Unreplicated ANOVA designs Block and repeated measures analyses Gerry Quinn & Mick Keough, 1998 Do not copy or distribute without permission of authors. Blocking • Aim: – Reduce unexplained variation, without increasing size of experiment. • Approach: – Group experimental units (“replicates”) into blocks. – Blocks usually spatial units, 1 experimental unit from each treatment in each block. Walter & O’Dowd (1992) • Effects of domatia (cavities of leaves) on number of mites - use a single shrub in field • Two treatments – shaving domatia which removes domatia from leaves – normal domatia as control • Required 14 leaves for each treament • Set up as completely randomised design – 28 leaves randomly allocated to each of 2 treatments Completely randomised design Control leaves Shaved domatia leaves Completely randomised ANOVA No. of treatments or groups for factor A = a (2 for domatia), number of replicates = n (14 pairs of leaves) Source Factor A Residual Total general df example df a-1 a(n-1) 1 26 an-1 27 Walter & O’Dowd (1992) • Effects of domatia (cavities of leaves) on number of mites - use a single shrub in field • Two treatments – shaving domatia which removes domatia from leaves – normal domatia as control • Required 14 leaves for each treament • Set up as blocked design – paired leaves (14 pairs) chosen - 1 leaf in each pair shaved, 1 leaf in each pair control 1 block Control leaves Shaved domatia leaves Rationale for blocking • Micro-temperature, humidity, leaf age, etc. more similar within block than between blocks • Variation in DV (mite number) between leaves within block (leaf pair) < variation between leaves between blocks Rationale for blocking • Some of unexplained (residual) variation in DV from completely randomised design now explained by differences between blocks • More precise estimate of treatment effects than if leaves were chosen completely randomly from shrub Null hypotheses • No effect of treatment (Factor A) – HO: m1 = m2 = m3 = ... = m – HO: a1 = a2 = a3 = ... = 0 (ai = mi - m) – no effect of shaving domatia, pooling blocks • No effect of blocks (?) – no difference between blocks (leaf pairs), pooling treatments Randomised blocks ANOVA No. of treatments or groups for factor A = p (2 for domatia), number of blocks = q (14 pairs of leaves) Source Factor A Blocks Residual Total general example p-1 q-1 (p-1)(q-1) pq-1 1 13 13 27 Randomised block ANOVA • Randomised block ANOVA is 2 factor factorial design – BUT no replicates within each cell (treatment-block combination), i.e. unreplicated 2 factor design – No measure of within-cell variation – No test for treatment by block interaction Expected mean squares If factor A is fixed and Blocks (B) are random: MSA (Treatments) s2 + sab2 + n (ai)2/a-1 MSBlocks s2 + nsb2 MSResidual s2 + sab2 Cannot separately estimate s2 and sab2: • no replicates within each block-treatment combination. Null hypotheses • If HO of no effects of factor A is true: – all ai’s = 0 and all m’s are the same – then F-ratio MSA / MSResidual 1. • If HO of no effects of factor A is false: – then F-ratio MSA / MSResidual > 1. Walter & O’Dowd (1992) Factor A (treatment - shaved and unshaved domatia) - fixed, Blocks (14 pairs of leaves) random: Source Treatment Block Residual df 1 13 13 MS F 31.34 1.77 2.77 11.32 0.64 P 0.005 0.784 ?? Randomised block vs completely randomised designs • Total number of replicates is same in both designs – 28 leaves in total for domatia experiment • Block designs rearrange spatial pattern of replicates into blocks: – “replicates” in block designs are the blocks • Test of factor A (treatments) has fewer df in block design: – reduced power of test Randomised block vs completely randomised designs • MSResidual smaller in block design if blocks explain some of variation in DV: – increased power of test • If decrease in MSResidual (unexplained variation) outweighs loss of df, then block design is better: – when blocks explain a lot of variation in DV Assumptions • Normality of DV – boxplots etc. • No interaction between blocks and treatments, otherwise – MSResidual will increase proportionally more than MSA with reduced power of F-test for A (treatments) – interpretation of treatment effects may be difficult, just like replicated factorial ANOVA Checks for interaction • No real test because no within-cell variation measured • Tukey’s test for non-additivity: – detect some forms of interaction • Plot treatment values against block (“interaction plot”) Interaction plots DV No interaction DV Interaction Block Repeated measures designs • A common experimental design in biology (and psychology) • Different treatments applied to whole experimental units (called “subjects”) or • Experimental units recorded through time Repeated measures designs • The effect of four experimental drugs on heart rate of rats: – five rats used – each rat receives all four drugs in random order • Time as treatment factor is most common use of repeated measures designs in biology Driscoll & Roberts (1997) • Effect of fuel-reduction burning on frogs • Six drainages: – blocks or subjects • Three treatments (times): – pre-burn, post-burn 1, post-burn 2 • DV: – difference between no. calling males on paired burnt-unburnt sites at each drainage Repeated measures cf. randomised block • Simple repeated measures designs are analysed as unreplicated two factor ANOVAs • Like randomised block designs – experimental units or “subjects” are blocks – treatments comprise factor A Randomised block Source Treatments Blocks Residual Total Repeated measures Source Between “subjects” Within subjects Treatments Residual Total df p-1 q-1 (p-1)(q-1) pq-1 df q-1 p-1 (p-1)(q-1) pq-1 Driscoll & Roberts (1997) Source df MS Between drainages 5 1046.28 12 2 10 443.33 246.78 196.56 Within drainages Years Residual F 6.28 P 0.017 Computer set-up - randomised block Treatment 1 2 3 1 2 etc. Block 1 1 1 2 2 DV y11 y21 y31 y12 y22 Computer set-up - repeated measures Subject Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 1 y11 y21 y31 2 y12 y22 y32 3 y13 y23 y33 etc. Both analyses produce identical results Sphericity assumption Block Treat 1 Treat 2 1 2 3 etc. y11 y12 y13 y21 y22 y23 Treat 3 etc. y31 y32 y33 Block T1 - T2 T2 - T3 T1 - T3 etc. 1 2 3 etc. y11-y21 y12-y22 y13-y23 y21-y31 y22-y32 y23-y33 y11-y31 y12-y32 y13-y33 Sphericity assumption • Pattern of variances and covariances within and between “times”: – sphericity of variance-covariance matrix • Variances of differences between all pairs of treatments are equal: – variance of (T1 - T2)’s = variance of (T2 T3)’s = variance of (T1 - T3)’s etc. • If assumption not met: – F-test produces too many Type I errors Sphericity assumption • Applies to randomised block and repeated measures designs • Epsilon (e) statistic indicates degree to which sphericity is not met – further e is from 1, more variances of treatment differences are different • Two versions of e – Greenhouse-Geisser e – Huyhn-Feldt e Dealing with non-sphericity If e not close to 1 and sphericity not met, there are 2 approaches: – Adjusted ANOVA F-tests • df for F-tests from ANOVA adjusted downwards (made more conservative) depending on value e – Multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) • treatments considered as multiple DVs in MANOVA Sphericity assumption • Assumption of sphericity probably OK for randomised block designs: – treatments randomly applied to experimental units within blocks • Assumption of sphericity probably also OK for repeated measures designs: – if order each “subject” receives each treatment is randomised (eg. rats and drugs) Sphericity assumption • Assumption of sphericity probably not OK for repeated measures designs involving time: – because DV for times closer together more correlated than for times further apart – sphericity unlikely to be met – use Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted tests or MANOVA Examples from literature Poorter et al. (1990) • Growth of five genotypes (3 fast and 2 slow) of Plantago major (a dicot plant called ribwort) • One replicate seedling of each genotype was placed in each of 7 plastic containers in growth chamber • Genotypes (1, 2, 3, 4, 5) are treatments, containers are blocks, DV is total plant weight (g) after 12 days Poorter et al. (1990) 3 4 1 2 5 Container 1 1 2 5 4 Container 2 Similarly for containers 3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 3 Source Genotype Block Residual Total df 4 6 24 34 MS F 0.125 0.118 0.033 3.81 P 0.016 Conclusions: • Large variation between containers (= blocks) so block design probably better than completely randomised design • Significant difference in growth between genotypes Robles et al. (1995) • Effect of increased mussel (Mytilus spp.) recruitment on seastar numbers • Two treatments: 30-40L of Mytilus (0.53.5cm long) added, no Mytilus added • Four matched pairs of mussel beds chosen, each pair = block • Treatments randomly assigned to mussel beds within a pair • DV is % change in seastar numbers + + + - - - + 1 block (pair of mussel beds) + - mussel bed with added mussels mussel bed without added mussels Source df Blocks Treatment Residual 3 1 3 MS 62.82 5237.21 115.09 F 45.50 P 0.007 Conclusions: • Relatively little variation between blocks so a completely randomised design probably better because treatments would have 1,6 df • Significant treatment effect - more seastars where mussels added