The ERP Boot Camp Baselines, Difference Waves, and the MONSTER Paradigm All slides © S. J. Luck, except as indicated in the notes sections of individual slides Slides may be used for nonprofit educational purposes if this copyright notice is included, except as noted Permission must be obtained from the copyright holder(s) for any other use Segmenting & Baselining • Prior to averaging, we must extract segments (epochs) of the EEG surrounding the relevant events Voltage Time • Baseline correction is usually performed at this point - This is important for some types of artifact rejection procedures (e.g., absolute voltage thresholds) - For most purposes, baseline correction can be performed at any time Reason 1: DC Offset 115 µV Reason 2: Baseline Drift How to Correct Baseline Goal: Subtract estimate of DC offset from the waveform Mean prestimulus voltage is usually a reasonable estimate Subtract this value from each point in the waveform 2 cond1 1.5 cond1-baseline 1 0.5 0 -200 0 200 400 600 800 -0.5 -1 Note: Anything that messes up the baseline (e.g., noise, overlap) will be propagated to your amplitude measurements Baseline Distortion Example 1 2 cond1+noise cond1+noise-baseline 1.5 1 0.5 0 -200 0 200 400 600 800 -0.5 -1 Entire waveform shifted down (negative) because of positive noise blip Baseline Distortion Example 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 -200 0 200 400 600 800 -0.5 cond1 -1 cond1+overlap -1.5 cond1+overlap-baseline How to Correct Baseline • • • What to use for response-locked averages? - A period that is equivalent across conditions - Often, only the prestimulus period is guaranteed to be equivalent Simple option- Average using a long pre-response interval and use a time range that is prior to the stimulus for every response Complex option- Use the prestimulus period for each individual trial Difference Waves w1,1 C1 w2,1 E1 w3,1 w2,2 C2 w1,2 E2 w3,2 w1,3 w2,3 C3 w3,3 E3 E1 E2 If a single component varies across conditions, it can be isolated by means of a difference wave C3 E3 C1 C2 For this to work, the conditions must be so similar that only one component varies across conditions Example: N170 This voltage reflects face-related activity plus everything else that is active at 170 ms Rossion & Jacques (2009) Example: N170 This difference reflects only brain activity that differentiates between faces and cars Rossion & Jacques (2009) N170 and Development Faces Scrambled Faces Cars Scrambled Cars Kuefner et al (2010, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience) It looks like the N170 scalp distribution changes over development But this could be due to other components in this time range Kuefner et al (2010, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience) Subtracting scrambled faces removes nonspecific activity The face-specific activity has the same distribution over development Kuefner et al (2010, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience) Shortcomings of Difference Waves • May not isolate a single component - Example: Rare-Frequent yields P2, N2, P3 • If Cond1–Cond2 difference wave is reduced, cannot distinguish between smaller Cond1 and larger Cond2 - For most components, need to look at original waveforms as well as difference waves (not for N2pc/LRP) - Ask Emily how we solved this problem for LRP • Changes in latency between Cond1 and Cond2 produce an apparent difference in amplitude - Not a problem if you view difference wave as simply meaning a difference in the time course of amplitude The MONSTER Approach • • • • Manipulation of Orthogonal Neural Systems Together in Electrophysiological Recordings General approach in which multiple components are simultaneously isolated with high efficiency by means of a factorial design and difference waves Each factor used to create a difference wave that isolates a specific component Our version of MONSTER isolates 4 components - C1- Sensory processing in primary visual cortex N2pc- Shift of visual attention P3- Task-governed stimulus categorization Lateralized Readiness Potential (LRP)- Response activation in primary motor cortex Isolating the C1 Wave • • • • • Onset 40-60 ms; peak ca. 80-100 ms Thought to arise from area V1 Negative for upper-field stimuli; positive for lower-field stimuli When positive, merges together with P1 Largest at midline occipital-parietal sites C1 Wave Elicited by Stimulus at Location Y +1mV -1mV C1 Wave Elicited by Stimulus at Location X 100 ms 200 ms Mangun, Hillyard, & Luck (1993) Isolating the C1 Wave C1 Wave Elicited by Stimulus at Location Y +1mV -1mV C1 Wave Elicited by Stimulus at Location X 100 ms 200 ms Mangun, Hillyard, & Luck (1993) Isolating the C1 Wave Isolating the LRP Left Hemisphere (Ipsi) Right Hemisphere (Contra) Right Hemisphere (Ipsi) Left Hemisphere (Contra) LRP = Contra minus ipsi, averaged over left & right hands Smulders & Miller (2010) Isolating the LRP LRP = Contra minus ipsi, averaged over left & right hands Smulders & Miller (2010) Isolating the LRP Left Hemisphere (Ipsi) Right Hemisphere (Contra) Right Hemisphere (Ipsi) Left Hemisphere (Contra) Nonlateralized components eliminated by subtraction Smulders & Miller (2010) Isolating the LRP Left Hemisphere (Ipsi) Right Hemisphere (Contra) Right Hemisphere (Ipsi) Left Hemisphere (Contra) Overall hemisphere differences eliminated by subtraction (RHem more positive than LHem for both hands) Isolating the LRP Left Hemisphere (Ipsi) Right Hemisphere (Contra) Right Hemisphere (Ipsi) Left Hemisphere (Contra) Overall hand differences eliminated by subtraction (RHand more positive than LHand for both hemispheres) Smulders & Miller (2010) Isolating the LRP LRP provides a pure measure of the relative activation of correct vs incorrect response at each moment in time Smulders & Miller (2010) Isolating the N2pc Component Isolating the P3 Wave • P3 amplitude depends on the probability of a task-defined stimulus • • • category, not the probability of a physical stimulus P3 probability effect cannot begin until after categorization occurs Rare-minus-frequent difference wave isolates processes that occur after perception and categorization P3 latency indexes “stimulus evaluation time” P3 Difference Waves The MONSTER Approach • Manipulation of Orthogonal Neural Systems Together in • Electrophysiological Recordings Goal- Efficiently measure the speed and integrity of a broad set of specific neurocognitive processes - First step in determining the nature of deficits in a given patient group - Possibly useful for diagnosis of disease subtypes (e.g., in multiple sclerosis) • Use orthogonal subtractions to isolate specific ERP components that reflect well-characterized neurocognitive processes - Same data divided different ways to isolate different components Stimuli and Task Task: Attend to black or white Press left for A (p = .80 or p = .20) Press right for B (p = .20 or p = .80) Left/Right and .80/.20 are counterbalanced Stim: Duration = 200 ms SOA = 1500 ± 150 ms 2 blocks of 512 stimuli Stimuli and Task C1: Upper Checks minus Lower Checks N2pc: Contra target minus Ipsi target P3: Rare minus Frequent LRP: Contra to response hand minus Ipsi to response hand These subtractions are orthogonal -- same 1024 trials divided into different pairs of subsets and then subtracted Electrode Sites LRP P3 N2pc C1