Improving Executive Functions Through Real-World Interventions: The Role of Social Media Betty Glisky Department of Psychology University of Arizona Background Laboratory training on cognitive tasks is often effective but highly specific: Improves performance on trained tasks and other highly similar tasks, but Does not transfer to other cognitive domains Generally little transfer to everyday life Carrying out interventions in the real-world environment rather than in the laboratory might be more effective Rationale The rationale for our study came from 2 sources: Intervention studies in which “social interaction” provided cognitive benefits equivalent to or greater than cognitive training or aerobic exercise (e.g., Park et al., 2013; Mortimer et al., 2012) Longitudinal studies suggesting that An active lifestyle may reduce age-related cognitive decline (Fratiglioni et al., 2000), but also Declining cognitive function may lead to social disengagement (e.g., Seeman et al., 2011; Small et al, 2012). We decided to see if connecting socially-isolated older adults through social media would improve their cognitive function Method Three Groups of Older Adults Average age = 79; lived alone Pretest-posttest Design Group 1 (N = 13) Group 2 (N = 13) Group 3 Penzu Waitlist Tests of working memory/ executive function, memory, and processing speed Given before and after the Facebook/Penzu intervention Interventions 6 hours of training over a week 7 weeks of daily postings in Facebook or Penzu (N = 14) Myrhe & Glisky, 2013 Procedure Pretests 2 weeks Training 1 week Home-Use Posttests 7 weeks 2 weeks Intervention What we’re looking for is differential changes in performance on the cognitive tests from Time 1 (pretests) to Time 2 (post-tests) 8-weeks later as a function of the intervention Executive Function Tests Adapted from Miyake et al, 2000 Shifting Updating/Working Memory Number-Letter Task Global-Local Task Consonant Updating Keep Track Task Inhibition Stroop Task Simon Task Updating Tasks Consonant Updating Participants were shown a serial list of letters and were required to continually recall out loud only the last four letters presented. The number of letters presented during a trial varied from 5 to 11. Updating Tasks Keep Track Participants are shown a list of 15 words from different categories and were asked to keep track of the last word from one or more specified categories. There were three trials of 1, 2, 3, and 4 categories. Shifting Tasks Letter-Number Local-Global Inhibition Tasks Simon Task Stroop Task Name the Ink Color XXXXX XXXXX XXXXX GREEN BLUE RED Results Changes in Updating Significant Group x Time interaction, F(2,37) = 5.95, p = .006 The Facebook group showed a significant increase in performance compared to no significant change in the other two groups. Global Shifting Costs Significant Group x Time interaction, F(2,33) = 4.01, p = .028. Only the Waitlist group showed a significant change from Time 1 to Time 2 Processing Speed Trails A Trails B Significant Time x Group interaction, F(2,38) = 3.40, p = .044, for Trails B; similar trend for Trails A performance, F(2,38) = 2.52, p = .094. No differential slowing for Trails B across groups Summary We found a specific cognitive benefit associated with using Facebook Only the Facebook group showed improvements in updating/working memory There were no changes in the other measures of executive function or in memory Both intervention groups showed increases in processing speed What accounts for the improvements in the Facebook group relative to the Penzu group? Increased social interaction in Facebook group relative to Penzu Facebook may be more cognitively challenging than Penzu or may place greater demands on working memory or the updating component of executive function. Social interactions in general may involve working memory and executive control and may present real-world opportunities for maintaining cognitive function Specific Advantages of Online Social Networking People who are truly socially isolated can stay connected, reducing the likelihood of cognitive decline People who are experiencing cognitive decline can continue social interactions at their own pace People who may be experiencing declines in vision or hearing can make adjustments so that they can remain socially connected Specific Advantages of Online Social Networking People can be selective in their social interactions, maintaining those that are positive and discarding those that are negative Some evidence suggests that negative social interactions increase stress and have negative effects on cognitive function (e.g., Tun etal., 2013) Ultimately, older people can age in place for a longer period of time, remaining socially connected and cognitively challenged Conclusions Social interaction is likely to be beneficial and may be a very acceptable way to reduce cognitive decline, whether it occurs in face-toface interactions or through online social media Training programs that focus on realworld functional tasks that require executive control and working memory may be more effective than laboratory tasks, and they don’t require transfer to make a difference. Training in the use of new technologies for solving everyday problems may be a meaningful way to keep people engaged and cognitively healthy. Ivy Bean, 104 years old, has 4,000 friends on Facebook Thanks! Thanks to all of the older adults from the Tucson community and from La Posada in Green Valley who contributed their time to this study. Annual Conference on Successful Aging 2013 Thanks also to the Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Research Foundation for support.