HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1 PSYCHOLOGY 3050: The Development of Folk Knowledge (Ch 6) Dr. Jamie Drover SN-3094, 864-8383 e-mail -- jrdrover@mun.ca Fall Semester, 2012 Theory Theories • We have innate theories that we modify during childhood. • A theory is tested and then revised when it no longer explains new data. • We have innate knowledge, or processing constraints in certain domains. – Neonativism 2 Theory Theories • They integrate innate knowledge with constructivism. • The cognitive processes that undergo gradual constructivist development are actually innate. • They require specific motor and/or sensory skills. – Object permanence – Very young infants can show this ability when tasks are modified. 3 Theory Theories • Gopnik and Meltzoff propose that children are born with sets of rules for operating on particular representations. • These rules are altered by experience. • Development is still constructive. • The child has a particular theory. • There is disorganization. • A new coherent theory emerges. 4 Developing a Theory of Mind • Recognizing different categories of mind, such as dreams, memories, beliefs, etc. • Includes the ability to understand that other people have beliefs, desires and intentions that are different from one's own. • A theory to understand, predict, and explain behavior. Development of Theory of Mind • Adult theory of mind is based on belief-desire reasoning. • We explain and predict what people do based on what we understand their beliefs and desires to be. – Referring to wants, wishes, hopes, and goals, and to their ideas, opinions and knowledge. Skills Underlying Theory of Mind • Viewing oneself and others as intentional agents. • Individuals who cause things to happen and whose behavior is designed to achieve some goal. • The ability to take the perspective of another person, i.e., understand the intentions of others. Skills Underlying Theory of Mind • This knowledge develops over the first few years of life, beginning with shared attention. • A triadic interaction between two partners and a third object. – Appears at about 9 months. – At 12 months, they point to things that others aren’t aware of. – At 12 to 18 months, they use the gaze of others to achieve shared attention. Skills Underlying Theory of Mind • There is early evidence that infants and toddlers view others as intentional agents. • More likely to copy the behavior of a model who is engaged in the action on purpose. – See Carpenter et al (1998) p 202. – See Meltzoff (1995) p 202. The Development of Mind Reading • Researchers have developed several tasks to assess when and how children develop this latter aspect of theory of mind. • False-belief task: a child watches as a candy is hidden in a special location. • Another child is present when the candy is hidden. • The hidden candy is then relocated. The Development of Mind Reading • Child is asked whether the other child will know the location of the candy. • 4-year-olds can solve the problem. 3-year-olds can not. • A variation of this is the Smarties task. • Suggests that children are unable to remember their original belief. The Development of Mind Reading • Young children have difficulty with contradictory evidence. • Can’t deal with two representations of a single object simultaneously. – Similar to the dual-representation hypothesis. – Young children will fail in situations where they must consider two beliefs or representations for a single target. The Development of Mind Reading • Gopnik and Astington assessed 3, 4, and 5 year-old’s abilities to solve tasks that require children to deal with contradictory evidence (See Fig 6-2 p 204). – Performance varied with age. – A domain-general mechanism underlies representational abilities. The Development of Mind Reading • Young children lack executive function. – Cognitive abilities involved in planning, executing, and inhibiting actions. – In theory of mind tasks, children often have to inhibit a dominant response to pass the task. • See the mean monkey example (p. 205). • Note however, 3-year-olds can solve other tasks that require an understanding of other’s minds. The Development of Mind Reading • See p. 206 (Clemens & Perner, 1994) – Implicitly, children were correct. – 3-year-olds have an implicit understanding of false belief that exceeds their verbal understanding. • In some studies, 18-month-olds show the beginnings of theory of mind. – See page 207 (Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997). The Development of Mind Reading • Performance on false beliefs task are related to family size. – Children from larger families perform better. – This only helps if one has older siblings. – Important for children with low language skills. Deception • When children play tricks on others, they seem to be aware of what the other person does and does not know. • 2- and 3-year-olds are often capable of deception. • See p. 210 (Chandler et al. 1989) – 2.5 to 4 year-old children showed several types of deception. – Withhold evidence, destroy evidence, lying, producing false evidence. Hiding Containers Sponge Footprints Main Container Deception • Even 2.5-3 year-olds will engage in deceptive practices to instill false beliefs in others. • See page 255 (Hala & Chandler, 1996; Sullivan & Winner, 1993). Do 3-Year-Olds Have a Theory of Mind • Currently there is debate whether 3-year-olds possess theory of mind. • Most researchers agree that 3-year-olds appear to have a limited knowledge of other’s minds. • Some argue that 3-year-olds have competence, but the nature of the task prevents them from showing it. • Others argue that there is real conceptual change during the preschool years. Do 3-Year-Olds Have a Theory of Mind • Wellman et al. (2001) conducted a metaanalysis of 178 studies. • They found age effects which support the conceptual change position. Theory of Mind, Evolved Modules, and Autism • Some theorists believe that Theory of Mind evolved during the course of human evolution and is the basis of social intelligence. • It’s assumed that we have domain-specific modules to handle theory of mind. • According to Baron-Cohen (1995), there are four interacting modules involved in mindreading that develop in the first 4 years. Theory of Mind, Evolved Modules, and Autism 1. Intentionality Detector (ID): interprets moving objects as having some intention 2. Eye Direction Detector (EDD): Detects the presence of eyes. Determines when another organism is looking at them. Develops between birth and 9 months. 3. Shared Attention Mechanism: Involves three-way interactions. Develops from 9-18 months. Theory of Mind, Evolved Modules, and Autism 4. Theory of Mind Module: Understanding that others have different beliefs, desires, and intentions (develops between 18 and 48 months). The Empathizing System • Baron-Cohen proposed The Emotion Detector (TED) which develops by 9 months of age. • It can represent affective or emotional states between two people. • Within 6 months of life, infants can pick up on the emotions of others. • At 9 months, the info derived from TED can be converted into a triadic representation of the SAM. The Empathizing System • The Empathizing SyStem (TESS) is online at 14 months of age. • It permits an empathic reaction to another person’s emotions, and assumes there is an associated drive to help others. Mindblindness • The inability to read minds. • Advanced forms of mindreading and empathizing are absent or delayed in children with autism. • Autistic children fail false-belief and theory of mind tasks but can pass nonsocial tasks. • They perform well on tasks requiring the ID and EDD modules, but not those requiring SAM or Theory of mind. Mindblindness • Children with Down Syndrome perform well on theory-of-mind tasks, but perform poorly on tasks of intelligence. The Development of Spatial Cognition • Processing information with respect to their spatial relations. • Coding information about the environment. • Knowing where something is in relation to you, or in relation to other objects and locations. • See Table 6.4 Spatial Orientation • How people understand the placement of objects in space with themselves as a reference point. • Eg. Distinguishing geographic directions in an unfamiliar locale, drawing your way from point A to B on a map. • Relatively well-developed in preschool. – See Huttenlocher & Vasilyeva (2003) on p 227 – See Newcombe et al. (1999) on p 227 Spatial Orientation • Older children are even more advanced…can form cognitive maps. – i.e., 5, 7, and 10-year-olds could re-create the layout of a large model town based on memory (Herman & Siegel, 1978). – Got better with age. • They get better at using real maps over the school years. – Adultlike by age 10 Spatial Orientation • Uttal et al. (2001) showed 3, 4, and 5-year-olds yellow carpet with 27 cups on it. • They had to find stickers under some cups using a map. • The maps included lines for half the kids, no lines for the other half. • Children got better with age. • Lines improved the performance for 5-year-olds. Spatial Orientation • Maps are a tool of intellectual adaptation that can eventually be internalized. • There is a reciprocal relationship between maps and spatial cognition. • Uttal & Wellman (1989) had 4 to 7-year-old children learn which of 6 animals went into each of 6 rooms in a life-sized playhouse. – Children learned the locations based on a map or on flash cards. Spatial Orientation • The children who learned with the map performed better on a “walk-through.” • The experience with the map allowed them to more easily see spatial relationships among elements. Spatial Visualization • Involves visual/mental operations. • Often assessed using mental rotation. – A stimulus must be rotated to see if it matches another. • Even 4 and 5-year-olds can do this. • Adults can have problems with complex stimuli. Spatial Visualization Spatial Visualization • Piaget and Inhelder (1967) tested spatial visualization using the waterlevel problem. • Most children can do this by age 7. Object and Location Memory • Remembering objects and their positions. • Often tested using card games. • Young children (5 yrs) perform almost as well as adults early in these games. – Strategies are less important. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • There are sex differences in spatial cognition that may have been selected for through the course of evolution. • Males needed to develop spatial abilities in order to navigate (Geary, 2007). • Sex differences in map reading and mental rotation have been found in preschool years. – Mental rotation differences may exist in infancy. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • According to meta-analyses, the magnitude of sex differences is very small. – Only 1 to 5% of the difference is due to gender. – Mental rotation is especially prominent. • Females show better performance in object and location memory. • May be due to evolution…role as gatherers. – Must be able to perceive small stimulus differences. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • Differential experiences may also play a role. • Newcombe et al. (1983) asked college students to classify activities as masculine, feminine, or neutral (see Table 6-5). • Tasks with high spatial content were considered masculine. • They found a gender difference on a test with strong spatial components. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • The more spatial activities one engages in, the greater one’s spatial ability. Object-Oriented Play and Tool Use • Object-Oriented Play: manipulation of objects (banging or throwing), including the use of objects to build something. • Difficult to distinguish from exploration. • Often involves making noise. Object-Oriented Play and Tool Use • Play eventually becomes more sophisticated. – Building things --- boys more likely. – May be a biological origin. Object-Oriented Play and Tool Use • Preschoolers spend 10 to 15% of their time engaged in object play. • This object play allows children to learn how objects can be used as tools. – This is something very few species can do. Learning to use Tools • Common in problem solving: forks, pens, hammers, etc. • Non-humans also use tools to solve problems Learning to use Tools • May originate from infants’ manipulation of their physical world (Lockman, 2000) • It’s a gradual process of discovery that arises from infants’ and children’s interaction with objects in the real world to achieve a perceptual outcome. The Development of Tool Use in Young Children • Piaget speculated that infants could use tools by their first birthday. • 9- to 12 mo-old infants learn to solve lure-retrieval problems – E.g., can use a stick to reach toy outside playpen • Chen and Siegler (2000): systematic study of tool use in toddlers: 18- to 30-mo – Child seated across from out-of-reach toys – Several tools available – Only one will retrieve the toy – Three trials – encouraged to get toy – Hint and modeling conditions given The Development of Tool Use in Young Children The Development of Tool Use in Young Children The Development of Tool Use in Young Children • 30 mo – no instruction: – more likely to use tools to get toy than 18-mo-olds without instruction (15% vs. 0%) – With instructions (hint) or modeling (social learning) • 30-mo improved; some 18-mo use tool to get toy • Type of strategies used – Forward: lean forward to get toy – Indirect: ask for help – Tool use: use one of the tools • Strategy use changed across trials – Tool strategy increased but forward and indirect continued to be used. The Development of Tool Use in Young Children • Boys were more likely than girls to choose a tool use strategy. The Design Stance • Once a person sees someone use a tool achieve a goal, he/she assumes the tool was designed for that purpose. – Leads to efficiency and functional fixedness. • Even 12-month-olds learn that some tools are special purpose objects. – See Barrett et al (2007) on page 238. The Design Stance • Young children select a tool based on past history, not based on its properties to solve a problem. The Relationship between Tool Use and ObjectOriented Play • The tendency to play with objects can influence tool use. • In some studies, children must select a properly shaped tool to retrieve a toy. • Those who are given the opportunity to play with the objects beforehand are more likely to use them later as tools. The Relationship between Tool Use and ObjectOriented Play • This can lead to gender differences. Children’s Understanding of Time • 2 and 3-year-olds can use temporal terms (yesterday, last night, tomorrow), but often use them incorrectly. • By age 4, they understand before and after, but have trouble understanding the recency of events. • 4 and 5-year-olds can distinguish between the events that happen during the course of the day, but have trouble with events occurring at longer intervals. Children’s Understanding of Time • Even 6 and 7-year-olds have trouble thinking about days of the week, or months in relation to each other.