical structure and function, locally, are essentially the same in humans as in cats and monkeys and rats • Moreover, in humans, – The regions that support language have the same structure locally as other cortical regions Uniformity of cortical function • Claims: – Locally, all cortical processing is the same – The apparent differences of function are consequences of differences in larger-scale connectivity • Conclusion (if the claim is supported): – Understanding language, even at higher levels, is basically a perceptual process Local uniformity follows from the basic connectionist claim • • • Lines and nodes (i.e., columns) are approximately the same all over Uniformity of cortical structure – Same kinds of columnar structure – Same kinds of neurons – Same kinds of connections Conclusion: Different areas have different functions because of what they are connected to Cortical columns cannot store symbols • They only – Receive activation – Maintain activation – Inhibit competitors – Transmit activation • Important consequence: – We have linguistic information represented in the cortex without the use of symbols – It’s all in the connectivity • Challenge: – How? Columnar Functions: Integration and Broadcasting • Integration: A column is activated if it receives enough activation from – Other columns – Thalamus • Can be activated to varying degrees • Can keep activation alive for a period of time • Broadcasting: An activated column transmits activation to other columns – Exitatory – Inhibitory • Learning: adjustment of connection strengths and thresholds Integration and Broadcasting Broadcasting • To multiple locations • In parallel Integration Integration and Broadcasting Broadcasting Integration Now I’ll tell my friends! Wow, I got activated! REVIEW Conceptual systems and perceptual systems • Likewise, conceptual systems in humans evidently use the same structures as perceptual systems • Therefore it is not too great a stretch to suppose that experimental findings on the structure of perceptual systems in monkeys can be applied to an understanding of the structure of conceptual systems of human beings • In particular to the structures of conceptual categories Findings of Mountcastle: Columns of different sizes for categories and subcategories • Minicolumn – The smallest unit – 70-110 neurons • Functional column – Variable size – depends on experience – Intermediate between minicolumn and maxicolumn • Maxicolumn (a.k.a. column) – 100 to a few hundred minicolumns • Hypercolumn – Several contiguous maxicolumns Hypercolums: Modules of maxicolumns A visual area in temporal lobe of a macaque monkey Perceptual subcategories and columnar subdivisions of larger columns • Nodal specificity applies for maxicolumns as well as for minicolumns • The adjacency hypothesis likewise applies to larger categories and columns – Adjacency applies for adjacent maxicolumns • Subcategories of a category have similar function – Therefore their cardinal nodes should be in adjacent locations Functional columns • The minicolumns within a maxicolumn respond to a common set of features • Functional columns are intermediate in size between minicolumns and maxicolumns • Different functional columns within a maxicolumn are distinct because of non-shared additional features – Shared within the functional column – Not shared with the rest of the maxicolumn Mountcastle: “The neurons of a [maxi]column have certain sets of static and dynamic properties in common, upon which others that may differ are superimposed.” Similarly.. • Neurons of a hypercolumn may have similar response features, upon which others that differ may be superimposed • Result is maxicolumns in the hypercolumn sharing certain basic features while differing with respect to others • Such maxicolumns may be further subdivided into functional columns on the basis of additional features • That is, columnar structure directly maps categories and subcategories (!) Hypercolumns: Modules of maxicolumns A visual area in the temporal lobe of a macaque monkey Category (hypercolumn) Subcategory (can be further subdivided) Category representations in the cortex • Hypercolumn • Supercategory • Maxicolumn • Category • Functional column • Subcategory • Sub-functional column • Sub-subcategory Hypothesis applied to conceptual categories • A whole maxicolumn gets activated for a category – Example: BEAR • Different functional columns within the maxicolumn for subcategories – BROWN BEAR, GRIZZLY, POLAR BEAR, etc. • Adjacent maxicolumns for categories related to BEAR (sharing various features) – I.e. , other carnivores • Similarly, CUP has a column surrounded by columns for other drinking vessels Phenomena associated with categories: 5 1. 2. 3. 4. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains 5. Categories are in the mind, not in the real world – – In the world, everything • is unique • lacks clear boundaries • changes from day to day (even moment to moment) Whorf: “kaleidoscopic flux” REVIEW Phenomena associated with categories: 6 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another English: bell French: cloche clochette sonnette grelot timbre glas (of a church) (on a cow) (of a door) (of a sleigh) (on a desk) (to announce a death) Phenomena associated with categories - 7 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No small set of defining features (with rare exceptions) Fuzzy boundaries Prototypical members and peripheral members Subcategories, and sub-subcategories, in hierarchical chains Categories are in the mind, not in the real world Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways – B.L. Whorf These phenomena (5-7) are interrelated Categories are in the mind, not in the real world 6. Categories and their memberships vary from one language/culture system to another 7. Categories influence thinking, in both appropriate and inappropriate ways – B.L. Whorf 5. Pertinent neuroanatomical findings: Bidirectional Processing • An established fact of neuroanatomy: – A connection from point A to point B in the cortex is generally accompanied by a connection from point B to point A • Separate fibers (axons): (1) A to B, (2) B to A • In short, cortico-cortical connections are generally bidirectional Bidirectional processing and inference These connections are bidirectional CUP T MADE OF GLASS SHORT CERAMIC HANDLE Separate fibers for the two directions; shown as one line in the notation Bidirectional processing and inference Thought process: CUP T SHORT HANDLE 1. The cardinal concept node is activated by a subset of its property nodes 2. Feed-backward processing activates other property nodes Consequence: We “apprehend” properties that are not actually perceived Category Structure and Inference Category T Consequence: A If A and B, then E and F B C Properties F D E Examples • Looks like a duck – Probably quacks • Ceramic, cup-shaped, handle – Probably holds coffee (without breaking) • Dark clouds, thunder – It’s going to rain • ATM – Probably has money Another hypothesis of Whorf • Grammatical categories of a language influence the thinking of people who speak the language • Can we explain this too in terms of brain structure? Mechanisms of operation Entrenchment – Strengthening of connections through repeated activation • An automatic brain process • Important in learning 2. Reverberation of activation 3. Priming 4. Language as a major means of learning conceptual and perceptual distinctions 1. Entrenchment and thinking: a mechanism • Connections become stronger with use – (entrenchment) • Grammatical categories make speakers constantly heed selected phenomena • Connections for phenomena which speakers must constantly heed.. – Will be repeatedly traversed – Therefore will get progressively stronger Thinking: Reverberating Activation Speaking and thinking in English: • Reverberating activation among categories and images of English Thinking in German or Spanish or Yucatec • Reverberating activation among categories and images of German or Spanish or Yucatec “When I speak Indian, I think differently” Wallace Chafe’s Oneida informant Example: Grammatical gender • Does talking about inanimate objects as if they were masculine or feminine actually lead people to think of inanimate objects as having a gender? • Could the grammatical genders assigned to objects by a language influence people’s mental representation of objects? Boroditsky (2003) Plausibility of the possibility • Children learning to speak a language with grammatical gender may suppose that gender indicates a meaningful distinction between types of objects • Other grammatical distinctions do reflect actual perceptual differences: singular:plural Children learning a language with gender • “For all they know, the grammatical genders assigned by their language are the true universal genders of objects.” Boroditsky et al, 2003 Experiment: Gender and Associations (Boroditsky et al. 2002) • Subjects: speakers of Spanish or German – All were fluent also in English – English used as language of experiment • Task: Write down the 1st 3 adjectives that come to mind to describe each object – All the (24) objects have opposite gender in German and Spanish • Raters of adjectives: Native English speakers Examples: • Key (masc in German, fem in Spanish) – Adjectives used by German speakers: • Hard, heavy, jagged, metal, serrated, useful – Adjectives used by Spanish speakers: • Golden, intricate, little, lovely, shiny, tiny • Bridge (fem in German, masc in spanish) – Adjectives used by German speakers: • Beautiful, elegant, fragile, peaceful, pretty – Adjectives used by Spanish speakers: • Big, dangerous, long, strong, sturdy, towering Results of the Experiment (Boroditsky et al. 2002) • Raters of adjectives were native English speakers • Result: Adjectives were rated as masculine or feminine in agreement with the gender in subject’s native language In conclusion.. All of these phenomena associated with categories (briefly reviewed in this presentation) can be explained as inevitable consequences of the structure and function of the human brain Thank you for your attention! References Boroditsky, Lera, Schmidt, Phillips. 2003. Sex, syntax, and semantics. Language in Mind (eds. Dedre Gentner & Susan Goldin-Meadow), MIT Press, 2003. Geschwind, Norman. 1964. The development of the brain and the evolution of language. Georgetown Round Table on Languages and Linguistics 17.155-169. Lamb, Sydney, 1999. Pathways of the Brain: The Neurocognitive Basis of Language. John Benjamins. Mountcastle, Vernon, 1998. Perceptual Neuroscience: The Cerebral Cortex. Harvard University Press. Pulvermüller, Friedemann, 2002. The Neuroscience of Language. Cambridge University Press Whorf, Benjamin Lee. 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality (ed. John B. Carroll). MIT Press. For further information . . www.rice.edu/langbrain