Stimulus Control of Behaviour Stimulus Control • Differential responding and stimulus discrimination • Complex environment • “Signal” from the “noise” • What is important? Reynolds (1961) Train Test Responses per minute • Pigeons, operant chamber, compound key Bird 107 triangle Bird 105 circle Discrimination and Generalization • Differential responding – Stimulus discrimination • Similar responding – Stimulus generalization Generalization Gradients • Train on one (or more) stimulus • Test on continuum (i.e., a “gradient”) of other, related stimuli • Plot on graph… Generalization Gradient • Tricky – Reinforcement problem – Extinction problem – Probe trials and extinction blocks Responses Gen. Grad.: Stimulus Control Training stimulus Stimulus Continuum (e.g., wavelength of light) Responses Gen. Grad: No Stimulus Control Training stimulus Stimulus Continuum (e.g., wavelength of light) Spetch & Mondloch (1993) • • • • • Pigeons in localization task Touch-screen Peck at target area of 2cm2 Target defined by landmarks Tested to determine the landmarks that controlled the discrimination Training Stimuli Quic kTime™ and a TIFF (LZ W) dec ompress or are needed to see this picture. Results: Pigeon C207 Results: Pigeon 271 Factors in Stimulus Control Sensory Capacity • Organism’s sensory capacity determines what stimuli can control behaviour • E.g., ultraviolet stimuli for honeybee, but not for human • Physical orientation Overshadowing • Competition among stimuli for access to learning processes • Higher intensity stimulus more easily conditioned • Speed of conditioning However… • Stimulus element approach – Elements in a compound stimulus treated as distinct and separate components – Overshadowing: most salient stimulus gains control • Configural-cue approach – Compound stimulus treated as an integral whole – Overshadowing: generaliztion decrement Group Training stimuli Test stimulus Generalization from training to test Overshadowing weak-STRONG weak Decrement Control weak weak No decrement • Configural-cue interpretation – Overshadowing due to degrees of generalization decrement from training to testing – No generalization decrement for control group – Overshadowing group learned weak-STRONG, but only tested on weak, so considerable generalization decrement • Evidence for both stimulus element and configural-cue approaches Type of Reinforcement • Biological predispositions; belongingness • Species • Sometimes seasonal – E.g., stimuli associated with courtship disregarded outside of mating season Force & LoLordo (1973) Tone & light Light Tone – L/T … press pedal … food • Group 2 – L/T … press pedal … avoid shock • Test compound and individual components Pedal Presses • Training: • Group 1 Group1 (food reinforcer) Group 2 (shock avoidance) Instrumental Response Factors • Nature of the response required for reinforcement can affect stimulus control Dobrzecka, Szwejkowska & Konorski (1966) Learning Factors in Stimulus Control • Pavlov – Inherent properties of nervous sytem – Innate – Due to physical similarity of stimuli • Lashley & Wade (1946) – Explicit discrimination training required – Stimulus control learned – Stimulus generalization because animals have NOT learned the difference between stimuli Stimulus Discrimination Training • In classical or instrumental conditioning – CS+ or CS-, S+ or S- Cummulative responses • Stimuli explicitly associated with other stimuli or outcomes S+ S- Time Training and Stimulus Control • Use generalization gradient to determine degree of stimulus control • Need to determine the feature(s) of the discrimination procedure that controls the gradient Jenkins & Harrison (1962) Range of Discriminative Stimuli • Wide range of stimuli have been used in discrimination studies – Music, auditory frequencies, painting styles, geometric shapes, etc., etc. • Can use these discrimination studies to assess the sensory capability of species Pigeon Art Appreciation • Watanabe et al. (1995) • Trained pigeons to discriminate between Monet and Picasso • Achieved high degree of accuracy • Pigeons generalized to other artists of the same style (Impressionist or Cubist) What’s Learned • Spence’s (1936) theory • Learn about both S+ and S– Stimuli treated separately • S+ represents excitation • S- represents inhibition • Test – Excitatory stimulus generalization gradient • Most response near S+ – Inhibitory stimulus generalization gradient • Least response near S- Honig et al. (1963) S+/S- Interactions • Interactions likely to occur between S+ and S• Especially likely if using intradimensional discrimination – Stimuli from same stimulus continuum • Example seen in peak shift Peak Shift • Maximum responding on generalization gradient not to trained stimulus • With S+/S- training • Already seen in Jenkins & Harrison (1962) Peak shift Hanson (1959 Spence’s Interpretation • Opponent process system • S+ generates internal excitatory gradient • S- generates internal inhibitory gradient • Actual generalization gradient (i.e., measured behavioural response) due to net sum of excitatory and inhibitory gradients Actual generalization gradient (i.e., net sum) excitatory inhibitory e.g., responses Hypothetical internal gradients S- S- S+ S- S- Stimulus continuum S- Absolute/Relative Control • Absolute interpretations – Learn specifics of individual stimuli – Spence’s theory is an absolute theory • Relational interpretatsions – Learn relationship between stimuli Transposition Task • Kohler (1939) • Can a relational rule be transferred to a new stimuli set? Training S+ S- Testing transfer (relational) absolute Intermediate Size Problem • Gonzales, Gentry & Bitterman (1954) • Chimpanzees, visual display of squares • Chimps choose intermediate size on transfer test Training 1 4 S+ Transfer Test 9 4 7 9 Training Technique • Simultaneous discrimination training – S+ and S- presented together • Successive discrimination training – S+ and S- presented alone on different trials • Relative stimulus control with simultaneous • Absolute stimulus control with successive Stimulus Equivalence Training • Train subjects to treat dissimilar stimuli as similar • Training to generalize, not discriminate • Categorization learning Herrnstein, Loveland & Cable (1976) • • • • • Pigeons Presence/absence pictures S+: item present S-: item absent Various S+ stimuli: water, trees, people Stimuli for Water as S+ Condition S- S+ Results Trees Water People Contextual Cues • Context cues can exert stimulus control • Perform behaviours appropriate to a given context Siegel (1975) • Morphine tolerance • Home room and injection room • Same amount of morphine across 4 days • Conditional compensatory response to context cues of injection room Perkins & Weyant (1958) • Two groups of rats run through two mazes, one white, one black; same maze layout • Half of each group tested in same colour maze, half in opposite colour maze • Poor performance for rats tested in opposite compared to same Kamin (1957) State-dependent learning Rats; avoidance learning Test at various retention intervals Rats’ own internal physiology serves as context cue Avoidance (%) • • • • 100 50 0 12 24 36 48 60 72 Retention Interval (hr) 84 Akins (1985) • Male quail sexual conditioning • Arena with two compartments – Sand floor, orange walls – Wire-mesh floor and walls, green ceiling • Individual subjects allowed to move back and forth in baseline – Less preferred compartment made CS+ • Conditioning – Experimental group: CS+ paired with sexually receptive female (US) – Control group: US only in home cage, never in CS+ compartment % time spent in CS+ compartment Results Experimental Control 1 2 Preference Tests 3 Conditional Relations • Binary relations: between two events (CS-US, operant-outcome) • Modulator: a third event that determines the nature of a binary relation • Modulator signals a conditional relation Modulators • Instrumental – S+: respond --> reinforcer – S-: respond --> no reinforcer • Classical – “Facilitators” or “occasion setters”, not excitatory or inhibitory conditional stimuli – CS = noise, US = food, modulator = light light light noise noise food food