ABSTRACT ACADEMIC-RELATED FACTORS AND EMOTIONAL EATING IN ADOLESCENTS By Ashley Chamberlin August 2015 Emotional eating is increasing food consumption in response to negative emotions such as stress or anger, rather than physiological hunger. Many studies have found stress to be related to emotional eating and weight gain in adults, yet few researchers have examined the impact of school-related stress on emotional eating in adolescents. School can be a large burden of anxiety which may lead to emotional eating as a method of coping. The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem (2) grade point average (GPA) and (3) academic worries were related to emotional eating. The results showed that GPA, academic self-esteem, and academic worries significantly predicted emotional eating in adolescents. Positive, significant relationships were found for academic worries and GPA. The relationship between academic selfesteem was negative and significant. Age and gender were not found to be statistically significant predictors. There were no significant differences in academic factors between 1 emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. More research is needed to determine the complex interaction between emotional eating behavior and academics. 2 ACADEMIC-RELATED FACTORS AND EMOTIONAL EATING IN ADOLESCENTS A THESIS Presented to the Department of Family and Consumer Sciences California State University, Long Beach In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in Nutritional Sciences Committee Members: Virginia Gray, Ph. D. (Chair) Selena Nguyen-Rodriguez, Ph. D. Wendy Reiboldt, Ph. D. College Designee: Wendy Reiboldt, Ph. D. By Ashley A. Chamberlin B.S., 2012, California State University, San Bernardino August 2015 UMI Number: 1594910 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. UMI 1594910 Published by ProQuest LLC (2015). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Thank you to my committee members, Virginia Gray, Ph. D., Selena NguyenRodriguez, Ph. D., and Wendy Reiboldt, Ph. D., for their tremendous support and guidance throughout the thesis process. For help with my statistical analyses, thank you to Selena Nguyen-Rodriguez, Ph. D. I would also like to thank Donna Spruijt-Metz, Ph. D., M.F.A for kindly granting permission to use previously collected data for this study. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ......................................................................................... iii LIST OF TABLES ....................................................................................................... vi CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION .................................................................................................................... 1 Introduction ................................................................................................ Statement of the Problem ........................................................................... Purpose of the Study .................................................................................. Hypotheses ................................................................................................. Definition of Terms .................................................................................... Assumptions............................................................................................... 1 3 3 4 4 5 2. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ............................................................................................................... 6 Background ................................................................................................ Worries in Youth........................................................................................ Impact of Stress on Dietary Intake in Youth .............................................. Complexity of Emotional Eating.............................................................................................. Effect of Academic Stress on Eating Behaviors in Students...................... Summary .................................................................................................... 6 7 8 9 10 11 3. METHODOLOGY............................................................................................ 12 Sample........................................................................................................ School Selection ......................................................................................... Student Recruitment ................................................................................... Procedure ................................................................................................... Instrumentation .......................................................................................... Data Analysis ............................................................................................. 12 12 13 13 14 15 iv CHAPTER Page 4. RESULTS ........................................................................................... 17 Characteristics of Sample .......................................................................... Differences in Academic Factors and Emotional Eating between Subjects .......................................................................................... Academic Factors as Predictors of Emotional Eating................................ Summary .................................................................................................... 17 5. DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS ................. 22 Discussion of Findings ............................................................................... Limitations ................................................................................................. Conclusions ................................................................................................ Recommendations for Future Research ..................................................... Summary .................................................................................................... 22 25 26 26 27 APPENDICES 17 19 21 ........................................................................................................ 29 A. SURVEY ITEMS ............................................................................................. 30 B. LETTERS OF APPROVAL............................................................................. 35 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................ 38 v LIST OF TABLES TABLE Page 1. Characteristics of the Sample............................................................................ 18 2. Differences in Academic Factors between Emotional and Non-Emotional Eaters.......................................................................................................... 19 3. Associations between Emotional Eating and Academic Factors ...................... 20 vi CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION AND STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Introduction Researchers have stated there needs to be a clearer understanding of the linkage between stress and obesity in youth; emotional eating may be a large, often overlooked part of the picture (Wilson & Sato, 2014). Emotional eating, comfort eating, and stressinduced eating are terms to describe the repeated response of increasing food consumption as a way to cope with unpleasant psychological emotions (Arnow, Kenardy, & Agras, 1995). Although emotional eating encompasses a variety of psychological feelings prompting intake, research in this area has largely focused on stress (Greeno & Wing, 1994). Whether experienced acutely or chronically, stress impacts the individual on a psychological and physiological level (Wilson & Sato, 2014). During times of stress eating behaviors have been found to vary; 30% of individuals decrease intake, but the majority increase food intake when they are stressed (Adam & Epel, 2007). Emotional eating in childhood and adolescence has been linked to higher incidence of developing binge eating, eating disorders, depressive symptoms, and problem behaviors, both internally and externally (Braet & van Stier, 1997; Braet et al., 2008). The longstanding research on emotional eating has been primarily focused on Caucasian, adult females, but it is likely that emotional eating occurs in people of all ages, genders, and ethnicities. A national survey found that 43% of Americans report overeating or 1 consuming unhealthy foods in response to stress (American Psychological Association, 2007). Research recently began focusing on larger culturally-diverse samples of children and adolescents; revealing that approximately 10-60% of youth emotionally eat (Braet et al., 2008; Nguyen-Rodriguez, Unger, & Spruijt-Metz, 2009). Many questions remain unanswered about stress-induced eating among adolescents. Nguyen-Rodriguez et al. (2009) found perceived stress and worries to be correlated with emotional eating in Latino adolescents, but the origin of stress and worries in adolescents was not determined. Hispanic and African American children have reported using food as a helpful coping mechanism for stress (Jenkins, Rew, & Sternglanz, 2005). Specific factors that support emotional eating in adolescence are not clearly understood yet, nor are the particular differences between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. School is a source of stress in adolescents’ lives (Wahab et al., 2013); anxiety stems from examinations, homework, deadlines, grades, and preparing for college. Thus, the stress and anxiety associated with school may be a trigger for emotional eating behavior in youth. In a recent study, female college students were found to increase food consumption when they felt stressed about school (Bennett, Greene, & Schwartz-Barcott, 2013). University students are often the focus of studies relating academic stress to emotional eating, yet these eating patterns may have been well-established as children or adolescents. Some studies have focused on high school students to examine the impact of academic stress on eating behavior. Michaud et al. (Laugesen, Dugas, & Bukowski, 2003) found that both genders changed eating habits in response to a stressful academic exam. Another study found Korean high school students with high academic stress ate 2 more at meals and had higher overall sugar intake than students with low academic stress (Kim, Yang, Kim, & Lim, 2013). It is probable that academic factors play a role in emotional eating of adolescents, and this is an area that needs to be more extensively investigated. Statement of the Problem The association between worries, stress, and emotional eating has been identified in adolescents (Braet et al., 2008; Nguyen-Rodriguez et al., 2009). Additionally, and perhaps most pressing, is that emotional eating is frequently established in early childhood or adolescence and remains stable over time (Ashcroft, Semmler, Carnell, van Jaarsveld, & Wardle, 2008; van Strien, van der Zwaluw, & Engels, 2010). Thus, eating in response to negative emotions is likely to persist throughout life and negatively impact one’s health. By developing a more robust understanding of the etiology of this eating behavior, multi-disciplinary approaches can be established to assist adolescents in developing healthier responses to stress. School is an often overlooked factor that can contribute stress, anxiety, and worry to students’ lives and has the potential to elicit emotional eating (Bennett et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2013; Michaud et al, 1990). Nevertheless, the impact of academic-related factors on stress and emotional eating has yet to be fully explored, especially in minority adolescents (Braet et al., 2008; NguyenRodriguez et al., 2009). Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both 3 males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem, (2) grade point average (GPA) and (3) academic worries were related to emotional eating. Hypotheses H01: There will be no significant difference in academic self-esteem status between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. H02: There will be no significant difference in grade point average between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. H03: There will be no significant difference in academic worries between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. H04: Academic self-esteem will not significantly predict emotional eating behavior in minority adolescents. H05: GPA will not significantly predict emotional eating behavior in minority adolescents. H06: Academic worries will not significantly predict emotional eating behavior in minority adolescents. Definition of Terms Academic self-esteem: An individual’s overall feelings of academic success and his or her satisfaction of their accomplishments (Skaalvik, 1990). Adolescence: A period of life from puberty to adulthood, beginning at roughly 11 years old; characterized by physiological, emotional, social, and neurological changes and maturity (J.E. Brown et al., 2011). 4 Emotional eating: Increasing food intake in response to negative emotions such as anger, fear, or anxiety (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; Spoor, Bekker, Van Strien, & van Heck, 2007), rather than physiological hunger. Stress: The negative response that results from stimuli, happenings, or conditions that adversely affect an individual’s welfare (De Vriendt, Moreno, & De Henauw, 2009; Gundersen,Mahatmya, Garasky, & Lohman, 2010). Grade point average (GPA): A measure of academic achievement calculated by dividing the total number of grade points received by the total number of credits or hours of course work taken (Grade point average, 2015). Worries: A chain of negative, intrusive inner thoughts that are relatively uncontrollable (Borkovec, Robinson, Pruzinsky, & 1983). Assumptions This study assumed that the study participants understood the survey questions and responded honestly. It also assumed that the academic factors measured will represent facets of academic-related stress in the population. 5 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF LITERATURE The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem, (2) GPA and (3) academic worries were related to emotional eating. Many studies have found stress to be related to emotional eating and weight gain in adults, yet few researchers have focused on the impact of school-related stress and emotional eating in adolescents (Epel, Lapidus, McEwen, & Brownell, 2001; Ozier et al., 2008; Rutters, Nieuwenhuizen, Lemmens, Born, & Westerterp-Plantenga, 2009). School can be a large burden of responsibility and anxiety which may lead to emotional eating in some students (Bennett et al., 2013; Wahab et al., 2013). Research has attempted to understand the complexity of emotional eating by examining factors related to individuals’ eating patterns and differences between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters Background Increasing food intake in response to negative emotions, otherwise known as emotional eating, has been established as a common, yet “inapt” response (Spoor et al., 2007). Although emotional eating can involve a variety of unfavorable emotions such as sadness and frustration, stress appears to play a significant role, resulting in a large body of literature focused on stress-induced eating. Adam and Epel (2007) found that 70% of 6 people increase food intake when stressed. The stress hormone cortisol elicits a biological response as it influences food cravings toward highly palatable, energy-dense foods laden with fat and sugar that help to alleviate psychological stress (Hou et al., 2013). Several other studies conducted in adults support the notion that stress significantly increases emotional eating behavior and weight gain (Epel et al., 2001; Ozier et al., 2008; Rutters et al., 2009). Ozier et al. (2008) found that adults who are most likely to eat in response to stress and emotions were 13 times more likely to be overweight or obese than adults who have low tendencies to eat in response to emotions. Although most research in this area has focused on adults, dietary habits may be established in childhood. Ashcroft et al. (2008) demonstrated this concept as they found emotional eating in early childhood to be a stable trait that continues over time. Therefore, if poor dietary habits such as repeated emotional overeating occur for long periods of time, they may contribute to weight gain and obesity. Because emotional eating appears to frequently manifest prior to adulthood, identifying perpetuating factors of emotional eating in youth can help identify methods of prevention. Thus, the study of emotional eating during formative years of adolescence is warranted. Worries in Youth Very few studies have examined the potential impact of worries on emotional eating in adolescents. A pioneer study by Belcher et al. (2011) found that worries about self-image, relationships, and school significantly predicted emotional eating in Latina female adolescents. This study also found that emotional eating significantly moderated 7 the relationship between baseline worries and body mass index (BMI) percentile at follow-up. Another study focused on worries in adolescents and found that 42% of students worried about school grades every day (S. Brown, Teufel, Birch, & Kancherla, 2006). Grades were the highest frequency of daily worry, followed by physical appearance and problems at home. Additionally, researchers asked students what they usually do when they are stressed or worried and gave the following options: talk to parents or friends, keep worries to self, take action towards cause of worries, or none of the above. Students who responded that they usually keep worries to themselves had higher rates of worrying about grades weekly. Although the survey did not include the choice of eating/snacking in response to stress, 16.5% of students answered “none of the above.” A portion of these responses may have included those who increase food intake. Additionally, students who answered that they keep worries to themselves (24.4%) may also engage in emotional eating, intentionally or unintentionally. Those who emotionally eat unintentionally may not be aware that eating is how they express or manifest stress. Impact of Stress on Dietary Intake in Youth Life stress has been shown to modify dietary intake in children and adolescents. Cartwright et al. (2003) investigated the relationship between stress and dietary patterns in a diverse population of children and found that increased stress was associated with elevated intake of high fat foods and more frequent snacking. In addition, they found high stress decreased frequency of healthy behaviors such as consuming fruits, vegetables, and daily breakfast. Another study conducted in China by Hou et al. (2013) found life stress and emotional symptoms were correlated with unhealthy eating patterns, 8 including emotional eating, in junior and high school students. This association between stress and maladaptive eating behaviors remained significant after adjusting for gender, age, BMI, parental education level, and family socioeconomic status. Nguyen-Rodriguez and colleagues (2009) discovered that perceived stress and emotional eating were significantly correlated in adolescents, independent of BMI. Researchers from the University of Texas in Austin found a significant correlation between perceived stress and unhealthy eating habits, such as using food to cope with stress, in school-age children and early adolescents. There were no differences in coping behaviors by school grade, but results varied by ethnicity. Hispanic and African American youth reported using food more often as a helpful tool to deal with stress than Caucasian children (Jenkins et al., 2005). These studies add to the growing body of evidence that dietary intake and emotional eating may increase during times of stress and worry beginning in adolescence. Complexity of Emotional Eating Despite the strong linkage between stress, worry, and emotional eating, these relationships are not necessarily linear nor unidimensional. One trial by Roemmich, Wright, and Epstein (2002) looked at the role dietary restraint has on stress-induced eating in children. Results found that children who usually self-limit food consumption (high dietary restraint) actually increased snacking under stress. Those with low dietary restraint had the opposite reaction to stress and decreased snack intake. Other studies have shown that the type of stressor experienced (i.e., physical threat vs. interpersonal stress) may impact whether or not a restrained eater will increase food consumption (Cattanach, Phil, & Malley, 1988; Tanofsky-Kraff, Wilfley, & Spurrell, 2000). These 9 studies portray that despite the inherent physiological effects of stress on the body, the impact on dietary intake is both multifactorial and individualized. According to Wilson and Sato (2014), researchers should continue examining the relationship between emotional eating and stress due to the complexity of the topic. Effect of Academic Stress on Eating Behaviors in Students Growing research has shown that perceived stress often increases emotional eating behaviors in children and adolescents (Cartwright et al., 2003; Hou et al., 2013; Wichianson, Bughi, Unger, Spruijt-Metz, & Nguyen-Rodriguez 2009). However, the source(s) and types of stress impacting diet have yet to be thoroughly identified and surveyed. Few studies have focused on academic stress as an influence on emotional eating in adolescents and children, despite school being a mandatory and often stressful obligation in their lives. The limited research aimed at understanding academic stress and its relationship with eating behaviors has targeted populations generally regarded as having increased levels of perceived stress, such as university students. These studies have found that college students with higher stress levels had poorer nutrition habits, increased emotional eating, and night-eating than those with less stress (Bennett et al., 2013; Fogle & Pettijohn, 2013; Wichianson et al., 2009). In a Korean study, similar results were found in high-school students. Students who reported high academic stress ate larger meals and consumed more sugar than those reporting low academic stress (Kim et al., 2013). During periods of major exams, students’ diets appear to be influenced as well. One study found that on the day of the exam, females increased total energy intake and males increased intake of fat as a percentage of calories (Michaud et al.,1990). Another 10 study’s results showed that students with high trait anxiety and low social support increased energy intake between exam sessions (Pollard, Steptoe, Canaan, Davies, & Wardle, 1995). It is apparent that elements related to school often influence the eating patterns of students, but more evidence is needed to understand which particular academic factors are associated with emotional eating. Summary Based on the available studies, school-related stress and worries may alter eating behavior or predict emotional eating in adolescents. Despite the fact that school can be a major source of stress for youth, it is currently unknown what school-related elements may contribute to emotional eating in adolescents. No known studies have examined whether academic factors such as grade point average, academic stress, and schoolrelated worries predict emotional eating behavior in adolescents. Since adults have been the primary focus of emotional eating research, it is necessary to study what drives adolescents to emotionally eat in order to help develop healthier behaviors for lifelong health. 11 CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem, (2) GPA and (3) academic worries were related to emotional eating. Sample The study utilized cross-sectional data from a sample containing 666 middle school students in Los Angeles County. The data were obtained from seven schools, both public and private, as part of a larger physical activity intervention in minority students (Get Moving!). Seventh and eighth graders completed a psychosocial survey that included scales to assess a variety of aspects related to physical and mental health. A multitude of areas were assessed including, but not limited to: physical activity, body image, smoking, and weight concerns. Emotional eating, academic self-esteem, and frequency of school-related worries were assessed in the questionnaire and will be the focus of the proposed study. Demographic information such as age, ethnicity, and sex were also included. School Selection Sampling methods were focused on selecting Los Angeles County schools based on ethnic makeup and socioeconomic status. Data were obtained from the California 12 Board of Education and the Roman Catholic Archdiocese. The schools that agreed to participate had high numbers of Latino students and were mixed socioeconomically. Eight of the nine schools that the primary researcher approached agreed to participate. However, data were only collected from seven schools as one later declined to participate. Student Recruitment Physical education instructors at each school were approached to invite their classrooms to take part in the study. Of the 18 physical education teachers approached to participate, 17 agreed to participate. All students in the class were invited to complete the surveys. Student recruitment lasted 3 days; the principal investigator explained the study and distributed parental consent forms on the first day. All forms were available in both English and Spanish. A separate parent refusal form was distributed on day 3 to those students who had not returned the consent forms. Students with implied consent, meaning those who did not provide parental consent or parental refusal, were given an abbreviated version of the survey. Student active assent was also required for participation. Eighty-five percent of students consented (active and implied) to participate. Study procedures were approved by the University of Southern California Institutional Review Board and the boards of participating schools and school districts. Procedure Data collection manual procedures and scripts were followed by trained data collectors. The paper-and-pencil survey was given over two class periods and took approximately 45 minutes in total for students to complete. Confidentiality was 13 explained to students and maintained by assigning individual, arbitrary identification numbers to each student. Permission to use the collected data in the proposed study was attained by a signed consent letter from the principle investigator. Instrumentation Emotional Eating Emotional eating was measured using the Emotional Eating Subscale of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ), a 13-item questionnaire assessing eating behaviors in response to emotions (van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986). A 5point Likert scale was used to measure responses ranging from “never” to “very often.” The mean scores of the 13 items were calculated in order to obtain a continuous score for emotional eating behavior (Cronbach α = 0.95). The emotional eating subscale shows high internal consistency and high factorial validity in samples of obese and normal weight men and women (van Strien et al., 1986). Based on age and gender cut-off values available in the DEBQ manual (van Strien, 2005), students were further categorized as “emotional eaters” or “non-emotional eaters,” creating a nominal variable for analysis. Academic Self-Esteem Academic self-esteem was measured using a subscale of the Self-Image Questionnaire for Young Adolescents (SIQYA) created by Marsh, Parker, and Smith (1983). The academic confidence (AC) subscale of the SIQYA consists of 10-items and was used to determine students’ attitudes and perceptions about their academic ability and performance. A 4-point Likert scale was used to measure responses ranging from “no, always” to “yes, always.” The mean scores of the ten items were calculated in order to obtain a continuous score for academic self-esteem (Cronbach α = 0.81). Rasch 14 analysis indicates that the academic confidence subscale of the SIQYA fits the Rasch model and is a one-dimensional instrument (Marsh, 1990). Grade Point Average (GPA) After approval from the school district and school, students’ GPA from student records (de-identified, labeled only by study ID number) were provided to the investigator. Grade point average is used as a measurement of academic achievement through grades and is calculated by dividing the total amount of grade points and divided by amount of attempted credit units (Grade point average, 2015). The values for GPA range from 0.00 to 4.00, with 4.00 indicating the highest academic performance. Academic Worries A scale developed by Spruijt-Metz and Spruijt (1997) was utilized to measure worries, which are internal and often uncontrollable negative thoughts. The questions assessed how often the adolescent worried about each item over the past month. A 4point Likert scale was employed with responses ranging from “I never worry about this” to “I worry about this a lot.” The worry scale shows good internal consistency in adolescent populations including both genders (Spruijt-Metz & Spruijt, 1997). Two items that were specifically related to school were used to create an academic worries subscale. The purpose of the academic worries subscale is to quantify adolescent worrying in order to assess how often students worry about school. These items were averaged to obtain a scale score (Cronbach α = 0.70). Data Analysis All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS, version 21.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY). Descriptive statistics were used to describe demographic factors of 15 participants. Independent-samples t-tests were used to examine potential differences in variables between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters for Hypotheses 1-3. Forced entry multiple linear regressions were used for testing of Hypotheses 4-6. Since data are nested within schools, multi-level modeling was performed to control for the random effect of school, addressing the violation of the assumption of independence encountered with the use of nested data. Residual analysis and influence diagnostics indicated the model was a good fit to the observed data. Further model analyses supported cross-validation of the model as there was no substantial shrinkage. The majority of assumptions of multiple linear regression were met, although normality of the residuals may be problematic. However, based on central limit theorem, the sample is likely large enough to meet normality and the model would likely generalize to other samples within the targeted adolescent population. 16 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem, (2) GPA and (3) academic worries were related to emotional eating. Characteristics of Sample In total, 666 students completed surveys and their responses were included in data analyses; Table 1 provides data to describe the sample. The age of the sample ranged from 11 to 15 years, with a mean of 12.5 years. Nearly three-fourths of the sample was females (73%) and over half were Latino (61%). Roughly 20% of participants were classified as emotional eaters. Considering the possible range of scores, the mean values for both academic self-esteem and worries were moderately high. Differences in Academic Factors and Emotional Eating between Subjects Null hypotheses 1 through 3 stated there would be no significant difference in academic factors between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. Two-tailed independent samples t-tests were used to test these hypotheses and results are presented in Table 2. Hypothesis 1 was accepted; there was not a statistically significant difference when comparing grade point average for the emotional eater group to the non-emotional eater group (p = .547, r = .02). 17 TABLE 1. Characteristics of the Sample (N = 666) Variable N % Male 179 26.9 Female 487 73.1 Asian/PI 107 16.1 Latino 407 61.1 Multi-ethnic 77 11.6 Other 44 6.6 White 24 3.6 Emotional eaters 124 19.8 M SD Range 155.67 7.81 138-184 Emotional eating score 1.83 0.82 1–5 GPA 2.77 0.86 0.0 – 4 Academic Self-Esteem 3.06 0.56 1–4 Academic Worries 3.14 0.90 1–4 Sex Ethnicity Variable Age in months 18 Hypothesis 2 was also accepted which examined differences in academic self-esteem between emotional eaters and normal eaters. There was no statistically significant difference (p = .278, r = .04). Hypothesis 3 compared academic worries between those who were classified as emotional eaters and those who were not. There was no statistically significant difference between groups, (p = .633) or real effect (r = .02). TABLE 2. Differences in Academic Factors between Emotional and Non-Emotional Eaters Emotional eaters Non-emotional eaters Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Academic factors t p-value Academic Self-Esteem 3.01 (0.05) 3.08 (0.03) -1.09 .278 GPA 2.81 (0.08) 2.76 (0.04) -0.60 .547 Academic Worries 3.18 (0.08) 3.14 (0.04) -0.48 .633 Note: Statistical significance set at p < .05. Academic Factors as Predictors of Emotional Eating Forced entry multiple linear regression was performed to identify predictors of emotional eating. Multi-level modeling was performed and analyses controlled for age and sex. Results indicate that the model significantly explained 2.8% of the variance in emotional eating, F(5,601) = 3.479, p = .004, as shown in Table 3. 19 Null hypotheses 4 through 6 stated that GPA, academic self-esteem, and academic worries would not significantly predict emotional eating; all hypotheses were rejected. Grade point average (95% CI: .016, .206), academic self-esteem (95% CI: -.336, -.060) and academic worries (95% CI: .006, .152) significantly predicted emotional eating. Age and gender were not statistically significant predictors. Relationships were positive for academic worries and GPA. Thus, as academic worries and GPA increased, emotional eating increased. The relationship between academic self-esteem and emotional eating, however, was negative; as academic self-esteem increased emotional eating decreased. TABLE 3. Associations between Emotional Eating and Academic Factors Emotional Eating Β1 SE p-value2 Academic Self-Esteem -0.144 0.007 .005 GPA 0.142 0.048 .022 Academic Worries 0.084 0.037 .035 Age -0.047 .253 Gender 0.006 .945 Note: R2= .028 1 All parameter estimates (betas) are adjusted for age, sex, and random effect of school. 2 Statistical significance set at p < .05. 20 Summary Although there were no differences between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters in terms of GPA, academic self-esteem, and academic worries, regression analysis indicated that each of these three factors were significant predictors of emotional eating. Relationships were positive for academic worries and GPA; the relationship between academic self-esteem and emotional eating was negative. The covariates, age and gender, were not statistically significantly related to emotional eating. 21 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem, (2) GPA and (3) academic worries are related to emotional eating. Discussion of Findings Previous studies have indicated a relationship between stress, emotional eating, and weight gain in adults, yet few studies have explored academic-related factors, which may perpetuate stress, and emotional eating in adolescents (Epel et al., 2001; Ozier et al., 2008; Rutters et al., 2009). The current study explored this relationship using two analytical approaches. First, differences in GPA, academic self-esteem, and academic worries were examined between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters through independent-samples t-tests. Secondly, forced entry multiple linear regression was employed to test whether these factors predicted emotional eating; forced entry allowed for the effects of all variables to be controlled for the other, simultaneously. Results of the t-tests indicated there were no significant differences in GPA, academic self-esteem, and academic worries between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters, supporting null hypotheses 1 through 3. One possible explanation for the null results may be that the bivariate analyses failed to control for additional factors; these 22 analyses only capture simple associations, however, the relationship between academic factors and emotional eating appears to be more complex. Additionally, t-tests are less able to capture linear relationships, potentially contributing to the non-significant results. The study performed by Belcher et al. (2011) illustrated that worries significantly predicted emotional eating in adolescents; the findings in the present study are similar, but specific to school-related worries. Additionally, the trial by Belcher et al. included entirely female subjects whereas the current study included both sexes, although the sample was predominantly female. The current study lends to the generalizability of findings in the area of emotional eating to both sexes (as historically studies have been largely focused on females). The present study found no association between emotional eating and sex, indicating that the problem may affect both genders equally. Regression analyses revealed a positive, significant relationship between GPA and emotional eating. It was unexpected that the relationship was positive; it was assumed that the relationship would be negative because students with poor grades (low GPA) may have higher stress levels about school, which would increase the likelihood of emotional eating as a coping response. However, those with higher GPA scores actually had higher scores on emotional eating. One possible explanation for this result may be that individuals maintaining noble grades through dedication to their school work may have high levels of stress and engage in emotional eating to deal with the pressures of earning and maintaining high grades. Results from Kim et al. (2013) indirectly support this idea as they found high-school students who reported higher academic stress ate larger portions and more sugar than those who were less stressed. Earning high grades 23 has the possibility of causing high academic-related stress, but the direct relationship of these variables was not examined in the present study. Results of the linear regression also showed a negative, significant association between academic self-esteem and emotional eating. As overall feelings of academic success and satisfaction of accomplishments increased, emotional eating decreased. Although no known studies have specifically examined academic self-esteem, these findings are also in line with previous similar research. One study scrutinized relationships among self-esteem, stress, coping, and eating behavior in adolescents (Martyn-Nemeth, Penckofer, Gulanick, Velsor-Friedrich, & Bryant, 2009). Martyn et al. found that those with higher self-esteem reported less unhealthy eating behaviors and used less avoidant coping behaviors than adolescents with lower self-esteem. Although Martyn et al. studied overall self-esteem, their results are similar to the present study which used academic self-esteem, focusing only on feelings related to school. Additionally, the present study identified 19.8% of the sample were emotional eaters which is similar to the results by Martyn et al.; they stated that 25% of adolescents utilized food as a coping mechanism to deal with problems. The current study revealed intriguing information that has been largely overlooked; school is a common thread for all youth and may be a major contributor to emotional eating behavior. It is possible that emotional eating behavior is wellembedded long before students enter universities, where the majority of research between academics and eating patterns have been studied. Discovering the etiologies of emotional eating in youth may help develop programs that can emphasize the development of healthier eating habits and alternative coping mechanisms. Although the present study 24 did not specifically focus on emotional coping mechanisms in adolescents, emotional eating, by definition, is an avoidant coping behavior in response to negative feelings. Results from previous research suggest that adolescent coping mechanisms may be lacking; teaching teenagers healthy alternatives to managing negative feelings may help decrease emotional eating behaviors. A meta-analysis of school-based stress management programs concluded that primary prevention programs should be promoted for children and adolescents, as overall results show positive outcomes including improved coping and stress reduction (Kraag, Zeegers, Kok, Hosman, & Abu-Saad, 2006). Successful reductions in psychological stress and anxiety have also been found by Tai Chi and Yoga programs for youth (Frank, Bose, & Schrobenhauser-Clonan, 2014; Wall, 2005). Although these programs aim to reduce overall stress, no known programs have targeted academic-related stress in an effort to reduce emotional eating in adolescence. Educating youth on the potential consequences of emotional eating and teaching healthier alternatives to deal with academic stress could help change unhealthy eating behaviors before they become embedded, lifelong habits. Limitations The cross-sectional design of this study allowed the associations between academic factors and emotional eating to be described, however, it did not establish causality. In addition, results may not be easily extrapolated to the general population as the sample was largely composed of minority adolescents with a high number of females. Data was self-reported which may not have accurately captured data, however self-report is the best means available for psychosocial information to be collected. 25 Conclusions Previous researchers have examined academic stress and eating behavior of students on a limited basis. This study was the first known to investigate specific schoolrelated factors that affect academic stress and how they relate to emotional eating in adolescents. Given that GPA, academic self-esteem, and academic worries significantly predicted emotional eating, it is important to continue research in this area and promote programs that can help adolescents develop healthier coping mechanisms that are not food-related. Although analyses aimed to assess the influence of each individual factor, it must be noted that the overall model only significantly explained 2.8% of the variance in emotional eating. This highlights the fact that academic-related factors only account for a small portion of this behavior, supporting the notion that the influences on this response are multifactorial. Recommendations for Future Research More research is needed to fully investigate the relationship between academicrelated factors and emotional eating in adolescents. Studying additional factors that may be related to academic stress such as examinations, workload, parental pressure, and personality type may provide information that could help further elucidate the relationship between academic stress and emotional eating. Conducting further studies focusing on academic-related factors in different populations will provide additional information about the generalizability of these relationships. Longitudinal studies are needed to determine if the association between academic-related factors and emotional eating is causal. In addition, long term studies may provide insight as to how emotional eating can impact adolescents’ lives and health over time; and if students who 26 emotionally eat in response to academic stress at a young age continue to do so at the university level. Furthermore, effective ways to reduce overall academic stress and teach healthier management techniques to adolescents should continue to be examined. Summary The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different academic factors on emotional eating, among a sample of minority adolescents (including both males and females). Specifically, this study examined whether (1) academic self-esteem, (2) GPA and (3) academic worries were related to emotional eating. There were no significant bivariate differences in academic factors between emotional eaters and non-emotional eaters. However, positive, significant linear relationships were found for academic worries and GPA. Further, the relationship between academic self-esteem was negative and significant. Considering the disparate findings resulting from the two analytic approaches, more research is needed to better understand the complex association between academics and emotional eating behavior. Although current results mimic trends found in previous research, this is the first study to identify specific, individual academic factors related to academic-stress and how they may influence emotional eating behavior. This unique and novel research suggests that emotional eating is not limited to a small demographic group of adult, Caucasian females as previously studied; but occurs in both sexes of minority youth as well and may be triggered by school-related stressors. Considering that academics are an integral aspect of the lives of all youth, the importance of addressing negative health behaviors associated with academic stress is underscored. The current study sheds light on the importance of examining the motives for emotional eating and identifying when these 27 patterns may initiate in order to teach alternative behaviors that promote lifelong health and effective stress management to youth. 28 APPENDICES 29 APPENDIX A SURVEY ITEMS 30 You and school H1. People come to me for help in most school subjects 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H2. I am too stupid at school to get into a good university 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H3. If I work really hard I could be one of the best students in my school year 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H4. I get bad marks in most school subjects 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H5. I learn things quickly in most school subjects 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H6. I am stupid at most school subjects 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H7. I do well in tests in most school subjects 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H8. I have trouble with most school subjects 31 1 2 3 4 False Mostly false Mostly true True H9. I am good at most school subjects 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True H10. Most school subjects are just too hard for me 1 False 2 Mostly false 3 Mostly true 4 True Eating and Emotions Think about how you usually feel before you start eating. Check the answer that matches how often the following statements apply to you: P1. I feel like eating when I am irritated. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P2. I feel like eating when I have nothing to do. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P3. I feel like eating when I am depressed or discouraged. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P4. I feel like eating when I am feeling lonely. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P5. I feel like eating when somebody lets me down. 32 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P6. I feel like eating when I am angry. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P7. I feel like eating when something unpleasant happens. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P8. I feel like eating when I am anxious, worried or tense. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P9. I feel like eating when things are going against me or have gone wrong. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P10. I feel like eating when I am frightened. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P11. I feel like eating when I am disappointed. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P12. I feel like eating when I am emotionally upset. 1 Never 33 2 3 4 5 Rarely Sometimes Often Very often P13. I feel like eating when I am bored or restless. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often How much have you worried about things in the past month? K5. My grades 1 I never worry about this 2 I worry about this a little bit 3 I worry about this sometimes 4 I worry about this a lot K6. Getting into a good school or college 1 I never worry about this 2 I worry about this a little bit 3 I worry about this sometimes 4 I worry about this a lot 34 APPENDIX B LETTERS OF APPROVAL 35 September 15, 2014 Ashley Chamberlin 215 Belmont Ave. Apt 8 Long Beach, CA 90803 Donna Spruijt-Metz, PhD, MFA University of Southern California 635 Downey Way, Suite 305 Los Angeles, CA 90089-3332 Dear Dr. Spruijt-Metz, My name is Ashley Chamberlin and I am a candidate for the Master of Science in Nutritional Science degree from California State University, Long Beach (CSULB). I am currently working on my thesis proposal titled, The Relationship between Emotional Eating and Academic Measures in Adolescents. I plan to examine the relationship between emotional eating, GPA, and academic self-esteem in middle school students. I would like to use baseline demographic and survey data from the Get Moving project for data analysis. If you agree to allow me to access your participant database for the first wave of the Get Moving project for this study, please sign the pre-written consent letter that has been written on your behalf by Monday, October 5, 2014. I appreciate your consideration regarding my request. If you have further questions, please feel free to contact me via e-mail at ashleychamberlin@gmail.com or at (951) 834-3638. Sincerely, Ashley Chamberlin September 23, 2014 36 Ashley Chamberlin 215 Belmont Ave. Apt# 8 Long Beach, CA 90803 To Whom It May Concern: I grant permission for Ashley Chamberlin to conduct the study entitled, The Relationship between Emotional Eating and Academic Measures in Adolescents using our baseline database from the first wave of the Get Moving project to examine the relationship between emotional eating, GPA, and academic self-esteem in adolescents attending middle school. I understand that this study is in partial fulfillment of the requirement for the Master of Science in Nutritional Science for Ashley Chamberlin. I am granting Ashley Chamberlin permission to conduct the appropriate analyses for the study. These analyses will be conducted during the spring semester in 2015. Sincerely, Donna Spruijt-Metz, PhD, MFA University of Southern California 635 Downey Way, Suite 305 Los Angeles, CA 90089-3332 37 REFERENCES 38 REFERENCES Adam, T. C., & Epel, E. S. (2007). Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiology & Behavior, 91(4), 449–58. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011 American Psychological Association. (2007). Stress a major health problem in the US warns APA. Retrieved November 11, 2014, from http://www.apa.org/print-this/aspx Arnow, B., Kenardy, J., & Agras, W. S. (1995). The emotional eating scale: The development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(1), 79–90. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/ 7670446 Ashcroft, J., Semmler, C., Carnell, S., van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., & Wardle, J. (2008). Continuity and stability of eating behaviour traits in children. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 62(8), 985–90. doi:10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602855 Belcher, B. R., Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T., McClain, A. D., Hsu, Y.-W., Unger, J. B., & SpruijtMetz, D. (2011). The influence of worries on emotional eating, weight concerns, and body mass index in Latina female youth. The Journal of Adolescent Health : Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 48(5), 487–92. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010. 08.008 Bennett, J., Greene, G., & Schwartz-Barcott, D. (2013). Perceptions of emotional eating behavior. A qualitative study of college students. Appetite, 60(1), 187–92. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.023 Borkovec T.D., Robinson E., Pruzinsky T., & Depree, J. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 9–16. Braet, C., Claus, L., Goossens, L., Moens, E., Van Vlierberghe, L., & Soetens, B. (2008). Differences in eating style between overweight and normal-weight youngsters. Journal of Health Psychology, 13(6), 733–43. doi:10.1177/1359105308093850 Braet C. & van Stier, T. (1997). Assessment of emotional, externally induced and restrained eating behaviour in non to twelve-year-old obese and non-obese children. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, 863– 873. 39 Brown, J. E., Issacs, J. S., Krinke, U.B., Lechtenberg, E., Murtaugh, M., Sharbaugh, C., & Splett, P. (2011). Nutrition through the life cycle. Belmont, CA: CENGAGE Learning. Brown, S.L., Teufel, J.A., Birch, D.A., & Kancherla, V. (2006). Gender, age and behavior differences in early adolescent worry. Journal of School Health, 76(8), 430–437. Cartwright, M., Wardle, J., Steggles, N., Simon, A. E., Croker, H., & Jarvis, M. J. (2003). Stress and dietary practices in adolescents. Health Psychology, 22(4), 362–369. doi:10.1037/02786133.22.4.362 Cattanach, L., Phil, M., & Malley, R. (1988). Psychologic and physiologic reactivity to stressors in eating disordered individuals. Appetite, 599, 591–599. De Vriendt, T., Moreno, L. A., & De Henauw, S. (2009). Chronic stress and obesity in adolescents: Scientific evidence and methodological issues for epidemiology research. Nutrition, Metabolism and Cardiovascular Diseases, 19, 511–519. Epel, E., Lapidus, R., McEwen, B., & Brownell, K. (2001). Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(1), 37–49. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00035-4 Fogle, G. E., & Pettijohn, T. F. (2013). Stress and health habits in college students. Open Journal of Medical Psychology, 61–68. Frank, J. L., Bose, B., & Schrobenhauser-Clonan, A. (2014). Effectiveness of a school-based yoga program on adolescent mental health, stress coping strategies, and attitudes toward violence: Findings from a high-risk sample. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 30(1), 29–49. doi:10.1080/15377903.2013.863259 Grade point average. (2015). In Merriam-Webster online dictionary. Retrieved from http://www.merriam-webster.com/info/copyright.htm Greeno, C. G., & Wing, R. R. (1994). Stress-induced eating. Physiological Bulliten, 115(3), 444– 464. Gundersen, C.,Mahatmya, D., Garasky, S., & Lohman, B. (2010). Linking psychosocial stressors and childhood obesity. Obesity Reviews, 12, 54–63. Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 86–108. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.110.1.86 Hou, F., Xu, S., Zhao, Y., Lu, Q., Zhang, S., Zu, P., … Tao, F. (2013). Effects of emotional symptoms and life stress on eating behaviors among adolescents. Appetite, 68, 63–8. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.010 40 Jenkins, S. K., Rew, L., & Sternglanz, R. W. (2005). Eating behaviors among school-age children associated with perceptions of stress. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 28(3), 175–91. doi:10.1080/01460860500227580 Kim, Y., Yang, H. Y., Kim, A.-J., & Lim, Y. (2013). Academic stress levels were positively associated with sweet food consumption among Korean high-school students. Nutrition, 29(1), 213–8. doi:10.1016/j.nut.2012.08.005 Kraag, G., Zeegers, M. P., Kok, G., Hosman, C., & Abu-Saad, H. H. (2006). School programs targeting stress management in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 44(6), 449–472. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2006.07.001 Laugesen, N., Dugas, M. J., & Bukowski, W. M. (2003). Understanding adolescent worry: The application of a cognitive model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(1), 55–64. Marsh, H. W. (1990). The structure of academic self-concept: The Marsh/Shavelson model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 623–636. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.82.4.623 Marsh, H. W., Parker, J. W., & Smith, I. D. (1983). Preadolescent self-concept: Its relation to self-concept as inferred by teachers and to academic ability. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53(1), 60–78. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1983.tb02536.x Martyn-Nemeth, P., Penckofer, S., Gulanick, M., Velsor-Friedrich, B., & Bryant, F. B. (2009). The relationships among self-esteem, stress, coping, eating behavior, and depressive mood in adolescents. Research in Nursing & Health, 32(1), 96–109. doi:10.1002/nur.20304 Michaud, C., Kahn, J., Musse, N., Burlet, C., Nicolas, J., & Mejean, L. (1990). Relationships between a critical life event and eating behaviour in high-school students. Stress Medicine Journal, 6, 57–64. Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T., Unger, J. B., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2009). Psychological determinants of emotional eating in adolescence. Eating Disorders, 17(3), 211–24. doi:10.1080/1064 0260902848543 Ozier, A. D., Kendrick, O. W., Leeper, J. D., Knol, L. L., Perko, M., & Burnham, J. (2008). Overweight and obesity are associated with emotion- and stress-related eating as measured by the eating and appraisal due to emotions and stress questionnaire. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 108(1), 49–56. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2007.10.011 Pollard, T. M., Steptoe, A., Canaan, L., Davies, G. J., & Wardle, J. (1995). Effects of academic examination stress on eating behavior and blood lipid levels. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2(4), 299–320. doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm0204_2 Roemmich, J. N., Wright, S. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2002). Dietary restraint and stress-induced snacking in youth. Obesity Research, 10(11), 1120–6. doi:10.1038/oby.2002.152 41 Rutters, F., Nieuwenhuizen, A. G., Lemmens, S. G. T., Born, J. M., & Westerterp-Plantenga, M. S. (2009). Acute stress-related changes in eating in the absence of hunger. Obesity, 17(1), 72–7. doi:10.1038/oby.2008.493 Skaalvik, E. M. (1990). Gender differences in general academic self-esteem and in success expectations on defined academic problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(3), 593–598. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.82.3.593 Spoor, S. T. P., Bekker, M. H. J., Van Strien, T., & van Heck, G. L. (2007). Relations between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite, 48(3), 368–76. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.005 Spruijt-Metz, D., & Spruijt, R. J. (1997). Worries and health in adolescence : A latent variable approach. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26(4), 485–501. Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Wilfley, D. E., & Spurrell, E. (2000). Impact of interpersonal and egorelated stress on restrained eaters. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27(4), 411–8. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10744847 Van Strien, T. (2005). Nederlandse vragenlijst voor eetgedrag handleiding en verantwoording [Dutch eating behaviour questionnaire, manual]. Amsterdam: Boomtestpublishers. Van Strien, T., Frijters, J., Bergers, G., & Defares, P. (1986). The Dutch eating behavior questionnaire ( DEBQ ) for assessment of restrained , emotional , and external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5(2), 295–315. Van Strien, T., van der Zwaluw, C. S., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2010). Emotional eating in adolescents: A gene (SLC6A4/5-HTT) - depressive feelings interaction analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44(15), 1035–42. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.012 Wahab, S., Rahman, F. N. A., Wan Hasan, W. M. H., Zamani, I. Z., Arbaiei, N. C., Khor, S. L., & Nawi, A. M. (2013). Stressors in secondary boarding school students: Association with stress, anxiety and depressive symptoms. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry : Official Journal of the Pacific Rim College of Psychiatrists, 5 (Suppl 1), 82–9. doi:10.1111/appy.12067 Wall, R. B. (2005). Tai Chi and mindfulness-based stress reduction in a Boston Public Middle School. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 19(4), 230–7. doi:10.1016/j.pedhc.2005.02.006 Wichianson, J. R., Bughi, S. A., Unger, J. B., Spruijt-Metz, D., & Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T. (2009). Perceived stress, coping and night-eating in college students. Stress and Health, 25(3), 235–240. doi:10.1002/smi.1242 Wilson, S. M., & Sato, A. F. (2014). Stress and paediatric obesity: What we know and where to go. Stress and Health : Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress, 30(2), 91–102. doi:10.1002/smi.2501 42