CHAPTER 10 ACUTE STRESS DISORDER AND POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Richard A. Bryant Acute stress disorder (ASD) is relatively new addition to the diagnostic systems used to describe stress reactions in the aftermath of trauma. It was introduced in 1994 in the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). It has been a notoriously controversial diagnosis insofar as people have questioned its conceptual basis, operational definition, and empirical support. Despite this, the advent of the diagnosis has resulted in an exponential increase in attention given to acute trauma reactions and a wealth of new research that has shed light on how we understand posttraumatic stress responses occurring immediately or soon after the traumatic event. This chapter reviews the historical roots of the ASD disorder, tracks its development to its current understanding in DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), discusses known mechanisms of ASD, and reviews prevailing assessment and treatment procedures. A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE ASD DIAGNOSIS Throughout the course of the various successive iterations of the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) diagnosis in DSM, there has been a restriction of 1 month after trauma exposure on how soon this diagnosis can be made. This stipulation was instituted because of concerns that a premature labeling of a maladaptive stress response would overpathologize normative and transient stress reactions. This perspective was debated in the lead up to DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) when commentators felt that this situation resulted in a diagnostic gap that precluded acutely distressed people from being accurately identified. One of the major reasons for this concern was that in the U.S. health-care system, access to care can be compromised if a person does not have a formally recognized diagnosis. Hence, it was proposed that a diagnosis describing acute stress reactions would promote pathways to health care within a month of trauma exposure. Additionally, a second goal of the new ASD diagnosis was proposed, which was to identify acutely traumatized people who subsequently will develop PTSD. As we will discuss, many people will display acute distress in the initial days after trauma exposure, and this distress can extend to the initial week or two. Many of these people, however, will not progress to develop PTSD. Thus, a major challenge for early intervention always has been to disentangle those people in the acute phase who have a transient stress response from those displaying the early signs of a chronic course of PTSD. It was proposed that ASD would provide a solution, at least to some degree, to this problem. ASD was defined by DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) in a format that was generally consistent with the PTSD diagnosis, with some important distinctions. The individual needed to be exposed to a traumatic event in which there was actual or threatened death or serious injury and must have experienced fear, helplessness, or horror (Criterion A). In addition, four additional symptom clusters needed to be satisfied. Cluster B, which http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0000019-010 APA Handbook of Trauma Psychology: Vol. 1. Foundations in Knowledge, S. N. Gold (Editor-in-Chief) Copyright © 2017 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved. APA Handbook of Trauma Psychology: Foundations in Knowledge, edited by S. N. Gold Copyright © 2017 American Psychological Association. All rights reserved. 161 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Richard A. Bryant is the cluster that distinguished ASD from PTSD, required at least three of the following dissociative symptoms: (a) a subjective sense of numbing, detachment, or absence of emotional responsiveness; (b) reduced awareness of surroundings; (c) derealization; (d) depersonalization; and (e) dissociative amnesia. These symptoms could exist either at the time of trauma (often called peritraumatic dissociation) or in the month afterward. One also needed to exhibit one or more reexperiencing symptoms, such as intrusive thoughts, images, nightmares, or distress to reminders of the event (Criterion C), marked avoidance of stimuli that remind the individual of the trauma must be present (Criterion D), and marked anxiety or arousal symptoms (Criterion E). These symptoms had to cause significant distress or impairment (Criterion F), and last for a minimum of 2 days and a maximum of 4 weeks (Criterion G). CRITICISMS OF THE ASD DIAGNOSIS When the new ASD diagnosis was introduced, it was met with much criticism for conceptual, empirical, and procedural reasons. From a conceptual view, it was considered unusual to have a diagnosis that had a goal of predicting another subsequent, and phenomenologically similar, diagnosis (Bryant & Harvey, 2000). Another conceptual concern was that the diagnosis may result in excessive diagnoses of normative stress responses, thereby labeling people as disordered when, in fact, they were simply experiencing understandable stress reactions that would later abate (Marshall, Spitzer, & Liebowitz, 1999). Other criticisms focused on the dearth of solid evidence for the criteria to both identify those who were acutely distressed and predict later PTSD (Bryant & Harvey, 1997). Notably, those who were strong supporters of the new diagnosis recognized that the link between ASD and later PTSD was “based more on logical arguments than on empirical research” (Koopman, Classen, Cardeña, & Spiegel, 1995, p. 38). The concerns for the lack of empirical evidence for the ASD diagnosis was highlighted by concerns that the emphasis in the diagnostic criteria on dissociation had a limited empirical base, 162 although more evidence did emerge after the diagnosis was introduced (Marshall et al., 1999). THE ROLE OF DISSOCIATION Given the emphasis placed on dissociation in ASD, it is important to fully understand the rationale and evidence for this feature of the acute trauma response. We can see the evolution of the dissociative school of thought commencing in the seminal work of Janet in the early 20th century in Paris (Janet, 1907). In his work with people who developed a range of psychosomatic and hysterical symptoms following trauma, he observed that some trauma survivors responded to the trauma by splitting awareness of their distressed state from normal consciousness. Although this led to short-term relief, he proposed that this splitting (or dissociation) led to psychological problems because of the demands this placed on psychological resources. It is curious that this school of thought did not enjoy great influence for much of the 20th century, arguably because of the dominance of psychoanalysis and the rising emergence of behaviorism. In the latter years of the 21st century, the notion of dissociation being a pivotal role in traumatic stress response gained momentum, with many commentators believing it was central to the development of posttraumatic psychopathology (van der Kolk & van der Hart, 1989). This notion was supported by evidence of elevated levels of hypnotizability, a trait associated with dissociation, in people with PTSD (Spiegel, Hunt, & Dondershine, 1988; Stutman & Bliss, 1985). Numerous reports also had indicated that people with chronic PTSD had higher levels of dissociation, as measured by dissociative tendencies scales (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986; Bremner et al., 1992; Coons, Bowman, Pellow, & Schneider, 1989). Relating this idea to ASD, it was argued by many that dissociative responses to the trauma in the acute phase resulted in limited access to the trauma memories, which subsequently could hinder processing of emotional experiences and thereby lead to ongoing PTSD (Spiegel, Koopman, & Classen, 1994). Supporting this idea, some limited evidence from early studies indicated that dissociation in the acute phase was Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder associated with subsequent PTSD (Cardeña & Spiegel, 1993; Holen, 1993); this finding was replicated numerous times after the introduction of the ASD diagnosis (Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 1998; Murray et al., 2002). Some critics of the emphasis on dissociation expressed concerns about the mechanism that dissociation was purportedly playing in the acute phase. In setting out diagnostic criteria for ASD, DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) allowed that dissociative symptoms could occur in this syndrome during the trauma or during its aftermath. The ambiguity over when dissociation arose in ASD is problematic because when it occurs may lead to distinct outcomes. For example, dissociative reactions that occur during a trauma may act to limit encoding of the traumatic experience and, in this sense, actually may be protective (Horowitz, 1997). In contrast, if dissociation persists continuously after the traumatic event, then it is possible that it serves the function of limiting access to the trauma memories and their affect. Most dissociative reactions after trauma abate with time (Davidson, Kudler, Saunders, & Smith, 1990), which underscores that persistent dissociation is not common. Furthermore, the alterations in awareness and perception that people often experience during a trauma are actually quite common in everyday life (Ross, Joshi, & Currie, 1990). It appears that these alterations in awareness are particularly common when we are highly aroused, and this can be attributed to reduced working memory capacity, limiting the capacity to encode normal amounts of information when our minds are being bombarded by lots of stimuli during an arousing experience. For example, people taking part in skydiving for the first time typically find this to be a frightening experience and also report high levels of dissociation, but this does not seem pathological because people do not subsequently develop PTSD (Sterlini & Bryant, 2002). Furthermore, when people are under threat, evidence indicates that they narrow their attention onto stimuli that are likely to be the source of threat, thereby reducing the encoding of other events (Kramer, Buckhout, & Eugenio, 1990; Maass & Köhnken, 1989). What if dissociative responses are more persistent and do not subside after the initial days following the traumatic event? This may have an impact on how the person can access trauma memories and arguably could lead to more psychopathological responses. This view is supported by studies that have focused on dissociation that occurs during the trauma and dissociation that persists after the traumatic event. Across studies, dissociation that persists after exposure to the trauma is linked to both acute (Panasetis & Bryant, 2003) and chronic (Briere, Scott, & Weathers, 2005) posttraumatic reactions. Probably the most commonly used measure of peritraumatic dissociation is the Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire (Marmar, Weiss, & Metzler, 1997); factor analytic studies have shown that it encompasses two subscales: reduced awareness and derealization or depersonalization. Although reduced awareness is not strongly linked to psychopathological responses, derealization or depersonalization is (Brooks et al., 2009). This finding highlights that dissociation is not a uniform construct but rather that different forms of dissociative experience play distinct roles in the posttraumatic response. One recent study that employed latent profile analysis revealed that acutely traumatized people who are most symptomatic are characterized by elevated dissociation and avoidance (Hansen, Armour, Wang, Elklit, & Bryant, 2015). This pattern may suggest that although dissociation is not prominent in all ASD individuals, it may feature in those who are particularly distressed. THE PREDICTIVE POWER OF ASD Following the introduction of the ASD diagnosis in DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), a series of longitudinal studies appeared that assessed for ASD within the initial month after trauma exposure and subsequently assessed for PTSD. This increasing body of evidence allowed the field to better understand how predictive ASD was of later PTSD. A review of the published longitudinal studies identified 23 investigations (Bryant, 2011). Several major points emerge from these studies. First, across the majority of these studies, most trauma survivors with ASD subsequently met criteria for 163 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Richard A. Bryant PTSD. That is, the ASD diagnosis appears to be doing a good job insofar as most people with the disorder do not remit but rather continue to have longterm PTSD. In contrast, the sensitivity in predicting PTSD was quite poor, meaning that most trauma survivors who eventually developed PTSD did not meet the criteria for ASD initially. Put another way, ASD was failing to identify most people in the acute phase who would develop PTSD. The emphasis on dissociation may preclude many distressed people from inclusion in the ASD diagnosis; although they manifest re-experiencing, avoidance, and arousal symptoms, they may not meet the stringent criterion for dissociation. In fact, several studies assessed subsyndromal ASD, which typically required meeting three of the four ASD symptom clusters (but not dissociation). These studies found that the sensitivity in predicting PTSD was better if one used subsyndromal ASD. Even these studies, however, found only modest sensitivity in predicting PTSD. An interesting pattern emerges when we look at the predictive capacity of ASD in children. Consistent with the adult literature, overall PTSD symptoms tend to abate over time in children (Kinzie, Sack, Angell, Manson, & Rath, 1986; Milgram et al., 1988). When we peruse the five child studies that have assessed ASD and PTSD longitudinally (Bryant, Salmon, Sinclair, & Davidson, 2007; Dalgleish et al., 2008; Ji, Xiaowei, Chuanlin, & Wei, 2010; KassamAdams & Winston, 2004; Meiser-Stedman, Yule, Smith, Glucksman, & Dalgleish, 2005), the pattern suggests that ASD is an even weaker predictor of PTSD in children than adults. The significant majority of children who develop PTSD after trauma do not display ASD initially. This may be attributed to the poor understanding of dissociative responses in children after trauma. TRAJECTORIES OF POSTTRAUMATIC RESPONSE As researchers have struggled with the predictive capacity of the ASD diagnosis, they have attempted to improve prediction by emphasizing certain symptoms, such as re-experiencing (Brewin, Andrews, Rose, & Kirk, 1999), insomnia (Shalev, Freedman, et al., 1998), emotional numbing (Harvey & Bryant, 164 1998), different symptom permutations (Brewin et al., 1999), or the overall level of acute symptoms (Bryant, Moulds, & Guthrie, 2000). Despite these attempts, none of these strategies have really provided adequate sensitivity or positive predictive power for identifying people in the acute phase who subsequently will develop PTSD. Why is prediction of PTSD so difficult? Probably the answer to this question is the increasing evidence that posttraumatic stress responses do not develop, or maintain, in a linear fashion. Instead, it appears that people fluctuate enormously over time in the degree of PTSD symptoms they may experience. In a large longitudinal study of traumatically injured patients who were assessed in hospital and again at 3, 12, and 24 months later, it was found that the rate of PTSD remained fairly constant over time—but approximately half of people who had PTSD, or even subsyndromal PTSD, at any time point had a different status at the next assessment (Bryant, O’Donnell, Creamer, McFarlane, & Silove, 2013). For example, of those who had PTSD at 12 months, 37% had no PTSD at 3 months—suggesting a worsening of symptoms for many people. This realization has led the field of traumatic stress to look beyond simple diagnostic categories to understand PTSD response because this oversimplifies the condition of the individual. Categorizing individuals as having PTSD or not is arbitrary because these individuals need to shift by only 1 of a possible 20 symptoms and they instantly jump from being categorized as a case to a noncase. Does this shift in one symptom accurately reflect their clinical condition? We know that it does not because of much evidence that people with subsyndromal PTSD can suffer as much functional impairment as those with full PSTD (Stein, Walker, Hazen, & Forde, 1997). Even when people have looked beyond diagnostic rates over time and studied severity of PTSD, they have tended to focus on the average response to a traumatic event—this can lead to the conclusion that whereas most people will display some distress initially, they will tend to adapt in the following weeks or months. More sophisticated statistical approaches, however, have now mapped the trajectories by using latent growth Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder mixture modeling, which permits distinct groups to be identified so that their overall path of adjustment can be modeled. Put another way, rather than assuming that all people follow the same path, it permits different trajectories to be mapped. This approach has resulted in numerous longitudinal studies being analyzed, and remarkably, the trajectories that have been identified show much consistency. Different researchers have noted (a) a resilient class with consistently few PTSD symptoms, (b) a recovery class with initial distress then gradual remission, (c) a delayed reaction class with initial low symptom levels but increased symptoms over time, and (d) a chronic distress class with consistently high PTSD levels. Across all studies, the resilient class is the most prevalent, with approximately three quarters of trauma survivors enjoying minimal distress across time. These patterns have been observed in survivors of traumatic injury (Bryant et al., 2015), severe acute respiratory syndrome (Bonanno et al., 2008), women diagnosed with breast cancer (Lam et al., 2010), disaster (Pietrzak, Van Ness, Fried, Galea, & Norris, 2013), disaster and terrorist attacks (Norris, Tracy, & Galea, 2009), and military personnel deployed to the Middle East (Bonanno, Mancini, et al., 2012). These lessons teach us important lessons about predicting PTSD. First, people will respond in a variety of ways after trauma, and this complicates how we will identify these people in the acute phase. Second, the factors that influence how a person adapts following trauma can include life events that occur after the trauma, and so these cannot be assessed in the acute phase. We are only at the beginning of using this approach to map trajectories, and so the next step is to predict which trajectory people will follow. One study of rape victims that found two trajectories noted that ASD predicted membership of the more distressed group (Armour, Shevlin, Elklit, & Mroczek, 2012). At this point, it appears that the simplest means to predict who will require mental health assistance is to identify those who will be resilient because they typically will be psychologically healthy over time. This is a different approach than the one adopted by the ASD diagnosis, which seeks to target those with a specific disorder rather than excluding those who appear healthy. ASD IN DSM–5 As the field of acute traumatic stress accumulated greater evidence, it became apparent that the conceptualization and operation of ASD was in need of revisions in the next edition of DSM. In 2013, the American Psychiatric Association released DSM–5 and in this iteration the ASD diagnosis underwent some important changes. One of the most important shifts in focus was that it no longer was intended to predict subsequent PTSD (Bryant, Friedman, Spiegel, Ursano, & Strain, 2011). On the basis that the ASD diagnosis was not identifying most people who subsequently develop PTSD, it was decided that this goal should be abandoned. Instead, the emphasis was placed on identifying people who were experiencing severe stress reactions and who could benefit from mental health services. In this sense, remember that the DSM is a U.S. product that needs to accommodate the distinctive needs of U.S. health-care systems; it was proposed that this diagnosis would facilitate access to mental services. Although it has been suggested that the ASD diagnosis was not necessary because the adjustment disorder diagnosis also could be made to describe people in the initial month after trauma, the latter diagnosis presents strong concerns because some health companies do not recognize this diagnosis like they do ASD, and it often is perceived as being transient and therefore not requiring clinical intervention. The DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) definition of ASD also moved away from the prior emphasis on dissociation. The DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) ASD definition required patients to display three dissociative symptoms and to satisfy the re-experiencing, avoidance, and arousal clusters. Since the introduction of the ASD diagnosis, it became apparent from crosssectional and longitudinal studies that patients experienced considerable heterogeneity in their symptoms, and there was actually little empirical justification for the specified number of symptoms in each symptom cluster. Several factor analytic studies have been conducted on ASD symptoms, with varying results. Whereas some have supported the DSM–IV conceptualization of the four factors (Brooks, Silove, Bryant, O’Donnell, Creamer, & McFarlane, 2008; 165 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Richard A. Bryant Cardeña, Koopman, Classen, Waelde, & Spiegel, 2000; Wang et al., 2000), others have found that this four-factor structure does not readily accommodate acute stress responses (Armour, Elklit, & Shevlin, 2013; Bryant, Moulds, & Guthrie, 2000). Accordingly, in DSM–5, a different approach was adopted that did not require specific symptom clusters to be present. Instead, it was decided that a minimum of 9 of a possible 14 symptoms should be present. This was deemed to reflect people who suffered severe stress reactions that may warrant mental health intervention. Although the DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) definition of ASD did not require symptom clusters to be satisfied, the criteria nonetheless are listed under specific headings to facilitate clinician use. The initial group of symptoms are the intrusive symptoms and include (a) recurring intrusive memories of the trauma, (b) recurrent dreams related to the traumatic experience, (c) flashback memories that involve some degree of reliving of the actual experience, and (d) marked psychological distress or physiological reactivity to things that remind the person of the trauma. The next category involves negative mood, which contains the single symptom of persistent inability to experience positive emotions. This used to be termed emotional numbing (a dissociative symptom in the DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), but in DSM–5 the symptom focused more on difficulties in accessing positive emotions (rather than all emotional responses) because of the evidence that PTSD is characterized by hyperreactivity to negative stimuli (Orr, 1997) but not to positive cues (Litz, Orsillo, Kaloupek, & Weathers, 2000). Next the dissociative symptoms are itemized, and these were reduced from the five symptoms listed in DSM–IV to two. Reduced awareness of one’s surroundings was deleted because it predominantly pertained to responses occurring during the trauma, and as noted, this is not associated with ongoing psychopathology. The symptoms of derealization and depersonalization that were listed separately in DSM–IV were combined into a single item in DSM–5 because of the documented overlap between these two responses in available data (Harvey & Bryant, 1999a). The remaining dissociative 166 symptom is dissociative amnesia. The next symptom heading refers to the avoidance symptoms. One symptom refers to effortful avoidance of traumarelated thoughts or emotions and the other refers to avoidance of situations or conversations that remind the person of the traumatic event. The last symptom category involves arousal symptoms, which lists five symptoms: sleep disturbance, irritable or aggressive behavior, hypervigilance, concentration problems, and heightened startle response. DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) also made a minor shift in the timeframe during which ASD can be diagnosed. The DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) stipulation that ASD could be diagnosed 2 days after the event lacked any empirical base, which is not surprising considering the paucity of research on the progression of stress symptoms in the initial days after trauma. Evidence suggests that many people who will experience stress reactions in the days immediately following a traumatic event subsequently will report markedly fewer reactions in the period after (Solomon, Laor, & McFarlane, 1996). Given this lack of evidence, in DSM–5, the time required to delay a diagnosis was extended from 2 to 3 days to minimize diagnosing people in the acute phase when they may be experiencing temporary stress reactions. It should be noted, however, that the timeframe of 3 days also lacks evidence to support it. Incidence of ASD In terms of the incidence of ASD, the temporary nature of the condition hinders proper epidemiological analysis of population rates. Perusing large-scale psychiatric studies of population mental health typically leads to the conclusion that ASD often is not included in the surveys because of the focus on more chronic disorders. Accordingly, our knowledge of how frequently ASD exists is limited to studies of trauma-exposed populations in which researchers have assessed for ASD within 1 month. Consistent with patterns of findings in studies of chronic PTSD, these studies have led to varied rates of ASD. For example, ASD has been reported following motor vehicle accidents (13–21%), mild traumatic brain injury (14%), assault (16–19%), burns Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (10%), industrial accidents (6%), and witnessing a mass shooting (33%; Harvey & Bryant, 2002). Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Cognitive Models of ASD One of the most influential models in PTSD involves the cognitive mechanisms that occur during and immediately after trauma exposure. Exemplifying this approach is Ehlers and Clark’s (2000) model, which posited that traumatic stress is affected strongly by two cognitive processes: maladaptive appraisals about the trauma and its aftermath, and autobiographical memories. Regarding appraisals, this model proposes that how we respond to trauma is influenced strongly by how we interpret what it means for our safety, our future, and our worth as a person. Central to this idea is that people can generalize from appraisals of the trauma to other life events, which can result in pervasive fear or other negative affective states. These appraisals can focus on one’s vulnerability (e.g., “I can never feel safe again”), one’s role in the trauma (e.g., “the attack was my fault”), or one’s capacity to cope with the aftermath (e.g., “I am a terrible mother for not coping better”). Such appraisals heighten the sense of threat that one can experience after the trauma, which fuels more stress symptoms and leads to PTSD. A large body of evidence supports the proposition that maladaptive appraisals in the acute phase play an important role in posttraumatic adjustment. It is worth noting that people with chronic PTSD often display distorted appraisals of the trauma (Dunmore, Clark, & Ehlers, 1997; Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin, & Orsillo, 1999) and also the extent to which having these symptoms reflect negatively on the trauma survivor (e.g., having flashback memories may be interpreted as a sign of losing one’s mind; Ehlers et al., 1998; Ehlers & Steil, 1995; Steil & Ehlers, 2000). Similarly, people with ASD tend to exaggerate both the probability of negative events happening to them as well as the severity of how adverse those events would be (Warda & Bryant, 1998). ASD patients also tend to exaggerate information related to external harm, somatic stimuli, and social events in ways that suggest these things are harmful (Smith & Bryant, 2000). In terms of predicting later posttraumatic stress, negative appraisals shortly after the trauma are predictive of subsequent PTSD (Dunmore, Clark, & Ehlers, 2001; Engelhard, van den Hout, Arntz, & McNally, 2002; Halligan, Michael, Clark, & Ehlers, 2003; Kleim, Ehlers, & Glucksman, 2007; Mayou, Ehlers, & Bryant, 2002; Murray, Ehlers, & Mayou, 2002; Wikman, Molloy, Randall, & Steptoe, 2011). As distinct from the content of appraisals, people’s cognitive styles also predict subsequent functioning—for example, ruminating after a traumatic event is predictive of later PTSD (Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 2003; Kleim et al., 2007; Mayou et al., 2002). Ehlers and Clark (2000) also emphasized the importance of autobiographical memories, insofar as the trauma experience needs to be integrated into one’s normal autobiographical base for it to be contextualized and processed. It is proposed that the strong fear and arousal associated with trauma results in information being encoded in a predominantly sensory (and often visual) manner, which results in poor intentional memory of certain aspects of the trauma. For example, an assault victim may have recurring images of an attacker’s face but have patchy recall of the sequence of events as they occurred. Termed data-driven processing, these fragmented memories of the trauma purportedly maintain a sense of nowness in that the person recalls the memory as if it is still in present rather than being able to contextualize it as being oriented in the past. Consistent with this perspective, it has been found that data-driven processing is linked with intrusive memories and PTSD (Ehring, Ehlers, & Glucksman, 2008; Halligan, Clark, & Ehlers, 2002; Halligan et al., 2003). Further support for this proposal is found in acute stress populations. Data-driven processing in the month after trauma is linked with heart rate increase in response to trauma-related cues (Ehlers et al., 2010). Children with ASD report more sensory-laden memories of their trauma, which in turn mediate the relationship between perceived threat of a trauma and severity of ASD (Meiser-Stedman, Dalgleish, Smith, Yule, & Glucksman, 2007). Also consistent with cognitive models is evidence that trauma memories tend to be fragmented in both those with chronic PTSD (Amir, Stafford, Freshman, & Foa, 1998; Foa, Molnar, & Cashman, 1995; Foa & Riggs, 1993; van der Kolk & 167 Richard A. Bryant Fisler, 1995) and those with ASD (Harvey & Bryant, 1999b). It also has been shown that when people with ASD receive exposure therapy, the increased organization is associated with decreased dissociative content of their memories (Moulds & Bryant, 2005). Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Biological Models of ASD The other major model of ASD focuses on biological processes. In this context, the most influential model involves fear conditioning, which posits that the traumatic experience promotes noradrenergic and glucocorticoid activation, which in turn contributes to consolidation of trauma memories. Trauma researchers have relied on this model to postulate that trauma survivors respond to a trauma with extreme fear (this is the unconditioned response). The fear experienced at the time of trauma results in strong associative conditioning between the fear and many stimuli associated with the trauma (these are the conditioned stimuli). When one is confronted with these reminders of the trauma, conditioned responses are triggered, which can include the range of re-experiencing symptoms, such intrusive memories and physiological reactivity (Pitman, 1989; Rauch, Shin, & Phelps, 2006). Supporting this model is extremely robust evidence that emotional and physiological reactivity is activated in patients with PTSD when they are exposed to trauma reminders (Blanchard, Kolb, Gerardi, Ryan, & Pallmeyer, 1986; Orr et al., 1998; Orr, Pitman, Lasko, & Herz, 1993). Central to biological conceptualizations of recovery from a traumatic experience is the notion of extinction learning. This term refers to recurrent exposure to the conditioned stimuli but in the absence of any aversive outcomes—in such situations, there is new learning that the previously conditioned reminders now signal safely and thereby override initial conditioning (Milad, Rauch, Pitman, & Quirk, 2006). In most traumatic situations, people often report marked posttraumatic stress in the initial days after exposure; however, these reactions often abate in the days and weeks that follow. It is possible that during this period people are experiencing many trials of extinction learning such that they are being exposed to trauma reminders, 168 but the traumatic outcome is not occurring, and so there is new learning of safety. For example, motor vehicle accident survivors who initially are frightened by the sounds of traffic and smell of petrol may learn in the weeks after the accident that each time they drive and are exposed to each of these reminders that they do not have an accident. In this sense, PTSD is regarded in conditioning models as an example of failed extinction learning because the minority of people who go onto develop PTSD apparently do not learn that the conditioned stimuli no longer need to be feared (Myers & Davis, 2007). As noted earlier, people follow diverse trajectories of posttraumatic stress response over time, with some people showing exacerbation of their condition. One feature of conditioning models that may account for this is fear reinstatement, which involves the return of fear reactions after initial conditioning has been extinguished because of reexposure to the initial unconditioned stimulus conditions (Bouton, 2004). This process underscores the finding that initial conditioning is not erased with extinction learning and can be reinstated if the appropriate conditions are met. Following from many experimental studies attesting to the process of fear reinstatement, this mechanism can explain symptom return in anxiety (Norrholm et al., 2006). Furthermore, this process may offer an explanation for the finding that posttraumatic stressors can contribute to delayed-onset PTSD (Bryant, O’Donnell, Creamer, McFarlane, & Silove, 2013; Smid et al., 2012). This occurs commonly in the acute period after trauma because people are exposed to many related stressors, such as hospital visits, legal inquiries, and confrontations with people involved in the traumatic event. Much research has explored the role of fearconditioning processes during the acute period after trauma. A series of studies have assessed resting heart rate immediately after trauma exposure because this can reflect sympathetic arousal and may be one sign of the level of fear conditioning the trauma survivor experiences. Across numerous studies with both adults and children, it has been found that higher resting heart rates in the initial days after trauma exposure predict subsequent PTSD (Bryant, Creamer, O’Donnell, Silove, & McFarlane, 2008; Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Kassam-Adams, Garcia-España, Fein, & Winston, 2005; Shalev, Freedman, et al., 1998). This pattern has even been found in people who suffer severe brain injury and who are amnesic of the traumatic event—suggesting that fear conditioning may occur at levels below full awareness (Bryant, Marosszeky, Crooks, & Gurka, 2004). Further support of the role of increased arousal after trauma is that elevated respiration rate in the first 48 hours predicts PTSD (Bryant, Creamer, et al., 2008). More direct support for the process of fear conditioning is a finding that phasic increases in heart rate in response to trauma reminders are a stronger predictor of PTSD than resting state heart rate (O’Donnell, Creamer, Elliott, & Bryant, 2007). In the context of extreme arousal after trauma, it is worth noting that nearly all people with ASD reported having panic symptoms during the trauma, and most of these persist in the weeks after the event (Nixon & Bryant, 2003). It seems that people are particularly vulnerable to the effects of trauma exposure if they were predisposed to enhanced conditioning or impaired extinction before the trauma exposure. In an early prospective study, newly recruited firefighters who at the time of assessment were in class-based training and had not commenced active duty were assessed on paradigms relevant to conditioning models. This study found that recruits’ skin conductance and eye blink startle reactions in response to startling tones predicted their level of acute stress symptoms within a month of subsequently being exposed to a traumatic event as a firefighter (Guthrie & Bryant, 2005). This finding may suggest the tendency to be reactive to stressors, which may increase vulnerability to fear conditioning. In a related study, this research assessed extinction learning before trauma exposure by having the recruits receive electric shocks when they were presented with a particularly colored shape but not when presented with a different color. They subsequently were presented with extinction trials; level of PTSD 2 years after commencing firefighting duties was predicted by impoverished extinction learning as a recruit (Guthrie & Bryant, 2006). This finding appears robust because it has been replicated both in first responders and military personnel who were assessed before exposure to traumatic events (Lommen, Engelhard, Sijbrandij, van den Hout, & Hermans, 2013; Orr et al., 2012). These data point to individual differences in extinction learning being pivotal in modulating vulnerability to PTSD. When humans are exposed to a traumatic event, they respond with two distinct stress systems. One is the fast-acting autonomic nervous system, and the other is the slower-acting hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal (HPA) axis. An initial sympathetic response secretes adrenaline and noradrenaline, which leads to increases in heart rate, respiration rate, and blood pressure; these responses are needed to engage in either fight or flight from a threat. There is also a slower HPA response, in which there is a cascade of HPA axis activity involving the hypothalamus secreting corticotropin-releasing hormone, which in turn leads to the anterior pituitary generating adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH). ACTH then elicits secretion of glucocorticoids (this is cortisol in humans), and importantly, this provides negative feedback to the hypothalamus, which inhibits further activation of corticotropin-releasing hormone (Radley et al., 2011). The role of the latter reaction is to restabilize the system by promoting homeostasis once the threat has passed. Many have argued that PTSD is characterized by diminished feedback emanating from the HPA axis, thereby leading to prolonged arousal in PTSD, with evidence that PTSD patients have lower cortisol levels (Yehuda, Boisoneau, Mason, & Giller, 1993; Yehuda et al., 1995; Yehuda et al., 1990). Supporting this proposal is that some evidence exists that lower cortisol levels shortly after trauma predict later PTSD (Delahanty, Raimonde, & Spoonster, 2000; Resnick, Yehuda, Pitman, & Foy, 1995). This evidence is not straightforward, however, because evidence also exists that acute levels of cortisol after trauma can be higher than in those who later develop PTSD (Delahanty, Nugent, Christopher, & Walsh, 2005). Further support for this notion comes from some research that has attempted to prevent PTSD by enhancing cortisol levels in the acute period after trauma exposure. This line of research builds on animal studies that have found that administering hydrocortisone to rats shortly after stressing them results in less fear behavior than administering a 169 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Richard A. Bryant placebo (Cohen, Matar, Buskila, Kaplan, & Zohar, 2008). Consistent with this finding, administering cortisol shortly after an experimentally induced stressor has been shown to result in fewer traumatic memories (Schelling et al., 2001; Schelling et al., 2004). In terms of clinical applications, one early study has found that giving trauma-exposed survivors cortisol within hours of trauma exposure can limit subsequent PTSD (Zohar et al., 2011). Although this research is new and has yet to be replicated, it does seem that the glucocorticoid system plays an important role in acute traumatic stress, and it contributes to ongoing arousal. The noradrenergic system is critically important in the acute stress response. One of the reasons for its pivotal role is that it underpins consolidation of emotional memories. In animal studies, it has been shown repeatedly that levels of adrenomedullary stress hormones during or immediately after learning are associated with enhanced memory (Liang, Juler, & McGaugh, 1986; Sternberg, Isaacs, Gold, & McGaugh, 1985). Levels of endogenous noradrenergic activation also are associated with memory for emotional material in humans (Segal & Cahill, 2009). Moreover, administering humans yohimbine (which is an adrenergic agonist) leads to greater memory for emotional stimuli (Southwick et al., 2002), yet β-adrenergic blockades decrease amygdala activation and impair memory for emotional (but not neutral) stimuli (Cahill & van Stegeren, 2003; O’Carroll, Drysdale, Cahill, Shajahan, & Ebmeier, 1999). Some evidence also shows that epinephrine and norepinephrine 1 month after trauma exposure are associated with PTSD severity in men (Hawk, Dougall, Ursano, & Baum, 2000), although another study has not replicated this finding (Videlock et al., 2008). This line of research has led to attempts to limit PTSD development by reducing noradrenergic release in the acute period after trauma exposure. Pitman and colleagues (2002) administered propranolol or placebo within 6 hours of trauma exposure to people admitted to an emergency department; although patients who received propranolol did not display different PTSD levels relative to placebo 3 months later, those who initially received propranolol displayed less reactivity to 170 trauma reminders 3 months later (Pitman et al., 2002). Consistent with this result, another study increased the dosage of propranolol to 240 mg a day for 19 days, commencing within 12 hours (E. A. Hoge et al., 2012); with this approach, it was found that when analyses were restricted to participants who complied with medication, there was less psychophysiological responding to reminders in those who received propranolol than among those receiving a placebo. A subsequent uncontrolled study in France found that trauma survivors administered propranolol immediately after trauma had fewer PTSD symptoms 2 months later (Vaiva et al., 2003). The role of propranolol in limiting subsequent PTSD seems complicated, however, because several studies have not reported significant findings. One study of medical records found that propranolol given in the first month after burn trauma did not reduce ASD symptoms in children (Sharp, Thomas, Rosenberg, Rosenberg, & Meyer, 2010). A controlled trial of patients in the emergency department found that acute propranolol did not limit subsequent PTSD development when administered within 48 hours after trauma (Stein, Kerridge, Dimsdale, & Hoyt, 2007). It also may be that the impact of stress hormones on emotional memory consolidation is influenced by gender, as one study found that propranolol had less benefit in female children than male children (Nugent et al., 2010). Further evidence for the importance of noradrenergic response in the acute phase after trauma comes from research using morphine to limit development of PTSD. Studies with animals indicate that morphine injections into the amygdala impair acquisition of fear conditioning (Clark, Jović, Ornellas, & Weller, 1972) and also impair memory for fear conditioning (McNally & Westbrook, 2003). In the context of human trauma, an early study of child burn victims found that that morphine dose in the acute period was inversely related to severity of subsequent PTSD (Saxe et al., 2001). Subsequent studies found that administering morphine in the 48 hours immediately after trauma also was associated with lower subsequent PTSD levels in injured adults (Bryant, Creamer, O’Donnell, Silove, & McFarlane, 2009) and children (Nixon et al., 2010). A large study of U.S. troops deployed in Afghanistan found Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder that morphine use following injury in theater was linked to reduced PTSD (Holbrook, Galarneau, Dye, Quinn, & Dougherty, 2010). None of these studies represent controlled trials, so caution when interpreting any causal relationship is essential; however, these reports are consistent with the notion that noradrenergic response is highly relevant in acute stress reactions. Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Assessment of ASD Since the introduction of the ASD diagnosis, several assessment instruments have been introduced to index ASD symptoms. A structured clinical interview, the Acute Stress Disorder Interview (ASDI), was developed based on the initial DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) items (Bryant, Harvey, Dang, & Sackville, 1998). This interview contained 19 dichotomously scored items based on the DSM–IV dissociative (Cluster B, 5 items), re-experiencing (Cluster C, 4 items), avoidance (Cluster D, 4 items), and arousal (Cluster E, 6 items) symptoms. It possessed good internal consistency (r = .90), test–retest reliability (r = .88), sensitivity (91%) and specificity (93%) relative to independent clinician diagnosis of ASD, based on DSM–IV criteria (Bryant, Harvey, Dang, & Sackville, 1998). Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the structure of the ASDI was best described by the four ASD symptom clusters in the DSM–IV (Brooks et al., 2008). Following the changes to the ASD diagnosis in DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), the interview was modified so that the symptoms matched the DSM–5 criteria. The DSM–5 scoring requires 9 of the possible 14 symptoms to be satisfied, as described by DSM–5. In terms of self-report measures, the earliest measure was the Stanford Acute Stress Reaction Questionnaire (SASRQ; Cardeña, Classen, & Spiegel, 1991). This measure has gone through several iterations and has included dissociative, intrusive, somatic anxiety, hyperarousal, attention disturbance, and sleep disturbance symptoms (Cardeña & Spiegel, 1993; Classen, Koopman, Hales, & Spiegel, 1998; Koopman, Classen, & Spiegel, 1994). Each item asks respondents to indicate the frequency of each symptom on a 6-point Likert scale from 0 (not experienced) to 5 (very often experienced) that can occur during and immediately following a trauma. It has been shown to possess high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .90 and .91 for dissociative and anxiety symptoms, respectively) and concurrent validity with scores on the Impact of Event Scale (r = .52–.69; Cardeña et al., 2000; Koopman et al., 1994). The more recent version of the SASRQ is a 30-item self-report inventory that encompasses each of the DSM–IV ASD symptoms (Cardeña et al., 2000). The other major self-report measure is the Acute Stress Disorder Scale (ASDS), which is based on the same items and wording as the ASDI. Each item is scored on a 5-point scale to index symptom severity (Bryant, Moulds, & Guthrie, 2000). Test–retest reliability between 2 and 7 days was strong (r = .94). Predictive ability of the ASDS has been reported using a cutoff score of 56 on the ASDS, predicting 91% of those who developed PTSD and 93% of those who did not. ASDS also has been updated to incorporate DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) changes. Other measures have been developed to assess acute stress reactions. Depression is a common posttraumatic stress response (Bryant et al., 2010; Shalev, Sahar, et al., 1998), and so one measure was developed to screen for depression and acute stress in the period shortly after trauma. The Posttraumatic Adjustment Scale (PAS; O’Donnell et al., 2008) includes 10 items that are each scored on a 5-point scale with the goal of identifying recently trauma-exposed people who are at risk of developing PTSD or depression. The PAS includes 10 items that are each scored on a 5-point scale. This scale was developed such that a summation of scores of all 10 items was predictive of PTSD (PAS–P), and the summation of 5 of these items was predictive of subsequent depression (PAS–D). In terms of predicting subsequent PTSD 12 months after trauma, the PAS–P has been shown to have strong sensitivity (.82) and specificity (.84), but modest positive predictive power of (.28). In terms of predicting subsequent depression, the PAS–D had a sensitivity of .72 and a specificity of .74, and positive predictive power of .30 (O’Donnell et al., 2008). 171 Richard A. Bryant Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. TREATING ASD The most compelling evidence for effective treatment for ASD involves trauma-focused cognitive– behavior therapy (CBT). Most treatment guidelines around the world recommend it as a frontline treatment for the condition (Foa, Keane, Friedman, & Cohen, 2009; Forbes et al., 2007). In short, this treatment approach begins with education about common psychological responses to trauma and then focuses on three major strategies: anxiety management, exposure, and cognitive restructuring. Education usually attempts to normalize the trauma response by informing the patient about the common reactions following a traumatic event and discusses the way in which the core symptoms will be treated during the course of therapy. Anxiety management strategies aim to reduce arousal through related techniques, such as breathing retraining, relaxation skills, and self-talk. Exposure therapy can involve both imaginal and in vivo exposure. Imaginal exposure (also called prolonged exposure) requires the patient to vividly imagine their traumatic experience for prolonged periods, usually for at least 30 minutes. The therapist asks the patient to provide a narrative of their traumatic experience in a way that emphasizes all relevant details with the goal of the heightening emotional engagement. In vivo involves graded exposure to feared stimuli in which the patient is asked to remain in proximity to mildly fearful reminders of the trauma and then to repeat this exercise with increasingly fearful situations until they feel comfortable with most reminders of their experience. Cognitive restructuring teaches patients to identify and evaluate the evidence for negative automatic thoughts, with particular focus on beliefs about the trauma, the self, the world, and the future (Foa, Rothbaum, & Steketee, 1993). The fundamental basis of this approach draws on both fear conditioning and cognitive models. From a conditioning perspective, it is posited that recovery from trauma involves extinction learning, including repeated exposure to stimuli that are associated with the trauma reminders until new learning is achieved that trauma reminders is no longer signaling danger (Davis & Myers, 2002). Through recurrent reliving of 172 the trauma memory in the safety of the therapy context as well as approaching feared stimuli during in vivo exposure, it is hoped that the patient diminishes the conditioned response. In terms of cognitive processes, the emphasis is on altering appraisals so that the person has a more realistic interpretation of their role in the trauma and of possible future harm (Ehlers & Clark, 2000; for a detailed discussion of prolonged exposure, see Volume 2, Chapter 8, this handbook). There is actually a long history of early interventions for posttraumatic stress responses. More than 30 years ago, people began to adapt behavioral principles to limit subsequent PTSD reactions. These interventions showed some promising results, but their conclusions were restricted by methodological shortcomings (Brom, Kleber, & Hofman, 1993; Kilpatrick & Veronen, 1983). Exemplifying this approach was a trial in which four sessions of CBT were given to assault victims shortly after assault and their responses were compared with others (who were not randomized to this condition) who received repeated assessments (Foa, Hearst Ikeda, & Perry, 1995). Although 10% of those who received CBT met criteria for PTSD at 2 months, 70% of the control group had PTSD; there were no differences between groups at 5 months (although the CBT group was less depressed); one implication of this study is that CBT may accelerate natural recovery from trauma. In a better-controlled trial by the same research team, several weeks after their trauma, assault survivors with acute PTSD were randomly allocated to four weekly sessions of CBT, repeated assessment, or supportive counseling (SC; Foa, Zoellner, & Feeny, 2006). Nine months later approximately 30% of participants in each group met criteria for PTSD. Again, this study may not have found a relative advantage in early provision of CBT because of the focus on PTSD rather than ASD patients. One common theme in these attempts at secondary prevention of PTSD was that they offered treatment to all distressed trauma survivors in the acute phase, thereby enhancing the likelihood that the effects of intervention would be confused with natural remission of symptoms that can occur in the weeks after exposure (Bryant, 2003). In the first study to focus on trauma survivors who met criteria for ASD, motor vehicle accident Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder or nonsexual assault survivors with ASD were randomly allocated to either CBT or SC (Bryant, Harvey, Dang, Sackville, & Basten, 1998). The CBT program included education about posttraumatic reactions, relaxation training, cognitive restructuring, and imaginal and in vivo exposure that was conducted in five 1.5-hour weekly individual therapy sessions. The SC condition included trauma education and more general problem-solving skills training that occurred at the same frequency as CBT. Six months after treatment, there were fewer participants in the CBT group (20%) who met diagnostic criteria for PTSD compared with SC participants (67%). Extending this work was another study that attempted to disentangle the most active ingredients of CBT for ASD in which 45 civilian trauma survivors with ASD were randomly allocated to five sessions of either (a) CBT (prolonged exposure, cognitive therapy, anxiety management), (b) prolonged exposure combined with cognitive therapy, or (c) supportive counseling (Bryant, Sackville, Dang, Moulds, & Guthrie, 1999). Six months after treatment, patients in both CBT programs had less PTSD (20%) than those in the SC condition (67%). This finding indicates that exposure and cognitive restructuring can achieve comparable therapy gains without the addition of anxiety management strategies. A follow-up that assessed the patients who participated in these two studies 2 years after treatment found that the treatment gains of those who received CBT were maintained 4 years after treatment (Bryant, Moulds, & Nixon, 2003). Other studies by the same research group provide further support for trauma-focused CBT for ASD. Some commentators had argued that because of the pivotal role of dissociative responses in ASD and the potential for this feature of the condition to impede recovery, hypnosis may be useful to facilitate the emotional processing in the treatment of ASD (Spiegel, 1996). Accordingly, a study was conducted in 89 ASD patients following civilian trauma were randomly allocated to either CBT, CBT associated with hypnosis, or supportive counseling (Bryant, Moulds, Guthrie, & Nixon, 2005). To facilitate emotional processing, a hypnotic suggestion was provided immediately before imaginal exposure that the patient would engage fully with the emotional memory. In terms of treatment completers, more participants in the supportive counseling condition (57%) met PTSD criteria at 6-month follow-up than those in the CBT (21%) or CBT plus hypnosis (22%) condition. Participants in the CBT plus hypnosis condition reported fewer re-experiencing symptoms at posttreatment than those in the CBT condition, possibly suggesting that hypnosis may have facilitated treatment gains. One of the most topical issues in the field of traumatic stress in recent years has been the impact of mild traumatic brain injury on stress responses, with numerous accounts in military (C. W. Hoge et al., 2008) and civilian (Bryant et al., 2010) settings indicating that sustaining a mild brain injury can increase the risk for PTSD. In a small pilot study (but the only one available to date), 24 ASD participants who sustained a mild traumatic brain injury following motor vehicle accidents were randomized to CBT or SC (Bryant, Moulds, Guthrie, & Nixon, 2003). This study was particularly interested in testing whether CBT was efficacious in treating this condition because, by definition, these patients are at least partially amnesic of their traumatic experience (Bryant, 2001). Consistent with previous findings, more participants receiving SC (58%) met criteria for PTSD at 6-month follow-up than those receiving CBT (8%). One of the issues that clinicians often consider is the relative weight that should be given to exposure versus cognitive restructuring in the course of treatment. To address this issue, one study randomized 90 trauma survivors to either five weekly sessions of (a) imaginal and in vivo exposure, (b) cognitive restructuring, or (c) assessment only (Bryant, Mastrodomenico, et al., 2008). This study found that immediately after treatment and also at the 6-month follow-up, those patients who received exposure therapy had lower levels of PTSD, depression, and anxiety than those in the other conditions. This finding provides strong support for the argument that emotional processing of the trauma memories is beneficial if ASD patients are to achieve optimal treatment response. It is worth noting that other studies have been conducted that have not adhered strictly to the ASD criteria, but their findings are relevant to 173 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Richard A. Bryant considering the treatment of acute stress. A large trial conducted in Jerusalem asked whether early intervention led to better results than later intervention (Shalev et al., 2012). This study randomized 242 patients admitted to an emergency department, and who met criteria for either full or subsyndromal ASD to either prolonged exposure, cognitive restructuring, wait list (who were then randomized to exposure or cognitive restructuring after 12 weeks), escitalopram (a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor [SSRI]), or placebo (Shalev et al., 2012). Nine months after treatment, PTSD rates were comparable across exposure (21%) and restructuring (22%) conditions, relative to the much higher rates in the SSRI (42%) and placebo (47%) conditions. A noteworthy finding to emerge from this study was that there were no long-term differences between participants who received the early or later provision of CBT. This outcome is consistent with the observation that effect sizes found in most CBT treatments of ASD or early PTSD are comparable to effect sizes of CBT for chronic PTSD (Foa et al., 2009). In terms of other controlled trials that have applied CBT to patients with acute PTSD, results have been mixed. In a British study, 152 traumatic injury survivors were randomized to receive four sessions of CBT or no intervention several weeks after civilian trauma (Bisson, Shepherd, Joy, Probert, & Newcombe, 2004). Although there were no group differences in anxiety or depression 1 year after treatment, patients who received CBT had fewer PTSD symptoms. A less encouraging finding was reported in a Dutch study in which 143 patients with acute PTSD were randomized within 3 months of trauma exposure to either four sessions of CBT or wait list (Sijbrandij et al., 2007). Although CBT resulted in greater PTSD reduction after treatment, this difference was not evident 4 months later. Mixed findings emerged from a study of 40 recent trauma victims who met criteria for ASD and were randomized to receive three sessions of psychoeducation and progressive relaxation and were randomized to additionally receive either exposure or supportive counseling (Freyth, Elsesser, Lohrmann, & Sartory, 2010). Although both groups displayed comparable symptom reduction 4 years after treatment, those participants who received 174 exposure displayed reduced heart rate increase in response to trauma reminders relative to those who received supportive counseling. Another British study tested whether providing therapist-delivered CBT differed from a self-help program in which patients were given the same elements in the form of a treatment manual. To reduce the potential confound of natural remission of symptoms, this study had recently trauma-exposed patients complete a 3-week self-monitoring phase before enrolling in the trial, and if they displayed acute PTSD at the end of the period, they were randomized to receive either (a) up to 12 weekly sessions of CBT, (b) a self-help condition (one session with a clinician and a self-help booklet), or (c) repeated assessments of PTSD symptoms (Ehlers, Clark, et al., 2003). Consistent with the cognitive model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), this adaptation of CBT placed much emphasis on altering maladaptive appraisals, but it also incorporated exposure elements. This study found at both posttreatment and at follow-up that CBT delivered by a therapist achieved greater reductions of PTSD relative to both the self-help and repeated assessment conditions. Reflecting the overall evidence reviewed in this chapter is the conclusion of a metaanalysis of four studies of CBT for ASD relative to supportive counseling. This meta-analysis reported the relative risk for a PTSD diagnosis was (.36; 95% confidence interval [CI], .17–.78), supporting the evidence for the utility of brief CBT for ASD (Kornør et al., 2008). Notably, a distinct meta-analysis that focused on a larger range of studies of early intervention, including studies with patients meeting criteria for ASD or acute PTSD, came to a similar conclusion (Roberts, Kitchiner, Kenardy, & Bisson, 2009). Other psychotherapeutic methods have been adopted to prevent PTSD, but these have tried to abort the psychopathological process in the early phases of trauma response. These usually commence with hours or days of trauma exposure and so they do not use ASD as the entry criteria to the study. One such study provided just two sessions of CBT intended to promote adaptive memory reconstruction in 17 accident survivors who in the emergency room had heart rates of at least 94 beats per minute Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (Gidron et al., 2001); this criterion was based on the work reviewed earlier indicating that elevated heart rate levels in the acute period is predictive of subsequent PTSD (Bryant, Harvey, Guthrie, & Moulds, 2000; Shalev, Freedman, et al., 1998). This study provided the telephone-administered CBT 1–3 days after the accident and found that patients who received CBT had greater reductions in severity of PTSD symptoms 3–4 months after the trauma than did those who received two sessions of supportive listening. In a similar vein, another study randomized 137 patients admitted to the emergency room to either three sessions of prolonged exposure or an assessment control condition (Rothbaum et al., 2012). The imaginal exposure therapy commenced in the emergency room and was repeated 1 and 2 weeks following the initial session. Patients in the exposure condition had significantly lower PTSD scores 12 weeks following the injury relative to those who received the assessment alone. This is an interesting study because it not only shows that early intervention that promotes emotional processing can be efficacious but also highlights that commencing this intervention soon after trauma can be done safely. In contrast to the wealth of studies in adults with ASD, there is relatively little evidence when it comes to children. In one study, 30 child survivors of assault with ASD were randomly allocated to either cognitive processing therapy or supportive counseling (Nixon, 2012). Cognitive processing therapy is a variant of CBT insofar as it requires the participant to write their traumatic experience and then devotes much therapy time on addressing unhelpful appraisals that the person has about their experience (Resick & Schnicke, 1992); there is considerable evidence for positive outcomes with this modality (Resick et al., 2008; Resick, Nishith, Weaver, Astin, & Feuer, 2002). This study of ASD found that at posttreatment and 6-month follow-up, both interventions were comparably effective. The generalizability of this study is clearly hampered by its small sample size, and so there is a need for replication with larger samples. Whereas we know quite a bit about psychotherapeutic interventions for ASD, relatively scare information pertains to pharmacological interventions. The previously reviewed Jerusalem study is presently the most robust pharmacological study in the literature. It suggests that (a) escitalopram does not perform markedly better than placebo, and (b) the use of an SSRI is inferior to that of trauma-focused psychotherapy for ASD. In terms of pharmacological interventions with children, one randomized trial reported that 7 days of treatment with imipramine was more effective in treating symptoms of ASD in 25 child and adolescent burn victims than chloral hydrate (Robert, Blakeney, Villarreal, Rosenberg, & Meyer, 1999). CONCLUSION The diagnosis of ASD has resulted in an explosion of novel research into the acute stress reactions after trauma, and our knowledge is much greater than what it was two decades ago. Since its introduction, our knowledge about trajectories of posttraumatic stress following the acute phase has grown enormously, which has challenged earlier views that a linear course of adaptation can be predicted in the acute phase. As we noted, longitudinal studies indicate that the course of posttraumatic recovery is complex and fluctuates over time (Bryant et al., 2013). Moreover, as growth mixture modeling has indicated, there are at least four pathways that people tend to follow after adversity (Bonanno, Kennedy, Galatzer-Levy, Lude, & Elfström, 2012; Bryant et al., 2015). These patterns challenge previous expectations that the diagnosis of ASD can accurately identify those at risk of PTSD in some linear way. Nonetheless, the current definition of ASD in DSM–5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) does seem to capture many of those who can benefit from interventions in the acute phase. As research continues to develop more novel psychotherapeutic and pharmacological methods to abort psychopathological responses, these early interventions hopefully will improve our current levels of success in secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress. References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. 175 Richard A. Bryant American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Amir, N., Stafford, J., Freshman, M. S., & Foa, E. B. (1998). Relationship between trauma narratives and trauma pathology. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 385–392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024415523495 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Armour, C., Elklit, A., & Shevlin, M. (2013). The latent structure of acute stress disorder: A posttraumatic stress disorder approach. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 5, 18–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024848 Armour, C., Shevlin, M., Elklit, A., & Mroczek, D. (2012). A latent growth mixture modeling approach to PTSD symptoms in rape victims. Traumatology, 18, 20–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534765610395627 Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 727–735. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053-198612000-00004 Bisson, J. I., Shepherd, J. P., Joy, D., Probert, R., & Newcombe, R. G. (2004). Early cognitive-behavioural therapy for post-traumatic stress symptoms after physical injury. Randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 63–69. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1192/bjp.184.1.63 Blanchard, E. B., Kolb, L. C., Gerardi, R. J., Ryan, P., & Pallmeyer, T. P. (1986). Cardiac response to relevant stimuli as an adjunctive tool for diagnosing posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans. Behavior Therapy, 17, 592–606. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/S0005-7894(86)80097-1 Bonanno, G. A., Ho, S. M., Chan, J. C., Kwong, R. S., Cheung, C. K., Wong, C. P., & Wong, V. C. (2008). Psychological resilience and dysfunction among hospitalized survivors of the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: A latent class approach. Health Psychology, 27, 659–667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.659 Bonanno, G. A., Kennedy, P., Galatzer-Levy, I. R., Lude, P., & Elfström, M. L. (2012). Trajectories of resilience, depression, and anxiety following spinal cord injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 57, 236–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029256 Bonanno, G. A., Mancini, A. D., Horton, J. L., Powell, T. M., Leardmann, C. A., Boyko, E. J., .!.!. Smith, T. C. (2012). Trajectories of trauma symptoms and resilience in deployed U.S. military service members: Prospective cohort study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 200, 317–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/ bjp.bp.111.096552 Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam combat veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 328–332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ ajp.149.3.328 Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., Rose, S., & Kirk, M. (1999). Acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in victims of violent crime. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 360–366. Briere, J., Scott, C., & Weathers, F. (2005). Peritraumatic and persistent dissociation in the presumed etiology of PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 2295–2301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi. ajp.162.12.2295 Brom, D., Kleber, R. J., & Hofman, M. C. (1993). Victims of traffic accidents: Incidence and prevention of post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 131–140. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/1097-4679(199303)49:2<131::AIDJCLP2270490202>3.0.CO;2-2 Brooks, R., Bryant, R. A., Silove, D., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., McFarlane, A. C., & Marmar, C. R. (2009). The latent structure of the Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22, 153–157. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/jts.20414 Brooks, R., Silove, D., Bryant, R., O’Donnell, M., Creamer, M., & McFarlane, A. (2008). A confirmatory factor analysis of the acute stress disorder interview. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 352–355. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/jts.20333 Bryant, R. A. (2001). Posttraumatic stress disorder and traumatic brain injury: can they co-exist? Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 931–948. http://dx.doi.org/ S027273580000074X Bryant, R. A. (2003). Early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 789–795. http://dx.doi.org/S0006322302018954 Bryant, R. A. (2011). Acute stress disorder as a predictor of posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72, 233–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09r05072blu Bryant, R. A., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Silove, D., & McFarlane, A. C. (2008). A multisite study of initial respiration rate and heart rate as predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 1694–1701. http://dx.doi.org/ ej07m04024 Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning and Memory, 11, 485–494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.78804 Bryant, R. A., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Silove, D., & McFarlane, A. C. (2009). A study of the protective function of acute morphine administration on subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 438–440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.biopsych.2008.10.032 Bremner, J. D., Southwick, S., Brett, E., Fontana, A., Rosenheck, R., & Charney, D. S. (1992). Bryant, R. A., Friedman, M. J., Spiegel, D., Ursano, R., & Strain, J. (2011). A review of acute stress disorder in 176 Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM–5. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 802–817. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20737 Bryant, R. A., & Harvey, A. G. (1997). Acute stress disorder: A critical review of diagnostic issues. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 757–773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00052-4 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Bryant, R. A., & Harvey, A. G. (2000). New DSM–IV diagnosis of acute stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1889–1890. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1889 Bryant, R. A., Harvey, A. G., Dang, S. T., & Sackville, T. (1998). Assessing acute stress disorder: Psychometric properties of a structured clinical interview. Psychological Assessment, 10, 215–220. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/1040-3590.10.3.215 Bryant, R. A., Harvey, A. G., Dang, S. T., Sackville, T., & Basten, C. (1998). Treatment of acute stress disorder: A comparison of cognitive-behavioral therapy and supportive counseling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 862–866. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-006X.66.5.862 Bryant, R. A., Harvey, A. G., Guthrie, R. M., & Moulds, M. L. (2000). A prospective study of psychophysiological arousal, acute stress disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 341–344. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.341 Bryant, R. A., Marosszeky, J. E., Crooks, J., & Gurka, J. A. (2004). Elevated resting heart rate as a predictor of posttraumatic stress disorder after severe traumatic brain injury. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 760–761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000138121.13198.84 Bryant, R. A., Mastrodomenico, J., Felmingham, K. L., Hopwood, S., Kenny, L., Kandris, E., .!.!. Creamer, M. (2008). Treatment of acute stress disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 659–667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.65.6.659 disorder: a four-year follow-up. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 489–494. http://dx.doi.org/ S0005796702001791 Bryant, R. A., Nickerson, A., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Forbes, D., Galatzer-Levy, I., .!.!. Silove, D. (2015). Trajectory of post-traumatic stress following traumatic injury: 6-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 206, 417–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/ bjp.bp.114.145516 Bryant, R. A., O’Donnell, M. L., Creamer, M., McFarlane, A. C., Clark, C. R., & Silove, D. (2010). The psychiatric sequelae of traumatic injury. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 312–320. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050617 Bryant, R. A., O’Donnell, M. L., Creamer, M., McFarlane, A. C., & Silove, D. (2013). A multisite analysis of the fluctuating course of posttraumatic stress disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 839–846. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1137 Bryant, R. A., Sackville, T., Dang, S. T., Moulds, M., & Guthrie, R. (1999). Treating acute stress disorder: An evaluation of cognitive behavior therapy and supportive counseling techniques. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1780–1786. Bryant, R. A., Salmon, K., Sinclair, E., & Davidson, P. (2007). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in injured children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20, 1075–1079. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20282 Cahill, L., & van Stegeren, A. (2003). Sex-related impairment of memory for emotional events with beta-adrenergic blockade. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 79, 81–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S1074-7427(02)00019-9 Cardeña, E., Classen, C., & Spiegel, D. (1991). Stanford Acute Stress Reaction Questionnaire. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Medical School. Bryant, R. A., Moulds, M., Guthrie, R., & Nixon, R. D. (2003). Treating acute stress disorder following mild traumatic brain injury. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 585–587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ appi.ajp.160.3.585 Cardeña, E., Koopman, C., Classen, C., Waelde, L. C., & Spiegel, D. (2000). Psychometric properties of the Stanford Acute Stress Reaction Questionnaire (SASRQ): A valid and reliable measure of acute stress. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 719–734. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1007822603186 Bryant, R. A., Moulds, M. L., & Guthrie, R. M. (2000). Acute Stress Disorder Scale: A self-report measure of acute stress disorder. Psychological Assessment, 12, 61–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.12.1.61 Cardeña, E., & Spiegel, D. (1993). Dissociative reactions to the San Francisco Bay Area earthquake of 1989. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 474–478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.150.3.474 Bryant, R. A., Moulds, M. L., Guthrie, R. M., & Nixon, R. D. V. (2005). The additive benefit of hypnosis and cognitive-behavioral therapy in treating acute stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 334–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0022-006X.73.2.334 Clark, A. G., Jović, R., Ornellas, M. R., & Weller, M. (1972). Brain microsomal protein kinase in the chronically morphinized rat. Biochemical Pharmacology, 21, 1989–1990. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0006-2952(72)90011-1 Bryant, R. A., Moulds, M. L., & Nixon, R. V. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy of acute stress Classen, C., Koopman, C., Hales, R., & Spiegel, D. (1998). Acute stress disorder as a predictor of posttraumatic stress symptoms. American Journal of 177 Richard A. Bryant Psychiatry, 155, 620–624. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ ajp.155.5.620 Cohen, H., Matar, M. A., Buskila, D., Kaplan, Z., & Zohar, J. (2008). Early post-stressor intervention with highdose corticosterone attenuates posttraumatic stress response in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 708–717. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.025 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Coons, P. M., Bowman, E. S., Pellow, T. A., & Schneider, P. (1989). Post-traumatic aspects of the treatment of victims of sexual abuse and incest. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 325–335. Dalgleish, T., Meiser-Stedman, R., Kassam-Adams, N., Ehlers, A., Winston, F., Smith, P., .!.!. Yule, W. (2008). Predictive validity of acute stress disorder in children and adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 392–393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.040451 Davidson, J. R., Kudler, H. S., Saunders, W. B., & Smith, R. D. (1990). Symptom and comorbidity patterns in World War II and Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 162–170. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0010-440X(90)90020-S Davis, M., & Myers, K. M. (2002). The role of glutamate and gamma-aminobutyric acid in fear extinction: Clinical implications for exposure therapy. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 998–1007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0006-3223(02)01507-X Delahanty, D. L., Nugent, N. R., Christopher, N. C., & Walsh, M. (2005). Initial urinary epinephrine and cortisol levels predict acute PTSD symptoms in child trauma victims. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 121–128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen. 2004.06.004 Delahanty, D. L., Raimonde, A. J., & Spoonster, E. (2000). Initial posttraumatic urinary cortisol levels predict subsequent PTSD symptoms in motor vehicle accident victims. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 940–947. http://dx.doi.org/S0006-3223(00)00896-9 Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (1997). Cognitive factors in persistent versus recovered posttraumatic stress disorder after physical or sexual assault: A pilot study. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25, 147–159. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1017/S135246580001835X Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2001). A prospective investigation of the role of cognitive factors in persistent posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1063–1084. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0005-7967(00)00088-7 Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0005-7967(99)00123-0 178 Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., Fennell, M., Herbert, C., & Mayou, R. (2003). A randomized controlled trial of cognitive therapy, a self-help booklet, and repeated assessments as early interventions for posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 1024–1032. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.10.1024 Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A., & Bryant, B. (1998). Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 508–519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.3.508 Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A., & Bryant, B. (2003). Cognitive predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder in children: Results of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00126-7 Ehlers, A., & Steil, R. (1995). Maintenance of intrusive memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: A cognitive approach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 217–249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ S135246580001585X Ehlers, A., Suendermann, O., Boellinghaus, I., Vossbeck-Elsebusch, A., Gamer, M., Briddon, E., .!.!. Glucksman, E. (2010). Heart rate responses to standardized trauma-related pictures in acute posttraumatic stress disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78, 27–34. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.04.009 Ehring, T., Ehlers, A., & Glucksman, E. (2008). Do cognitive models help in predicting the severity of posttraumatic stress disorder, phobia, and depression after motor vehicle accidents? A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 219–230. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.219 Engelhard, I. M., van den Hout, M. A., Arntz, A., & McNally, R. J. (2002). A longitudinal study of “intrusion-based reasoning” and posttraumatic stress disorder after exposure to a train disaster. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1415–1424. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00018-9 Foa, E. B., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Tolin, D. F., & Orsillo, S. M. (1999). The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 11, 303–314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 1040-3590.11.3.303 Foa, E. B., Hearst-Ikeda, D., & Perry, K. J. (1995). Evaluation of a brief cognitive-behavioral program for the prevention of chronic PTSD in recent assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 948–955. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022006X.63.6.948 Foa, E. B., Keane, T. M., Friedman, M. J., & Cohen, J. A. (Eds.). (2009). Effective treatments for PTSD: Practice guidelines from the International Society of Traumatic Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Stress Studies (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Foa, E. B., Molnar, C., & Cashman, L. (1995). Change in rape narratives during exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 675–690. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ jts.2490080409 Foa, E. B., & Riggs, D. S. (1993). Post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. In J. Oldham, M. B. Riba, & A. Tasman (Eds.), American Psychiatric Press review of psychiatry (Vol. 12, pp. 273–303). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, B. O., & Steketee, G. S. (1993). Treatment of rape victims. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8, 256–276. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1177/088626093008002006 Foa, E. B., Zoellner, L. A., & Feeny, N. C. (2006). An evaluation of three brief programs for facilitating recovery after assault. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 29–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20096 Forbes, D., Creamer, M., Phelps, A., Bryant, R., McFarlane, A., Devilly, G. J., .!.!. Newton, S. (2007). Australian guidelines for the treatment of adults with acute stress disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 637–648. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1080/00048670701449161 Freyth, C., Elsesser, K., Lohrmann, T., & Sartory, G. (2010). Effects of additional prolonged exposure to psychoeducation and relaxation in acute stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 909–917. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.016 Gidron, Y., Gal, R., Freedman, S., Twiser, I., Lauden, A., Snir, Y., & Benjamin, J. (2001). Translating research findings to PTSD prevention: Results of a randomized-controlled pilot study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 773–780. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1023/A:1013046322993 Guthrie, R. M., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). Auditory startle response in firefighters before and after trauma exposure. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 283–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi. ajp.162.2.283 Guthrie, R. M., & Bryant, R. A. (2006). Extinction learning before trauma and subsequent posttraumatic stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 307–311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000208629. 67653.cc Halligan, S. L., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2002). Cognitive processing, memory, and the development of PTSD symptoms: Two experimental analogue studies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 73–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0005-7916(02)00014-9 Halligan, S. L., Michael, T., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder following assault: The role of cognitive processing, trauma memory, and appraisals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 419–431. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-006X.71.3.419 Hansen, M., Armour, C., Wang, L., Elklit, A., & Bryant, R. A. (2015). Assessing possible DSM–5 ASD subtypes in a sample of victims meeting caseness for DSM–5 ASD based on self-report following multiple forms of traumatic exposure. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 31, 84–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.janxdis.2015.02.005 Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A prospective evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 507–512. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.507 Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1999a). Dissociative symptoms in acute stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 673–680. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1023/A:1024773202939 Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1999b). A qualitative investigation of the organization of traumatic memories. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 401–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466599162999 Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (2002). Acute stress disorder: A synthesis and critique. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 886–902. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00332909.128.6.886 Hawk, L. W., Dougall, A. L., Ursano, R. J., & Baum, A. (2000). Urinary catecholamines and cortisol in recent-onset posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 423–434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006842200005000-00016 Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., Cox, A. L., Engel, C. C., & Castro, C. A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 453–463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa072972 Hoge, E. A., Worthington, J. J., Nagurney, J. T., Chang, Y., Kay, E. B., Feterowski, C. M., .!.!. Pitman, R. K. (2012). Effect of acute posttrauma propranolol on PTSD outcome and physiological responses during script-driven imagery. CNS Neuroscience and Therapeutics, 18, 21–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ j.1755-5949.2010.00227.x Holbrook, T. L., Galarneau, M. R., Dye, J. L., Quinn, K., & Dougherty, A. L. (2010). Morphine use after combat injury in Iraq and post-traumatic stress disorder. New England Journal of Medicine, 362, 110–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa0903326 Holen, A. (1993). The North Sea oil rig disaster. In J. P. Wilson & B. Raphael (Eds.), International handbook of traumatic stress syndromes (pp. 471–478). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-2820-3_39 179 Richard A. Bryant Horowitz, M. J. (1997). Stress response syndromes: PTSD, grief, and adjustment disorders (3rd ed.). Northvale, NJ: Aronson, 1997. Janet, P. (1907). The major symptoms of hysteria. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10008-000 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Ji, L., Xiaowei, Z., Chuanlin, W., & Wei, L. (2010). Investigation of posttraumatic stress disorder in children after animal-induced injury in China. Pediatrics, 126, e320–e324. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1542/peds.2009-3530 Kassam-Adams, N., Garcia-España, J. F., Fein, J. A., & Winston, F. K. (2005). Heart rate and posttraumatic stress in injured children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 335–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.62.3.335 Kassam-Adams, N., & Winston, F. K. (2004). Predicting child PTSD: The relationship between acute stress disorder and PTSD in injured children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 403–411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583200404000-00006 Kilpatrick, D. G., & Veronen, L. J. (1983). Treatment for rape-related problems: Crisis intervention is not enough. In W. L. C. L. H. Cohen & C. A. Spector (Eds.), Crisis intervention (2nd ed., pp. 165–185). New York, NY: Human Sciences Press. Kinzie, J. D., Sack, W. H., Angell, R. H., Manson, S., & Rath, B. (1986). The psychiatric effects of massive trauma on Cambodian children: I. The children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 370–376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S00027138(09)60259-4 Kleim, B., Ehlers, A., & Glucksman, E. (2007). Early predictors of chronic post-traumatic stress disorder in assault survivors. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1457–1467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ S0033291707001006 Koopman, C., Classen, C., Cardeña, E., & Spiegel, D. (1995). When disaster strikes, acute stress disorder may follow. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 29–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490080103 Koopman, C., Classen, C., & Spiegel, D. (1994). Predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms among survivors of the Oakland/Berkeley, Calif., firestorm. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 888–894. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.151.6.888 Kornør, H., Winje, D., Ekeberg, Ø., Weisaeth, L., Kirkehei, I., Johansen, K., .!.!. Steiro, A. (2008). Early trauma-focused cognitive-behavioural therapy to prevent chronic post-traumatic stress disorder and related symptoms: a systematic review and metaanalysis. BMC Psychiatry, 8, 81. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1186/1471-244X-8-81 Kramer, T., Buckhout, R., & Eugenio, P. (1990). Weapon focus, arousal, and eyewitness memory: Attention 180 must be paid. Law and Human Behavior, 14, 167–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01062971 Lam, W. W., Bonanno, G. A., Mancini, A. D., Ho, S., Chan, M., Hung, W. K., .!.!. Fielding, R. (2010). Trajectories of psychological distress among Chinese women diagnosed with breast cancer. PsychoOncology, 19, 1044–1051. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ pon.1658 Liang, K. C., Juler, R. G., & McGaugh, J. L. (1986). Modulating effects of posttraining epinephrine on memory: Involvement of the amygdala noradrenergic system. Brain Research, 368, 125–133. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91049-8 Litz, B. T., Orsillo, S. M., Kaloupek, D., & Weathers, F. (2000). Emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 26–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.109.1.26 Lommen, M. J., Engelhard, I. M., Sijbrandij, M., van den Hout, M. A., & Hermans, D. (2013). Pre-trauma individual differences in extinction learning predict posttraumatic stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 63–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.11.004 Maass, A., & Köhnken, G. (1989). Eyewitness identification: Simulating the “weapon effect.” Law and Human Behavior, 13, 397–408. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/BF01056411 Marmar, C. R., Weiss, D. S., & Metzler, T. J. (1997). The Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire. In J. P. Wilson & T. M. Keane (Eds.), Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD (pp. 412–428). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Marshall, R. D., Spitzer, R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (1999). Review and critique of the new DSM–IV diagnosis of acute stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1677–1685. Mayou, R. A., Ehlers, A., & Bryant, B. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents: 3-year follow-up of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 665–675. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00069-9 McNally, G. P., & Westbrook, R. F. (2003). Anterograde amnesia for Pavlovian fear conditioning and the role of one-trial overshadowing: Effects of preconditioning exposures to morphine in the rat. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 222–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0097-7403.29.3.222 Meiser-Stedman, R., Dalgleish, T., Smith, P., Yule, W., & Glucksman, E. (2007). Diagnostic, demographic, memory quality, and cognitive variables associated with acute stress disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 65–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021843X.116.1.65 Meiser-Stedman, R., Yule, W., Smith, P., Glucksman, E., & Dalgleish, T. (2005). Acute stress disorder Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents involved in assaults or motor vehicle accidents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1381–1383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ appi.ajp.162.7.1381 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Milad, M. R., Rauch, S. L., Pitman, R. K., & Quirk, G. J. (2006). Fear extinction in rats: Implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychology, 73, 61–71. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008 Milgram, N. A., Toubiana, Y. H., Klingman, A., Raviv, A., & Goldstein, I. (1988). Situational exposure and personal loss in children’s acute and chronic stress reactions to a school bus disaster. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 1, 339–352. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1002/jts.2490010306 Moulds, M. L., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). Traumatic memories in acute stress disorder: An analysis of narratives before and after treatment. Clinical Psychologist, 9, 10–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 13284200500116971 Murray, J., Ehlers, A., & Mayou, R. A. (2002). Dissociation and post-traumatic stress disorder: Two prospective studies of road traffic accident survivors. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 363–368. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1192/bjp.180.4.363 Myers, K. M., & Davis, M. (2007). Mechanisms of fear extinction. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 120–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001939 Nixon, R. D. (2012). Cognitive processing therapy versus supportive counseling for acute stress disorder following assault: A randomized pilot trial. Behavior Therapy, 43, 825–836. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.beth.2012.05.001 Nixon, R. D., & Bryant, R. A. (2003). Peritraumatic and persistent panic attacks in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1237–1242. http://dx.doi.org/S0005796703001505 Nixon, R. D., Nehmy, T. J., Ellis, A. A., Ball, S. A., Menne, A., & McKinnon, A. C. (2010). Predictors of posttraumatic stress in children following injury: The influence of appraisals, heart rate, and morphine use. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 810–815. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.002 Norrholm, S. D., Jovanovic, T., Vervliet, B., Myers, K. M., Davis, M., Rothbaum, B. O., & Duncan, E. J. (2006). Conditioned fear extinction and reinstatement in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Learning and Memory, 13, 681–685. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/ lm.393906 Norris, F. H., Tracy, M., & Galea, S. (2009). Looking for resilience: Understanding the longitudinal trajectories of responses to stress. Social Science and Medicine, 68, 2190–2198. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.socscimed.2009.03.043 Nugent, N. R., Christopher, N. C., Crow, J. P., Browne, L., Ostrowski, S., & Delahanty, D. L. (2010). The efficacy of early propranolol administration at reducing PTSD symptoms in pediatric injury patients: A pilot study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23, 282–287. O’Carroll, R. E., Drysdale, E., Cahill, L., Shajahan, P., & Ebmeier, K. P. (1999). Stimulation of the noradrenergic system enhances and blockade reduces memory for emotional material in man. Psychological Medicine, 29, 1083–1088. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1017/S0033291799008703 O’Donnell, M. L., Creamer, M., Elliott, P., & Bryant, R. (2007). Tonic and phasic heart rate as predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 256–261. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ PSY.0b013e3180417d04 O’Donnell, M. L., Creamer, M. C., Parslow, R., Elliott, P., Holmes, A. C., Ellen, S., .!.!. Bryant, R. A. (2008). A predictive screening index for posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following traumatic injury. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 923–932. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012918 Orr, S. P. (1997). Psychophysiologic reactivity to traumarelated imagery in PTSD. Diagnostic and theoretical implications of recent findings. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 821, 114–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997. tb48273.x Orr, S. P., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Pineles, S. L., Chang, Y., & Pitman, R. K. (2012). Predicting post-trauma stress symptoms from pre-trauma psychophysiologic reactivity, personality traits and measures of psychopathology. Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders, 2, 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ a0012918 Orr, S. P., Lasko, N. B., Metzger, L. J., Berry, N. J., Ahern, C. E., & Pitman, R. K. (1998). Psychophysiologic assessment of women with posttraumatic stress disorder resulting from childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 906–913. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022006X.66.6.906 Orr, S. P., Pitman, R. K., Lasko, N. B., & Herz, L. R. (1993). Psychophysiological assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder imagery in World War II and Korean combat veterans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 152–159. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.152 Panasetis, P., & Bryant, R. A. (2003). Peritraumatic versus persistent dissociation in acute stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16, 563–566. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:JOTS.0000004079.74606.ba Pietrzak, R. H., Van Ness, P. H., Fried, T. R., Galea, S., & Norris, F. H. (2013). Trajectories of posttraumatic 181 Richard A. Bryant stress symptomatology in older persons affected by a large-magnitude disaster. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 520–526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.jpsychires.2012.12.005 Pitman, R. K. (1989). Post-traumatic stress disorder, hormones, and memory. Biological Psychiatry, 26, 221–223. http://dx.doi.org/0006-3223(89)90033-4 Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., Healy, A. R., Cheema, F., Lasko, N. B., .!.!. Orr, S. P. (2002). Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 189–192. http://dx.doi.org/ S0006322301012793 Radley, J. J., Kabbaj, M., Jacobson, L., Heydendael, W., Yehuda, R., & Herman, J. P. (2011). Stress risk factors and stress-related pathology: Neuroplasticity, epigenetics and endophenotypes. Stress, 14, 481–497. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/10253890. 2011.604751 Rauch, S. L., Shin, L. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2006). Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: Human neuroimaging research—past, present, and future. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 376–382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.biopsych.2006.06.004 Resick, P. A., Galovski, T. E., O’Brien Uhlmansiek, M., Scher, C. D., Clum, G. A., & Young-Xu, Y. (2008). A randomized clinical trial to dismantle components of cognitive processing therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in female victims of interpersonal violence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 243–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022006X.76.2.243 Resick, P. A., Nishith, P., Weaver, T. L., Astin, M. C., & Feuer, C. A. (2002). A comparison of cognitiveprocessing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 867–879. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022006X.70.4.867 Resick, P. A., & Schnicke, M. K. (1992). Cognitive processing therapy for sexual assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 748–756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.60.5.748 Resnick, H. S., Yehuda, R., Pitman, R. K., & Foy, D. W. (1995). Effect of previous trauma on acute plasma cortisol level following rape. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1675–1677. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/ajp.152.11.1675 Robert, R., Blakeney, P. E., Villarreal, C., Rosenberg, L., & Meyer, W. J., III. (1999). Imipramine treatment in pediatric burn patients with symptoms of acute stress disorder: A pilot study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 182 873–882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583199907000-00018 Roberts, N. P., Kitchiner, N. J., Kenardy, J., & Bisson, J. I. (2009). Systematic review and meta-analysis of multiple-session early interventions following traumatic events. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 293–301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi. ajp.2008.08040590 Ross, C. A., Joshi, S., & Currie, R. (1990). Dissociative experiences in the general population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1547–1552. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/ajp.147.11.1547 Rothbaum, B. O., Kearns, M. C., Price, M., Malcoun, E., Davis, M., Ressler, K. J., .!.!. Houry, D. (2012). Early intervention may prevent the development of posttraumatic stress disorder: A randomized pilot civilian study with modified prolonged exposure. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 957–963. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.002 Saxe, G., Stoddard, F., Courtney, D., Cunningham, K., Chawla, N., Sheridan, R., .!.!. King, L. (2001). Relationship between acute morphine and the course of PTSD in children with burns. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 915–921. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583200108000-00013 Schelling, G., Briegel, J., Roozendaal, B., Stoll, C., Rothenhäusler, H. B., & Kapfhammer, H. P. (2001). The effect of stress doses of hydrocortisone during septic shock on posttraumatic stress disorder in survivors. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 978–985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01270-7 Schelling, G., Kilger, E., Roozendaal, B., de Quervain, D. J. F., Briegel, J., Dagge, A., .!.!. Kapfhammer, H. P. (2004). Stress doses of hydrocortisone, traumatic memories, and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in patients after cardiac surgery: A randomized study. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 627–633. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.09.014 Segal, S. K., & Cahill, L. (2009). Endogenous noradrenergic activation and memory for emotional material in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 1263–1271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.020 Shalev, A. Y., Ankri, Y., Israeli-Shalev, Y., Peleg, T., Adessky, R., & Freedman, S. (2012). Prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder by early treatment: Results from the Jerusalem Trauma Outreach And Prevention study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 166–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archgenpsychiatry.2011.127 Shalev, A. Y., Freedman, S., Peri, T., Brandes, D., Sahar, T., Orr, S. P., & Pitman, R. K. (1998). Prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. American Journal of Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Psychiatry, 155, 630–637. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ ajp.155.5.630 Shalev, A. Y., Sahar, T., Freedman, S., Peri, T., Glick, N., Brandes, D., .!.!. Pitman, R. K. (1998). A prospective study of heart rate response following trauma and the subsequent development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 553–559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.553 Sharp, S., Thomas, C., Rosenberg, L., Rosenberg, M., & Meyer, W., III. (2010). Propranolol does not reduce risk for acute stress disorder in pediatric burn trauma. Journal of Trauma, 68, 193–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/TA.0b013e3181a8b326 Sijbrandij, M., Olff, M., Reitsma, J. B., Carlier, I. V. E., de Vries, M. H., & Gersons, B. P. R. (2007). Treatment of acute posttraumatic stress disorder with brief cognitive behavioral therapy: A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 82–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.1.82 Smid, G. E., van der Velden, P. G., Lensvelt-Mulders, G. J., Knipscheer, J. W., Gersons, B. P., & Kleber, R. J. (2012). Stress sensitization following a disaster: A prospective study. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1675–1686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ S0033291711002765 Smith, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2000). The generality of cognitive bias in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 709–715. http://dx.doi.org/ S0005-7967(99)00096-0 Solomon, S., Laor, N., & McFarlane, A. (1996). Acute posttraumatic reactions in soldiers and civilians. In B. A. Van der Kolk, A. C. McFarlane, & L. Weisaeth (Eds.), Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body and society (pp. 102–114). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Southwick, S. M., Davis, M., Horner, B., Cahill, L., Morgan, C. A., III, Gold, P. E., .!.!. Charney, D. C. (2002). Relationship of enhanced norepinephrine activity during memory consolidation to enhanced long-term memory in humans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1420–1422. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1420 Spiegel, D. (1996). Hypnosis in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. In S. J. Lynn, I. Kirsch, & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Casebook of clinical hypnosis (pp. 99–111). http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/11090-005 Spiegel, D., Hunt, T., & Dondershine, H. E. (1988). Dissociation and hypnotizability in posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 301–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.145.3.301 Spiegel, D., Koopman, C., & Classen, C. (1994). Acute stress disorder and dissociation. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 22, 11–23. Steil, R., & Ehlers, A. (2000). Dysfunctional meaning of posttraumatic intrusions in chronic PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 537–558. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00069-8 Stein, M. B., Kerridge, C., Dimsdale, J. E., & Hoyt, D. B. (2007). Pharmacotherapy to prevent PTSD: Results from a randomized controlled proof-of-concept trial in physically injured patients. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20, 923–932. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ jts.20270 Stein, M. B., Walker, J. R., Hazen, A. L., & Forde, D. R. (1997). Full and partial posttraumatic stress disorder: Findings from a community survey. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1114–1119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.154.8.1114 Sterlini, G. L., & Bryant, R. A. (2002). Hyperarousal and dissociation: A study of novice skydivers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 431–437. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00021-3 Sternberg, D. B., Isaacs, K. R., Gold, P. E., & McGaugh, J. L. (1985). Epinephrine facilitation of appetitive learning: Attenuation with adrenergic receptor antagonists. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 44, 447–453. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S01631047(85)90856-8 Stutman, R. K., & Bliss, E. L. (1985). Posttraumatic stress disorder, hypnotizability, and imagery. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 741–743. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/ajp.142.6.741 Vaiva, G., Ducrocq, F., Jezequel, K., Averland, B., Lestavel, P., Brunet, A., & Marmar, C. R. (2003). Immediate treatment with propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress disorder two months after trauma. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 947–949. http://dx.doi.org/S0006322303004128 van der Kolk, B. A., & Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: Overview and exploratory study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 505–525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ jts.2490080402 van der Kolk, B. A., & van der Hart, O. (1989). Pierre Janet and the breakdown of adaptation in psychological trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1530–1540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ ajp.146.12.1530 Videlock, E. J., Peleg, T., Segman, R., Yehuda, R., Pitman, R. K., & Shalev, A. Y. (2008). Stress hormones and post-traumatic stress disorder in civilian trauma victims: a longitudinal study. Part II: the adrenergic response. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 11, 373–380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1461145707008139 Wang, X., Gao, L., Shinfuku, N., Zhang, H., Zhao, C., & Shen, Y. (2000). Longitudinal study of earthquakerelated PTSD in a randomly selected community sample in north China. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1260–1266. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1260 183 Richard A. Bryant Warda, G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). Cognitive bias in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 1177–1183. http://dx.doi.org/ S0005796798000710 Wikman, A., Molloy, G. J., Randall, G., & Steptoe, A. (2011). Cognitive predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms six months following acute coronary syndrome. Psychology and Health, 26, 974–988. Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution. Yehuda, R., Boisoneau, D., Mason, J. W., & Giller, E. L. (1993). Glucocorticoid receptor number and cortisol excretion in mood, anxiety, and psychotic disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 34, 18–25. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90252-9 Yehuda, R., Kahana, B., Binder-Brynes, K., Southwick, S. M., Mason, J. W., & Giller, E. L. (1995). Low urinary cortisol excretion in Holocaust survivors 184 with posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 982–986. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/ajp.152.7.982 Yehuda, R., Southwick, S. M., Nussbaum, G., Wahby, V., Giller, E. L., Jr., & Mason, J. W. (1990). Low urinary cortisol excretion in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 366–369. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053199006000-00004 Zohar, J., Yahalom, H., Kozlovsky, N., Cwikel-Hamzany, S., Matar, M. A., Kaplan, Z., .!.!. Cohen, H. (2011). High dose hydrocortisone immediately after trauma may alter the trajectory of PTSD: Interplay between clinical and animal studies. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 796–809. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.06.001