This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Psychological Bulletin 1988, Vol. 103, No. 2, 179-192 Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0033-2909/88/500.75 Footedness: Asymmetries in Foot Preference and Skill and Neuropsychological Assessment of Foot Movement Michael Peters University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada If footedness is defined in terms of a reliable role differentiation of the two feet and legs, righthanders show a right-foot bias for activities requiring fine manipulation and focused attention. In adult right-handers, the left leg tends to be the longer and heavier one, in keeping with the support role of that leg. In left-handers, anatomical asymmetries tend to be in the opposite direction, and functional preferences are somewhat less clearly expressed. Foot biases and their interaction with hand biases are of practical importance in the design of human-machine systems. The considerable sensitivity of foot and leg performance to neurological insult rendere the assessment of foot and leg use very attractive for purposes of clinical neuropsychology. The unbridled enthusiasm for matters pertaining to lateral- second, shorter part of the review deals with a quite different ization in structure and function has led psychologists to ex- aspect of footedness. When motor performance tests are con- plore the left and right of phenomena ranging from ability in sidered within the neuropsychological context, the focus is usu- mathematics to crooked smiles. It comes as a surprise, therefore, that footedness as a basic phenomenon has been relatively ally on hand performance. I argue that foot motor performance tests should be a routine part of evaluation of motor perfor- neglected. This neglect is not justified. From a practical point mance in neuropsychological applications. of view, any structural and functional biases in the lower extremities are of direct interest in the context of athletics, danc- Laterality ing, the use of some musical instruments, and the operation of Structural Asymmetries machines. From the perspective of laterality-of-function research, there is the question of how reliable or valid foot prefer- Earlier sources on anatomical asymmetries were influenced ence measures are. There certainly is no unanimity on this point. Bryden(1982), for instance, questioned footedness as an by the idea that dominance extends to the entire body half. Bell (1870) claimed that in right-handed people the left side is the indicator of lateral dominance, citing a study by Raczkowski, weaker side and that shoemakers and tailors are aware that the Kalat, and Nebes (1974), who challenged both the validity and right side of the body requires larger measurements. reliability of foot preference measures. In contrast, Porac and More recently, von Bonin (1962) observed, "We are generally Coren (1981) reported marked and reliable foot preference biases. Another important issue concerns the interaction of foot right-handed and left-footed" (p. 1). His statement was largely based on the X-ray studies by Ingelmark (1947), who measured preference with hand preference. It is quite clear that in many the leg and arm length of right- and left-handers and whose work activities foot roles are determined by the role of the hands, as remains the most comprehensive and carefully conducted study in the stance taken when throwing a rock. In such situations, on anatomical leg asymmetries to date. Ingelmark's results were foot roles are complementary to hand roles. There may, how- somewhat more complex than von Benin's summary suggests. ever, also be foot preferences that do not relate in any simple For all right-handed subjects between 6 and 20 years old, the way to hand preferences. If such preferences are similar in kind right arm was the longer one, and for all left-handed subjects to hand preferences, it can be assumed that the lateral special- between 6 and 20 years old, the left arm was the longer one. ization seen in hand preference does not reside in the ma- However, whereas 85% of right-handed subjects between 6 and chinery that guides hand movement as such but at a higher level 13 years old had a longer right leg, 85% of right-handed subjects (Peters, 1983) and can therefore express itself in lateral biases between 14 and 20 years old had a longer left leg. Ingelmark independent of a particular limb or class of movement. This review consists of two somewhat unequal parts. First, I reported that for left-handers the situation was reversed: the 6to 13-year-old gnjup had a longer left leg, whereas the 14- to 20- discuss laterality in foot preference and foot performance. The year-old group had a longer right leg. Ingelmark's results need to be taken seriously because of his careful methods and large number of subjects (150, of which 35 were left-handers). In- This work was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant A7054. I am very grateful for the extensive and constructive criticism that Lauren Harris provided for this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael Peters, Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario NIG 2W1, Canada. gelmark's data for adults is in harmony with Chibber and Singh's (1970) finding that in adults the left leg tends to be the heavier one. Chibber and Singh (1972) reported that in human fetuses the left lower limb is heavier than the right lower limb. Ingelmark (1947), however, found no reliable asymmetries in a sample of 71 fetuses at or close to full gestation age. This is in 179 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 180 MICHAEL PETERS agreement with generally contradictory data on asymmetries for infants and fetuses (L. J.Harris, 1983). In view of the observations on adult samples, Levy and Levy's (1978) report of an asymmetry in the size of human feet comes as no surprise. An added element in the Levy and Levy (1978) study was an interaction among foot length, sex, and handedness. They interpreted this interaction in the context of differences in developmental gradients, as a function of hormone action early in life. This speculation assumes renewed significance in view of the suggestion that lateralization is related to hormonal factors during development (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1984). Levy and Levy (1978) reported that whereas right-handed girls had larger left than right feet, right-handed boys showed an advantage of right-foot size. The opposite was the case for left-handers. Subsequent work showed various degrees of support. Pomerantz and Harris (1980) provided partial support for the Levy and Levy (1978) findings by showing that 15-year-old boys had longer right than left feet. The only other significant finding was that 11-year-old girls had longer right than left feet, and this is contrary to the findings of Levy and Levy (1978). Mascie-Taylor, MacLarnon, Lanigan, and McManus (1981), Means and Walters (1982), Peters, Petrie, and Oddie (1981), and Yanowitz, Satz, and Heilman (1981) found either contradictory patterns or no relation between sex, handedness, and foot size. Orsini and Satz (1985), using a very large sample, were unable to find any significant relation among sex, handedness, and foot size. Orsini and Satz (1985) did report, "Most subjects had a longer left foot" (p. 128), but continued to say that this finding did not reach statistical significance, invalidating the first statement. Mascie-Taylor et al., although not able to find sex and handedness interactions with foot length, did indicate greater length for the left foot. To further confuse the picture, Means and Walters (1982) stated that there was a trend for greater right-foot length in all their groups. In commenting on the various articles by researchers attempting to replicate the original finding of an interaction among foot length, sex, and handedness, Levy and Levy (1981) pointed out that in their original study foot length was measured while the subjects were standing. Levy and Levy (1981) correctly stated that this in itself could be a major source of the contradictory findings. Orsini and Satz (1985), however, also measured foot length while subjects were standing and could not confirm the Levy and Levy (1978) findings. Another methodological difference is that Levy and Levy (1978) measured from the big toe to the heel, whereas Orsini and Satz and Peters et al. measured from the longest toe to the heel. A cautious interpretation of these findings is that foot length asymmetry, however measured, does not reliably vary as a function of sex and handedness. A different measure of the foot, for instance, one that considers the entire plantar surface, might reveal a slight relation. The caution that is exercised here in not dismissing the possibility of foot asymmetry outright stems from anatomical observations favoring the left leg. Additional information allows for anatomical foot asymmetries. For instance, Singh (1970) found that the left shoe of right- and lefthanded youths showed greater signs of wear than did the right shoe. In the section on functional asymmetries, I suggest that in right-handers at least, the left leg is often preferred for sup- port and power functions. Thus, although an interaction between foot measures, sex, and handedness does not look very convincing, the possibility of a small overall left-foot anatomical advantage (however defined) cannot be dismissed. The question of asymmetries in foot size is relevant not only in the narrower context of lateralization of function but also with reference to neurology. Lesions to the frontoparietal cortex of one hemisphere (Penfield & Jasper, 1954; Penfield & Robertson, 1943) can produce a reduction in contralateral limb size. If there were systematic asymmetries in foot size in the normal population, relatively small reductions would be difficult to evaluate. However, in those studies in which researchers found systematic differences among some groups, the actual differences in foot length were very small indeed. In conclusion, the consensus is that in adult right-handers the left leg is longer and heavier than the right leg. Whereas the right arm in right-handers is longer than the left arm as early as age 6, the left leg only begins to outstrip the right leg in length in the early teens. Because the asymmetry increases up to the age of 20 (Ingelmark, 1947), it can be suggested that active foot use plays a role in this trend. Ingelmark found no group differences in the magnitude of length asymmetries between right- and lefthanders, except for 14-20-year-olds, in which left-handers had smaller arm length asymmetries. Functional Asymmetries: Foot Preference The definition of foot preference. Is it meaningful to talk about foot preference? Why not talk about leg preference? These questions are best answered with reference to hand preference. In evaluating hand preference, for instance, with the Oldfield (1971) questionnaire, none of the items refer exclusively to the use of the hand. Because the hand is unavoidably attached to the arm, hand movements do not happen in isolation and cannot be separated from arm movements. From a functional point of view, it would make little sense if the lateral specialization in movement that is observed in the digits of the hands was not also accompanied by a corresponding lateral specialization of the movements of the arm (Kimura & Davison, 1975). Nevertheless, some items, such as writing, place relatively more emphasis on hand and wrist movements than items such as throwing. From a kinesthetic point of view, these classes of movement are rather different. Hand preference, then, is not exclusively defined by any particular class of movement but rather by the preferential allocation of focal attention to one hand and its contribution to the goal of movement. That attention is a very decisive factor can be shown by a paradigm in which the focusing of attention on one or the other hand is manipulated while the movements are kept constant (Peters, 1985). An additional important dimension of hand preference is role differentiation; the preferred hand realizes the goal of the movement, and the nonpreferred hand lends support. The differentiation of hand and arm roles is most salient in naturally occurring and explicitly bimanual skilled activities, which form the bulk of skilled human activities (Peters, 1981, 1983). This view of hand preference, defined in terms of role differentiation, is helpful in understanding footedness. If pure unimanual movements are rare, it is even harder to conceive of purposeful movements of one leg and foot in isolation from the other. After This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. FOOTEDNESS 181 all, whatever one does with one leg and foot is supported in vated because all children in his sample had been taught to lead some way by the movements of the other leg and foot. off with the left foot. Because Haefner's scoring system did not The definition that I adopt for footedness, then, emphasizes allow a disentanglement of who did what with which foot, left- the role differentiation of the feet. The foot (henceforth used in handers ended up with more congruent scores than did right- place of foot and leg) that is used to manipulate an object or to lead out, as in jumping, is denned here as the preferredfoot. The handers, indicating a relatively greater use of the left foot in lefthanders than right-foot use in right-handers. Because of the ease foot that is used to support the activities of the preferred foot of scoring and general relevance of kicking, more data are avail- by lending postural and stabilizing support is defined as the able for kicking than for any other measure. As will be seen, nonpreferred foot. In the subsequent discussion, I show that a although general trends can be discerned, the agreement in the literature is by no means overwhelming. definition in terms of role differentiation is in some cases not satisfactory unless qualifications are made. An example is bal- Kicking. In terms of the definition adopted above, kicking ancing, in which head, arms, torso, and legs collaborate to appears to be an ideal foot movement, and this is most likely achieve postural stability. Which is the preferred leg here—the why A. J. Harris (1958) included the subtest of kicking a bean one used for postural support or the one used for dynamic bag in his lateralizatipn inventory. In kicking a ball, the focus counterbalancing? Nevertheless, the definition is of sufficient of attention unquestionably rests on the foot that manipulates usefulness to be adopted here. the ball. Both fine manipulative skill in handling the ball and The older literature. In the older literature, the "hand-foot a more ballistic application of force can be observed, and this theory," as Smith (1917) put it, seemed predominant. This the- suggests that ball handling extends across different categories of ory assumes a congruence of handedness and footedness. Bell movement. The nonpreferred foot supports the movements of (1870), for instance, was convinced that the right-hander is also the preferred foot by providing postural support and a proper a right-footer, a conviction shared by modern writers (Corballis, body balance that is necessary for the preferred foot to operate. 1983; Delacato, 1963). But the failure to specify what sort of Not unexpectedly, there is a population preference for kick- movements would indicate right-footedness subsequently led to ing with the right foot. Stern and Schilf (1932) studied the prev- some confusion. Gould (1908) was struck by the observation alence of right-footedness in 7,000 schoolchildren. They re- that soldiers tend to march with the left foot. By that, he meant ported that it is more common to observe right-footedness that soldiers lead out with the left foot when marching. This among left-handers than it is to observe left-footedness among observation was also made by Stern and Schilf (1932), who right-handers. In support, they stated that in a school with 300 stated, "Why it is that the army begins to march with the left children, none of the 15 left-handers were left-footers. Stern and foot, or why the about-turn is always done to the left we have not Schilf also remarked, with some consternation, that there were been able to find out, in spite of queries in all sorts of places" (p. 42, translated by Peters). Perhaps the habit is related to what marked and inexplicable fluctuations in the prevalence of leftfooters for kicking between schools. For example, in one school the arms do. In formal marching, the gun rests against the left with 435 children, there were 6.2% left-footers, whereas in an- shoulder, and the right arm is free to move in concert with the other with 368 children, only 0.5% were left-footers. The dis- left leg. In bygone days, the shield was carried on the left, and crepancy could be accounted for if in the latter school left- in that case it would also make sense if it was the right arm (with footed kicking was actively discouraged. or without weapon) that showed the more active movement. In Komai and Fukuoka (1934) recorded the foot chosen for fighting with shield and weapon, the left foot advances toward kicking in about 17,000 Japanese schoolchildren. Over the first the enemy. Similarly, in firing a rifle, the left foot faces the tar- eight school grades, the prevalence of left-footed kicking ranged get. Could the marching convention have evolved as a ritualized advance toward the enemy? Perhaps. The interpretation applies from 5.5% to 8.5% in boys and from 3.5% to 6% in girls. The prevalence of left-footed kicking in boys was higher than that only to instances in which both arms are directly involved. In of girls in all grades, and left-footed kicking showed a somewhat individual dueling with pistol or fencing weapons, in which the irregular decline over age. The prevalence of left-footedness right hand holds the weapon and the other hand is not involved parallels the prevalence of left-handedness for activities that are in defense, the right foot faces the adversary. Convinced that the right foot is dominant, Gould (1908) was not subject to powerful cultural pressures. For instance, lefthanded use of a penknife and left-handed throwing showed fre- not deterred by the observation of left-footed marching and quencies similar to left-footed kicking, whereas by Grade 8 only stated that the tendency to use the left foot was evidence for 0.2% of the boys and none of the girls wrote (brush stroke) with right-sidedness. He made his argument clear with reference to the left hand. The decline of left-footed kicking in the Komai kicking. Gould felt that kicking with the left foot is evidence for right-footedness because the right is chosen to support and and Fukuoka study suggests that there are some cultural pressures against left-footed kicking, and such pressures may, as the steady the body. In his desire to provide supporting evidence, prevalence figures for one school in the Stern and Schilf (1932) Gould also convinced himself, though not others (Downey, report suggest, be quite strong in isolated cases. Kovac (1973) 1927), that a right-handed person would generally use the left also reported a decline of left-footedness over the ages of 10 foot on a spade. Haefner (1930) used four measures of footed- to 19 years. In Kovac's large sample, however, there was also a ness: leading off, stepping up on a chair, kicking a ball with the decrease in clear right preference so that there was an overall foot, and pressing a sheet of paper down with the foot. He as- decrease in the prevalence of right-footedness. signed scores on the basis of congruence for foot and hand Gardner (1941) also found a very high prevalence of right- choice but ran into difficulties when the congruence scores of footed kicking. She indicated that in her sample of young adults right-handers were depressed and those of left-handers were ele- about 89% preferred the right foot for kicking. This contrasts This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 182 MICHAEL PETERS with lower prevalence figures from Blau (1946), who reported that in a large sample of American schoolchildren, 74.2% used the right foot for kicking, whereas 25.8% used the left foot. Even lower prevalence figures were given by Cohen (1976), in whose sample of over 500 schoolchildren there was an almost even split between those who preferred the right and those who preferred the left foot for kicking. Nachshon and Denno (1986) studied a very large sample of 7-year-old Black children and reported an overall prevalence of right-footed kicking in 63%. Nachshon, Denno, and Aurand (1983) suggested that Black children have a higher prevalence of variable foot choices in kicking than do White children. Because the two groups did not differ in the prevalence of right preference for hand and eye use, this difference may be taken as a further indicator of cultural and environmental influences on foot preference. Annett and Turner (1974) distinguished between pure and mixed handedness groups. Pure left-handers, for instance, choose the left hand for all items on a handedness questionnaire, whereas mixed left-handers declare some right-hand preferences. About 96.2% of the pure right-handers and 86.7% of the mixed right-handers preferred the right foot for kicking. Of the pure left-handers, 83.8% preferred the left foot, whereas of the mixed left-handers, only 52.7% preferred the left foot. Because there are fewer pure than mixed left-handers, lefthanders as a group would show not nearly as strong a preference for the left foot as right-handers would show for the right foot. Peters and Durding (1979a) showed comparable results, keeping in mind that they did not differentiate between pure and mixed handedness. Of 56 right-handers, 95% preferred to kick with the right foot, and of 56 left-handers, 50% preferred to kick with the left foot. In the large-scale studies by Nachshon and Denno (1986) and Nachshon et al. (1983), left-handed children were just as likely to be right- as left-footed. In a survey of about 150 Canadian Indian schoolchildren (Peters, 1986), however, 80% of the left-handers preferred the left foot for kicking a ball, whereas close to 100% of the right-handers preferred the right foot. There was an unusually low prevalence of left-handers in this sample (about 6.4%), and 3.8% were what Annett and Turner would have called pure left-handers. Perhaps the lower prevalence figures suggest that in this community a bias against left-handedness was operative and that the left-handers who persisted were quite committed to their preference. If so, this might be taken as indirect support for Annett and Turner's contention that the more clearly left-handers are left-handed, the more likely they are to kick with the left foot. The results on kicking preferences allow two qualitative statements. First, right-handers show a clear right-foot preference for kicking. Second, left-handers as a group do not show a correspondingly clear preference for the left foot. There is some reason to believe that pure left-handers show a stronger left-foot preference than mixed left-handers. In quantitative terms, the prevalence figures are not in as much agreement as one would wish for such a simple measure. The figures given by Cohen (1976) show a relatively low prevalence of right-foot preference in the population, whereas at the other extreme, Singh (1970) found almost exclusive right-footed kicking in right-handers and a fair proportion ofleft-handers who kicked with the right foot as well. To further complicate the picture, a lack of consistency is seen even in studies that have been used as standard instruments for the assessment of laterality. For instance, in the normative data published in the A. J. Harris (1958) laterality test, the prevalence figures for footedness show enough inconsistency to lead to concerns. Without doubt, cultural factors are important. For instance, in cultures in which soccer is common, children and adults probably give more decisive and informed answers about foot preference than in cultures in which such or similar sports are uncommon. In some cultures, biases against left-hand use might also transfer to use of the left foot, but this factor is difficult to evaluate and probably is no longer of major importance in Western cultures. A further source of variability is the precise phrasing of the question. Prevalence figures indicate less lateralization if subjects have the impression that the question is in the nature of "Which foot can you kick with?" and more lateralization if subjects think that the nature of the question is "Which foot would you choose when it really counts?" Finally, handedness is an important variable. Performance testing might reveal that left-handers are quite proficient with either foot. This speculation is based on interviews with left-handed soccer players, who, though stating a left-foot preference, often made it clear that they felt quite adept with the right foot as well. A converse statement cannot be made about right-handed soccer players. Porac and Coren (1981) looked at proficiency scores of soccer players relative to right- and leftfootedness and relative to congruent or crossed laterality of hand and foot. The term proficiency does not refer to a direct measure of performance but rather to an indirect measure in which points are awarded on the basis of the league level at which a playerplays. Such a measure, of necessity, provides only a very indirect index of playing ability, and considerable variability can be expected at the same league level. Porac and Coren found no significant differences when they compared proficiency scores of right- and left-footed soccer players, although the means favored the left-footers. Soccer players with mixed preferences had significantly higher proficiency scores than those with congruent preferences. Unfortunately, the data do not allow a judgment on whether this was based on righthanders who kicked with the left foot or on left-handers who kicked with the right foot. A guess, however, can be made. As in boxing, in which the left-hander enjoys an advantage because he has greater experience in fighting right-handers than righthanders have in fighting left-handers (Porac & Coren, 1981), the left-footed soccer player also has advantages. For instance, in close maneuvering, the player who can advance the ball with the left foot can keep the body between the ball and the right-footed defender. In addition, a player who can kick penalty shots with either the left or the right foot (the equivalent of a switch-hitter in baseball) is at a great advantage. Porac and Coren speculated that in soccer a crossed hand-foot preference is of advantage because the postural adjustments favor a pattern in which the kicking foot is opposite the preferred hand. The assumption, by no means substantiated, is that the compensatory movements made by the contralateral upper limb are the sorts of movements that are done best by the preferred upper limb. In spite of a lack of precise figures on the prevalence of left-footed or mixed-footed soccer players, however, it is possible to make some statements about the degree to which the choice of the kicking foot can be affected by training. Considering that the ability to kick equally well with both feet is strongly selected for This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. FOOTEDNESS 183 in soccer and considering that left-footed players are in demand rails in bars recognize this preference), with frequent shifts for the position of left forward in particular, the persistence of from one side to the other, and lateral preferences may be a right-foot bias in world-class soccer competition is striking. readily observed. Bird-watchers may have a good pastime look- Many players have such a strong right-foot bias that even at the ing for asymmetries in standing birds! world-cup level of competition, they advance the ball largely Finally, Stern and Schilf (1932) used an unorthodox but fa- with the right foot, even if that means breaking stride. It can miliar activity, sliding on ice. They asked children which leg was be concluded that for many individuals, the choice of foot for leading when they ran and slid on ice. The right leg tended to kicking is as compelling as the choice of hand for writing. be favored. Other common foot preference measures. First, which foot Early preferences for stepping and placing feet. Peters and leads when beginning to march or walk is probably contami- Petrie (1979) studied the stepping reflex in a group of infants nated by environmental influences (Gould, 1908; Haefner, ranging in age from 17 to 105 days and conducted four follow- 1930) because marching with the left seems to be the norm. In up tests. They found a strong overall preference for leading the a more recent look at this measure, Singh (1970) found less stepping reflex with the right foot. Melekian (1981) also investi- extreme preference figures than were reported in the older stud- gated the stepping reflex but with a much larger sample. Mele- ies, but in his sample of schoolchildren, the majority of both kian tested 95 infants at birth, 109 infants both at birth and at right-handers and left-handers led with the left foot. discharge from hospital, and a further 132 infants at discharge Second, choice of foot for picking up a pebble tends to be only. With this sample, Melekian also found a strong right-foot congruent with choice of foot for kicking (Augustyn & Peters, bias in leading the stepping reflex, with prevalence figures that 1986a) and is part of the foot inventory of the A. J. Harris were not significantly different from those seen in the Peters and (1958) lateralization test. Singh (1970) reported that all of his Petrie study. Trehub, Corter, and Shosenberg (1983), however, right-handers lifted a small object with the right foot and that failed to find a right-foot bias in their study of the stepping re- 29% of the left-handers chose the right foot as well. Augustyn flex and placing responses in infants. They found no consistent and Peters (1986a) found that 75% of the right-handers and 13% directional differences in the stepping reflex and a left bias for of the left-handers preferred the right foot for lifting a pebble. at least some comparisons for the placing response. In a careful Porac and Coren (1981) included this measure in their behav- study, Kamptner, Cornwell, Fitzgerald, and Harris (1985), us- iorally validated self-report inventory but gave no separate ing the criteria that Petrie and Peters used, also failed to find prevalence figures. Lifting a pebble is a good foot preference reliable right-foot biases in the stepping reflex in infants. In 24 task in the context of the present definition of footedness be- familiar right-handed infants, they found a small (nonsignifi- cause it forces a clear division of functional roles of the two feet cant) right bias in the period from 2 to 3 days afterbirth and no in terms of a foot that manipulates and one that supports, but discernible trends at later testing dates. Thelen, Ridley-John- this is so only if the subject is standing, forcing a choice of which son, and Fisher (1983) studied the kicking response in young leg is used for support and which is used for manipulation. infants and failed to find a consistent group preference during Third, stepping up on a chair (Gardner, 1941; Haefner, 1930) a longitudinal study of 8 infants. (After 6 weeks of age, group and stepping down from a chair do not give preference choices differences showed a slight right-leg advantage in seven of the that distinguish as clearly between the right and left feet as do nine follow-up tests). Thelen et al. suggested that shifting bilat- other measures. For instance, Gardner (1941) reported that in eral coordination and laterality patterns in infants may be due her sample 60% preferred the right foot for stepping up on a to a rivalry among reflex pathways that mature at different chair and 53% preferred the right foot for stepping down from rates. The work of Coryell and Cardinali (1979) suggests an in- a chair. Augustyn and Peters (1986a) found somewhat higher teraction among reflexes. They observed more leg movements figures, with 82% of the right-handers and 22% of the left-hand- in the direction on the side of preferred head direction in the ers preferring the right foot. Here, as with other tests, the situa- tonic neck reflex, but they did not see this result on all occa- tion is probably more complicated than it appears at first sions. This cannot, however, really explain the discrepancies glance. Does the reason for stepping up on a chair, that is, step- seen in the laterality of the stepping response. The kicking re- ping up to tie a shoelace versus stepping up to reach something, sponse is quite different from the stepping reflex. Thelen et al. make a difference, and if so, is there an interaction between noted the strong positive correlation between arousal and the chair height and choice of foot? kicking reflex. Kicking can be seen both when the infant is cry- Fourth, Chaurasia (1976) studied the position of the legs ing and during more pleasant (but always quite active) states, tucked under the body in the legs-crossed "Palthi" posture that and it may be considered a nonspecific and quite automatic mo- is common in India and stated that the preference patterns favor tor corollary of arousal. In contrast, the stepping reflex is associ- the left leg. Blau (1946) observed which leg was on top in the ated with a quieter state and is not as easily elicited when the crossed leg sitting position and found the right leg favored in infant is strongly aroused. Zelazo (1983) remarked that infants 66% of subjects. These tests are perhaps analogous to the question of which thumb is on top of the other in hand clasping—a appear to experience the stepping response as a pleasurable activity. If the elicitation of the stepping response is somewhat de- hand posture that tends to favor the left hand (Blau, 1946). pendent on the infant's being at ease, it is possible that the spe- Fifth, a measure that is of potential interest because it is easily cific interaction between experimenter and infant (quite apart observed in a variety of settings is the asymmetry seen in re- from technical aspects, such as control for the tonic neck reflex laxed standing, in which one leg is rigidly extended for support and how the infant is positioned) plays some role in the way in and the other has a slight flexion (Kovac & Horkovic, 1970). which the response is manifested. I do not attempt here to relate This is by far the preferred standing posture in humans (foot a possible right-leg bias (which, as the above discussion shows, This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 184 MICHAEL PETERS cannot be considered firmly established) in the stepping re- are always run counterclockwise so that the left leg always faces sponse to specific forms of foot dominance later in life. Instead the inside) and propel with the right, even though this might not and in agreement with the model of developing lateralization be the natural preference of the hurdler. advanced by Peters (1983), laterality at this stage would be seen Another common preference is found in mounting horses. simply as an early precursor of lateral biases in focusing atten- The rider uses the stirrup on the left side of the horse with the tion toward action. left leg and swings up and on the horse with the right leg. Before Informally observed fool and leg preferences. In athletics, the stirrups, the preference for leading with the right leg probably choice of the arm often determines the role of the feet. For in- resulted in the same directional preference. As with marching, stance, which foot is used in the final phase of throwing the the need for uniformity would also be expected to favor a com- javelin, discus, or shot is determined by the hand and arm used, mon direction of mounting horses in the cavalry. but the relation between hand and foot movements is not sim- Ballet dancers are commonly expected to overcome inherent ple. The right-handed boxer has the left foot forward, and the asymmetries, but casual discussions with both ballet dancers line offeree for a right-handed punch is also initially from right and ice skaters suggest that individuals have directional prefer- foot to right arm. In the pole vault, the situation is slightly ences, in terms of pirouettes and the leg that leads off jumps, different: The leg that is used to propel the body interacts with that must be overcome. Whether these preferences are system- the position in which the pole is held. If the left foot is used to atic remains to be determined. In this context, consider an observation by Bell (1870) propel the body, the left arm leads, and the right arm is used for the initial lift as the pole vaulter "climbs" the pole to reach greater height. In the high jump, most athletes prefer the left foot as the foot that provides the thrust, a fact that Stern and Schilf (1932) remarked on some time ago. With older jumping styles, this preference could be explained by virtue of the fact that the right leg was used to lead over the bar. The backward jumping style (the Fosbury flop), favored by most current high jumpers, does not assign such a clear leading role to the right leg but is nevertheless associated with a preference for thrusting with the left leg. For instance, I observed in the 1986 Canadian national trackand-field competition that six of the seven top-ranking male high jumpers jumped off with the left leg. I observed a similar ratio for female high jumpers in the 1986 Commonwealth Games. In the long jump, the situation is slightly more complicated because there are two conflicting demands on the jumper: The foot used for jumping off would by preference be the left foot, but in the long jump that foot has to be very delicately placed under peripheral visual guidance, presumably something that favors the right foot. The final choice of foot used to We see that opera dancers execute their more difficult feats on the right foot: but their preparatory exercises better evince the natural weakness of the left limb: in order to avoid awkwardness in public exhibitions, they are obliged to give double practice to the left leg; and if they neglect to do so, an ungraceful preference to the right side will be remarked, (p. 92) In dancing, individual preferences must be overcome not only for the obvious reasons but also because in dancing with partners foot preference for a given activity cannot be expressed independent of the partner. One of the most commonly encountered role differentiations for the feet is seen in operating cars. Cars generally have the accelerator on the right and the brake and clutch on the left. Interestingly, this arrangement is found regardless of whether a car has a right- or left-handed steering arrangement. Even in racing cars, in which individual driver preferences in terms of the arrangement of the gearshift and the steering wheel are accommodated, it is virtually unknown to have the accelerator pedal operated by the left foot. The design whereby the right foot operates the accelerator probably owes its existence to right-handed engineers. A similar situation is encountered with propel the athlete has to be the best possible compromise be- musical instruments. For instance, in piano playing it is the tween these two conflicting aspects. This perhaps explains why right foot that is most active and that most intimately interacts I observed an even split among the six top-ranking long jumpers with the hand that carries the voice or melody. Anton Rubin- in terms of foot choice for jumping off in the same competition. stein (Bernstein, 1981) called the right pedal "the soul of the One can perhaps apply a similar argument to athletes in the piano" (p. 143). hurdles events. Here, as with long jumpers, the number of hur- It appears that in the design of machines and instruments, the dlers who propel themselves over the hurdle with the right foot predilection of right-handers dominates the layout of controls. and the number of hurdlers who propel themselves over the hur- Clearly, right-handers feel more comfortable with arrangements dle with the left foot are more evenly matched than is the case that recognize the right foot as the preferred foot. Designers for high jumpers. For instance, in the 1986 International Ama- generally do not consider left-handers—a failing that is easy to teur Athletics competition in Rome, more than half of the fe- understand considering both the number and the directional in- male and male hurdlers led over the hurdle with the left leg and consistencies in that group. One assumption having face valid- propelled themselves with the right leg in the short-distance ity but yet to be fully supported by data is that left-handers are hurdles. For the short distance, hurdlers make three strides be- more flexible in adapting to various hand-foot-task combina- tween hurdles and have consistent leg roles. In the 400-m hur- tions than right-handers. If not, their situation is unfortunate dles, athletes ideally have an uneven number of strides between because they have little choice but to conform to the mode of hurdles as well (usually 15) so that they can always take off with right-handers. Whereas left-handers may be able to obtain left- the same leg, but some have to add strides toward the end as handed hand instruments (scissors and even guitars and vio- they tire. Because of this, 400-m hurdlers are trained to master lins), few if any instances of choice of foot exist in the layout of taking off with either leg. An added complication arises because industrial machines and musical instruments. in the turns it is advantageous to lead with the left leg (the races Is preference related to neuroanatomical variables? Little is This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. FOOTEDNESS known about the relation between handedness and the motor system, but even less is known about the relation between footedness and the motor system. A hint of possible significance, however, comes from Irving, Rebeiz, and Tomlinson (1974), who counted the number of motor neurons in the lumbar and sacral regions of the human spinal cord. All 5 of their subjects showed remarkable right-left asymmetries in cell counts at the S3 (third sacral vertebra) level. Although in some cases one side had more than twice the number of motor neurons than the other, the fact that relatively few motor neurons are found at this level makes the finding difficult to interpret. Nevertheless, further studies along these lines might be of interest. This section shows that humans have marked foot preferences. Interestingly, the only other group of animals with a wellstudied foot bias (Rogers, 1980), parrots, favors the left foot for holding objects. Corballis (1983) suggested that the left foot of the parrot is comparable to the right hand of humans. An alternate interpretation, however, is possible. Parrots use their beak primarily as a manipulative tool, and when the beak is used for manipulation, it is analogous to the right hand of humans. The left foot of the parrot then performs the role that the left hand performs in the human right-hander, positioning the object so that the beak can act on it. Foot Performance Measures Ultimately, preference measures should be indicators of performance differences. In the case of the hands, the dictum by Ludwig (1970) that the preferred hand is faster and performs more easily and better seems to hold. Does this apply to foot preference as well? Tapping. I discuss foot tapping separately because it is the most thoroughly explored foot performance measure. Gardner (1941) had subjects tap on the key of a telegraph switch with the big toe. Her trial duration was unusually long, four sessions of 75 s each. My experience indicates that with such trial durations, inexperienced subjects have great difficulty maintaining proper performance with either foot. This perhaps explains why only a minor speed advantage for the right foot was found. Gardner (1941) did comment that the left foot was often irregular and spasmodic in performance. Ruisel (1970) found a rightfoot superiority in tapping for 80% of a sample of 128 boys. Malmo and Andrews (1945) tested 100 men as a control sample in a study on the foot performance of polyneuropathic subjects. They found a clear right-foot advantage and a test-retest reliability of .92. Peters and Durding (1979a) also found a clear right-foot advantage in tapping for right-handers and a slight group advantage for the right foot in left-handers. Unlike finger tapping, in which relatively consistent speeds are found across studies, the absolute speeds in foot tapping appear to be sensitive to trial durations and the tapping apparatus. In the Malmo and Andrews study, the right foot managed, on average, 34.4 taps/10 s, whereas the left foot managed 30.4 taps/10 s. In the Peters and Durding (1979a) study, the corresponding figures were 53.2 and 51.4. In the former study, 30-s trials were used, whereas in the latter each foot performed ten 10-s trials. The Peters and Durding (1979a) study and the Gardner (1941) study agree in that both found faster foot tapping in men than in women. This corresponds to similar findings for finger tapping 185 (Peters & Durding, 1979b). Although the right-foot advantage for right-handers comes as no surprise, the fact that left-handers as a group showed a right-foot advantage is somewhat puzzling. In a subsequent study, Augustyn and Peters (1986b) reported two replications. Overall, in the three experiments in which the tapping performances of the right and the left foot were compared in right- and left-handers, right-handers showed a consistent right-foot advantage, whereas there was an inconsistent (across studies) right-foot advantage for left-handers. A left-foot advantage for left-footers was not found in any of the studies. Because all subjects in the three experiments were young adults, a possible source of a right-foot bias for left-handers cannot be ignored. In driving a car, the right foot operates the accelerator, and the movement that the foot makes by rotating around the ankle is similar to the movement that subjects make while tapping with the foot. A study of foot tapping in right- and lefthanded children below driving age would allow evaluation of this factor. Alternatively, adults who do not drive a car could be tested. Although there is a contaminant for this measure in adults, the speed of foot tapping is potentially a more suitable motor test for laterality in children than hand performance tests because the general transfer bias that benefits the preferred hand is less of a problem with the feet. When the hands are engaged in coordinated tapping of a simple rhythm in which one hand performs one tap for every two of the other hand, a clear role differentiation emerges in righthanders (Peters, 1985). Performance is better when the right hand takes the faster beat and the left hand takes the slower beat. In left-handers, no group asymmetry of this type can be seen. When right- and left-handed subjects are asked to perform the 2:1 task with the feet, the same pattern is observed; right-handers perform better when the right foot taps two beats to every one in the left foot. In the converse arrangement, performance tends to be slower and more variable (Peters, 1987). Left-handers show no such difference; performance tends to be similar with either foot-task combination. This finding supports the conjecture offered in the discussion of human-machine interactions, that left-handers as a group are less clearly committed to a particular laterality pattern than are right-handers. Other performance measures. It is to Gardner's (1941) credit that this entire section is concerned mostly with foot performance tasks studied by her. The tests are listed not so much because they allow a clear differentiation between right and left (in Gardner's, 1941, study they do not) but because they offer a point of departure for new standardized foot performance tests. Subjects were asked to grasp marbles with their toes and carry them to and drop them into a container, move a marble up an incline and maneuver it into a hole in a tin plate, grasp a rubber stopper and throw it 15 in. (38.1 cm) into a hole, and unlock bolt locks with their feet. These tasks were done under visual guidance, but additional tasks were done without visual guidance. Among these were placing pegs in a Wallin pegboard, placing shapes into the Seguin form board, grasping and placing clothespins, and lifting marbles. The right foot had an overall advantage in all these tasks, but this advantage was not impressive, except when subjects had to undo a bolt lock with their feet. Gardner (1941) provided no statistical analysis, but those who wish to can perform such an analysis on the basis of extensive reporting of summary data for all the subjects. Overall, This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 186 MICHAEL PETERS Gardner (1941) stated that men surpassed women in tasks in- Coren (1981) reported a Pearson product-moment correlation volving speed and accuracy under visual guidance but that coefficient of only .527 (accounting for 28% of the variance be- women performed better on tasks that were factually complex tween indexes of lateral preferences for hands and feet). In Eyre and that were done without visual guidance. In a subsequent and Schmeekle's (1933) sample, however, 50 of the 72 subjects study, Gardner (1942) explored the extent to which lateral ad- showed a correspondence for the preference of the right foot vantages could be explained by differences in foot sensitivity and the right hand, and 17 subjects showed a correspondence but came to the conclusion that the sensitivity advantage of the of left-hand and left-foot preference. Clymer and Silva (1985) right foot was quite minor. According to Gardner (1942), how- found a very strong association as well. In their sample, 585 ever, the sensitivity differences between the feet were larger than right-handers had a right-foot preference, compared with only those between the hands. Summarizing the performances on 37 right-handers who had a left-foot preference. There were 31 Gardner's (1941) imaginative measures, one can state that the left-handers with a left-foot preference and 14 left-handers with right-left differences were not very marked and that the lack a right-foot preference. These figures suggest a rather strong as- of marked performance differences was in contrast to the very sociation between hand and foot preference, particularly for strong right-foot preference for kicking that was shown by her sample. Gardner (1941) did indicate, however, that when the right-handers. In terms of tapping performance, Peters and Durding (1979a) found only a very weak correlation between performance asymmetry favors the left foot, the right-left hand and foot tapping speed, but they found, as did Malmo and differences in subjects with this pattern are not as large as the Andrews (1945), that the performances of the right and left foot performance differences between the feet for subjects who show were strongly correlated. a right-foot advantage. Singh (1970) used a strength measure; it required subjects to Little information is available on hand-foot relations in concurrent performance. This is an important area from the ergo- position themselves between two walls 3 ft (0.91 m) apart and nomics perspective because many machines (e.g., planes, grad- push against a measuring device with the foot while supported ers, forklifts, and cranes) require the finely coordinated use of against one wall with the back. There were no significant differ- hands and feet. The only experimental data pertaining to hand- ences in leg strength for right-handers (in both men and women foot coordination were found in Stetson's (1905) remarkable a slight majority pushed more strongly with the left, and 48.9% article on rhythmic performance. Stetson found that subjects of the 92 subjects showed equal strength), but in left-handers the who were required to beat triplets with various combinations left leg was stronger in 68% of the 25 subjects, with 8% showing of beats delivered by the hand and foot did well when a foot- greater strength in the right leg and 24% showing equal strength. hand-hand sequence with accent on the foot was delivered. Singh's findings, if replicated, are remarkable because they con- Subjects performed badly, however, when a hand-foot-hand or stitute the reverse of the pattern normally found in comparing a foot-hand-foot sequence was required. A discussion of the the performances of right- and left-handers; there is usually a reasons for this pattern is beyond the scope of this review, but it greater lateral asymmetry in right-handers and more evenly is possible that "inertia of attention" (Titchener, 1908, p. 242) matched performances in left-handers. Two additional studies interacts with a tendency to favor the hands when attention has (Carnahan, Elliott, & Lee, 1986; Rosenrot, 1980) provide evi- to be switched. Obviously, percussionists and, in particular, or- dence for left-foot superiority in strength or accuracy of force gan players would be attractive subjects for the study of precise production in right-handers. hand-foot coordination patterns. Finally, a number of foot performance tests have been used Gretmer (1947) compared the performance of the hands and in the context of motor assessment, without a particular focus feet in a task that simulated some aspects of piloting. The sub- on lateral performance asymmetry. I will discuss these with ref- jects had to compensate for movements of a pointer either with erence to neuropsychological testing. stick movement or wheel turning by using the hands or with rudder pedal action by using the feet. Gretmer found that foot Relation Between Hands and Feet When hand preference is correlated with other lateral preferences, such as eye and ear preferences, the correlation between performance on this task was significantly poorer than hand performance. The differences between hand and foot performance were confounded, however, by different types of movement. hand and foot preferences tends to be strongest (Annett & An electrophysiological study by Brunia, Voorn, and Berger Turner, 1974; Eyre &Schmeeckle, 1933; Porac & Coren, 1981; (1985) examined the magnitude and laterally effects of hand Searleman, 1980). In terms of the computation of correlations, and foot movement-related slow potentials in right- and left- the criteria used to determine hand and foot dominance are, of course, very important. This is illustrated by Searleman's find- in right- and left-handers the potentials for finger movements ing that footedness is a better predictor of direction and strength were larger in the hemisphere contralateral to the moving fin- of language lateralization than is handedness. Searleman used gers (i.e., larger over the left hemisphere when the fingers of the only two measures of foot preference, however, compared with right hand were moving), the potentials were larger in the hemi- seven measures of hand preference. The conclusions could have sphere ipsilateral to the movement in the case of foot move- been different if only two hand preference measures had been ment. Brunia et al. found no differences between right- and left- used. In addition, the problem of attenuation of correlation that handers in this regard. Does this mean that there is a fundamen- arises when variables crowd one end of the spectrum of possible level? It is true that in the motor homunculus for humans the values has to be taken into account. For instance, Porac and foot region occupies a much smaller area than does the hand handers. Brunia et al. reported an unexpected pattern. Whereas tal difference between hand and foot control at the cortical This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. FOOTEDNESS 187 region (Penfield & Rasmussen, 1950), but it is also true that the for the hand is also seen in the foot of left-handers (Chapman, foot area is proportionately quite large—larger than the com- Chapman, & Allen, 1987). bined areas for the knee and thigh. Observation of motor defi- Of the existing models of laterally (cf. Bryden, 1982), An- cits after pyramidal tract lesions suggests that the relation of the nett's (1978) model is perhaps best suited to account for the foot to the leg is comparable to that of the hand to the arm. above patterns. Annett's (1978) single-gene right-shift model is Brodal (1973) described, for instance, that after a right-sided unusual in that it proposes a right-shift tendency for right-hand- stroke, paralysis of the left side was quite complete. He noted ers but no directional predisposition for left-handers. In that that during recovery, whereas arm and leg movement (proximal sense, Annett's (1978) model can deal with the lack of an over- musculature) was possible, extension of the fingers—especially whelming foot asymmetry for left-handers and can also account the thumb—was impossible and that dorsiflexion of the toes for the strong right-foot preference of right-handers. Annett's and dorsiflexion and pronation of the left foot were completely (1978) model explicitly allows for an interaction among envi- abolished. In this sense, the specific motor innervation of hands ronmental, random, and genetic factors, and the occurrence of and feet seems quite similar. Boschert and Deecke (1986) sug- the occasional left-footed right-hander is not irreconcilable with the model. gested that the ipsilateral dominance of the readiness potential related to foot movement is not due to a fundamental difference Some theories suggest that it is the strength of lateral prefer- in the way in which hand and foot movement are organized. ences rather than the direction that is a heritable trait (Bryden, Instead, it is an artifact of the recording arrangement for such 1982; Collins, 1977), and these theories should be able to deal potentials; the ipsilateral predominance is due to the medial with left-handers relatively well. The literature shows that left- location of the foot representation and the direction in which handers tend to be somewhat less lateralized in preference and the negative dipole points on activation. performance than right-handers in both hand and feet, and the strength-of-laterality theories are not contradicted by a different direction of that laterality in hand and feet. Theories that Conclusions stress the role of either natural (Corballis & Morgan, 1978) or abnormal (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1984) development as a Evidence suggests that in humans the musculature and bones cause for handedness would also be able to deal with a lack of of the left leg are, on average, slightly larger than the muscula- concordance in hand and foot preference. Some quite specific ture and bones of the right leg. This applies to right-handers, assumptions, however, would have to be made about the devel- and the situation in left-handers is unclear. opmental schedules in the acquisition of motor control in the Whereas anatomical asymmetries may be subtle, functional hands and feet. Finally, a group of theories attributes the devia- asymmetries in the majority of people are marked. By and tion from right-handedness to pathological factors (e.g., Bakan, large, the concepts used in defining functional roles for the Dibb, & Reed, 1973). According to Bakan et al., all people are hands apply to the feet as well. In both cases, an action toward essentially right-handers unless they suffer brain damage during a goal is carried out most directly by the preferred limb, and the birth process, with a consequent switch in handedness. At support for that action is provided by the other limb. This prin- least with the populations that I have dealt with, there is no ciple expresses itself over a great range of movement classes. For indication of a motor impairment severe enough to lead a sub- instance, the goal may be kicking a ball (the other foot supports ject to switch a natural preference. The pathological model also and balances the body) or moving a car (the other foot operates has a problem in accounting for crossed preferences: If brain the clutch). Where hands and feet collaborate, this principle can damage is severe enough to force a switch from the right to the be extended to an entire body half. For instance, in operating a left hand, it should also produce a switch in footedness from motorcycle, the right hand (accelerator) and right foot (brake) right to left. In other words, left-handers should also be left- realize the goal of directly changing the rate of movement of footers. There are enough exceptions to such a concordance to the bike, whereas the left hand (gearshift) and left foot (clutch) question the theory in its general form. support the actions of the right hand and foot. In many cases, Beyond this, some general and some specific problems in as- foot action is somewhat subordinate to hand action. This subor- sessing foot preference relative to theories of handedness are dination can assume different degrees. Thus, in throwing the posed. A general problem is presented by whether the need to javelin, shot, hammer, or discus, the role of the feet is entirely determined by the choice of hand. Similarly, in shooting a rifle specialize foot use is strictly comparable to the need to specialize hand use. In daily activities, the hands are continually en- or using a bow and arrow, the foot stance is determined by the gaged in movements that require role differentiation, and it role of the hands. More subtle in terms of underlying causes is matters which hand does what in order to perform well. In con- the choice of the right foot in operating the mute pedal in sup- trast, there are only a few activities encountered by the average port of the hands in piano performance. In the examples given, the majority of right-handers use the person that require a role differentiation of the feet and in which it matters very much which foot does what. For instance, few right foot as the foot that most directly parallels the role of the people perform the kind of tasks used in the assessment of foot- right hand, and it is for this reason that the term preferred foot edness on a frequent basis, and this might be one reason for the has some face validity when applied to cases in which the feet less powerfully expressed lateral bias in foot use. Even in the act on or manipulate (Corballis, 1983) objects. The literature case of kicking, one of the few instances in which role differen- makes it clear that the right foot tends to be the preferred foot tiation of the feet matters in a practical way, only relatively few of right-handers. The situation remains unclear in left-handers, people use this movement in sports in a way that really matters. but the pattern of a reduced consistency in lateral preferences As a result, the choices made on foot preference questionnaires, This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 188 MICHAEL PETERS though they may be relatively consistent, do not reflect as strong a lateral bias as the choices made on the hand preference questionnaires. The lack of hand-foot congruence should therefore be seen relative to the fact that foot preference has a weaker status than hand preference. This latter premise is testable by examining the degree to which subjects can attain proficiency on a given task with the nonpreferred foot. It should be noted in this context that Kovac (1973) reported that about 13% of the 16-19-year-olds in his sample felt that they were equally adept with both feet; this information has to be seen relative to an astonishingly high 6%-7% in the same age group that claimed that they were equally adept with both hands. Finally, as has been pointed out in the context of foot use in athletics, foot preference is often determined by what the hands do, and for this reason alone, there must be sufficient flexibility in foot preference mechanisms to accommodate the hands. A more specific, methodological problem has to do with the classification of handedness, particularly with reference to the labeling of pure right- and left-handers. Generally, the \abelpure is conferred when a person shows complete lateral consistency in all choices. It is clear that the prevalence of pure handedness is quite dependent on the composition and length of a handedness catalog. For instance, there will be far more pure righthanders when the only questions in the catalog are "Which hand do you write with; which hand do you hammer with?" than when a longer questionnaire (e.g., Oldfield, 1971) is used. Depending on the makeup of the questionnaire, then, the proportion of cases with or without concordance for hand and foot can be quite different in different studies. Perhaps it would be useful to define primary items, as has been done in handedness questionnaires (Annett, 1970; Bryden, 1982), for foot preference questionnaires. Even in the case of the much studied hands, the question of how to weigh the items remains unresolved, and it is not surprising that even less thought has been given to this issue relative to foot preference. So much for the theoretical considerations of foot measures relative to handedness theories. But, however imperfectly expressed, there are observable lateral biases in foot use that emerge quite clearly from the review. In more practical terms, the patterns observed in studies can be summarized through a number of principles that will be of use in human-machine designs: 1. In the design of foot controls, the preferred foot should perform the actions that are more directly related to the goal of movement, and the nonpreferred foot should, whenever possible, be assigned supportive movements. 2. Where hands and feet collaborate, complementary roles should be assigned to the hand and foot on the same side. 3. If operations can be subdivided in terms of a hierarchy of complexity and the degree to which the movement is directly attended to, foot actions should be given a role subordinate to that of hand actions whenever the question of assigning a subordinate role to hand or foot arises. 4. For almost all right-handers, the preferred foot is the right foot. Left-handers, if they do not already have foot preference patterns similar to those of right-handers, appear to adapt quite readily to the arrangement that is suitable for right-handers. Neuropsychological Assessment of Foot Performance For the practical purposes of neuropsychological testing, motor performance is of interest in a variety of contexts. When apraxias are suspected, the basic ability of the patient to perform certain movements must be at the basis of any further examination. Specific motor deficits are tested for when particular cortical or subcortical areas are involved. Finally, there is frequently an interest in overall motor performance in the context of developmental difficulties or when the issue of clumsiness as a clinical entity is raised. Much of the interest in motor performance is limited to hand movement, but I make a case here in favor of examination of foot movement. The study of foot movement is of interest in itself, but a case can be made that tests of foot motor performance have broad implications in neuropsychological testing. To argue the point, I give several circumstantial observations. Those who get drunk on occasion or have an opportunity to observe drunks will note the peculiar fact that impairment of gait precedes impairment of hand movements. For instance, a drunk person may perform quite passably on the piano, only to stagger about markedly when rising from the piano bench. The principle of "the legs go first" can also be seen in a completely different context. In tick paralysis, for instance, caused by tick bites, impairment first affects the legs and then involves the upper limbs (Mongan, 1979). Related observations can be made in animals as well. When animals such as cats are anesthetized with barbiturates, they may pull themselves along with the front legs in the early phases of "going under," while the hindlegs may already be immobilized. Similar observations can be made when larger carnivores are hit with tranquilizer darts. These observations suggest that under certain conditions of impaired central nervous system function, impairments in foot movement are more sensitive indicators of trouble than are impairments in hand movements. For this reason alone, foot movement should be an important aspect of neuropsychological examinations. Two of the most commonly examined indicators of foot performance in the traditional literature are the milestones of standing and walking in infants. Here, too, the principle of better control in the upper extremities is clear: Infants can competently manipulate objects with their hands long before they master standing or walking. This is also seen in infants who have delayed motor milestones, as in Down's syndrome (Henderson, 1984). In a condition such as Down's syndrome, it is not clear whether foot movement is relatively more affected than hand movement because of an overall motor impairment. Difficulties in running and walking, however, are often among the most easily recognized signs. In some conditions, such as Asperger's syndrome (Burgoine & Wing, 1983), problems with gait and posture are among the obvious signs—at a distance—that something is not quite right. Gubbay, Ellis, Walton, and Court (1965) reported problems with gait and leg use in a group of children with below-normal intelligence and apractic and agnosic defects. Motor problems in retarded children are, of course, part of the general pattern of problems that the children encounter, and for this reason the assessment of motor deficits independent of the general condition is difficult. There is a category of children, This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. FOOTEDNESS 189 however, who possess full mental faculties, who do not have any provides standardized data for a somewhat broader range of obvious neurological deficits (such as cerebral palsy), and who foot motor categories is the Henderson revision of Stott's test do show motor disturbances. These children are labeled—or for motor impairment (Stott, Moyes, & Henderson, 1984). I mislabeled—as clumsy (Henderson, 1982). recommend this test as a promising tool for the assessment of Here the interest is in foot movement in such children rather foot movement. The test covers the age range of 5 to 12 years than in the overall pattern of motor problems. Even in the most and assesses, in four age bands, balance and variations of heel- general discussion of clumsy children (e.g., Illingworth, 1968), to-toe walking and jumping. The test lacks any items that assess there are comments about problems in foot coordination, such manipulative skill, and investigators might wish to add the ball- as in jumping and hopping. In Hulme, Smart, and Moran's rolling test to the battery of tasks. One aspect of foot perfor- (1982) study of clumsy children, rolling a ball with the feet and mance that has great ecological validity but is, for obvious rea- skipping with the feet were among the better quantitative indi- sons, rarely considered in the laboratory is the speed of running. cators of motor problems. Lesny (1980) suggested that in con- Perhaps a reasonable compromise is given by agility runs, in genital children's clumsiness, examination for cerebellar func- which an element of speed is combined with eye-foot coordina- tion tends to yield vermian rather than neocerebellar signs. Al- tion (Arnheim & Sinclair, 1975). Agility runs can be set up in a though Lesny did not discuss foot and leg clumsiness, lesions of limited space and allow time and error measures. Here, as in the vermis (the midline region of the cerebellum) are associated the ball-rolling test, normative data are lacking. with difficulties with the proximal musculature and, specifi- A general problem with available motor tests of foot behavior cally, gait disturbances. A dissociation between hands and feet is that although the tests are sensitive to developmental delays can be seen in the atrophy of the cortex of the anterior cerebel- useful in neuropsychological application, their construction is lum, where marked disturbances of gait can be seen together crude with regard to factors underlying foot control. For in- with only very mild impairment of the upper extremities (Dow, stance, impairments in the ball-rolling test might be due to 1958). Shaw, Levine, and Belfer (1982) in their investigation of problems with the vestibular system, with visuo-motor integra- the effects of clumsiness on self-esteem used such categories as tion, or with control of muscle tension, to name some possible falls easily, hopping in place, sideways tandem gait, and heel- factors. Do the tests of heel-to-toe walking or tests of balance toe alternate tapping. test different components from those of the ball-rolling test? No In motor tests for clumsiness, the foot test of ball rolling is, evidence is available to answer these questions. Perhaps the ap- according to one of the pioneers in investigation of clumsiness, plication of factor-analytic or dual-scaling techniques can an- Gubbay, one of the four motor tests that best discriminate be- swer these questions. Another problem lies in the failure of tests tween clumsy and control children (Gubbay, 1978). Even when to formally dissociate volitionally guided movements from a finer subdivision of clumsiness is made, foot movement re- largely automated movements. Such a dissociation is hinted at mains an important variable. Dare and Gordon (1970) divided by casual observation of motor impairment. For instance, it is clumsy children into three groups. Group 1 (specific develop- not uncommon for a person who is considered clumsy to dis- mental disorder) was the group with the mildest signs, but even play remarkable motor control in some movements. For exam- in this group, problems in running and hopping were clearly ple, Beethoven, who was considered the greatest pianist of his expressed. The aspects of leg movement are not always consis- time (Schonberg, 1970) was notoriously clumsy: "Badly coordi- tent. Reuben and Bakwin (1968), for instance, reported on a nated, he could never learn to dance" (p. 92). child who had normal gait and posture but was unable to hop. In the examination by Rasmussen and Gillberg (1983) of chil- I suggest that the discrepancy between clumsiness for some movements and a high degree of skill for others can often be dren with, in their words, "minor neuropsychological deficits" resolved with reference to the nature of the movements in- (p. 125), questions that related to walking, running, climbing, volved. Although practically all skilled movement depends on and hopping on one leg were among those that best discrimi- an intact reflex repertoire (Easton, 1972), the degree to which nated between these children and a control group. This brief summary of some findings in this area is not meant instance, a dancer or athlete cannot be proficient if there is a to be exhaustive but does indicate rather well that foot testing problem with the basic postural reflexes and their integration should be an integral part of motor testing in neuropsychology. with volitional behavior. If the dependency on postural and One problem is the lack of consistent assessment methods other inherent integrations is less, as in playing a musical instru- learned motor behaviors depend on this repertoire differs. For across studies, and another is that the evaluation of perfor- ment, the relative contribution of the motor systems that com- mance is rarely based on quantitative tests. Of the tests used pose new movement and those that provide the postural sup- that permit quantification, the ball rolling described by Gubbay port can be ascertained. An example, again from the cerebellar (1975) is by far the most common. Variations in procedure literature, clarifies the point: make comparisons across groups difficult, but the detailed description provided by Hulme, Smart, Moran, and Raine (1983) should permit standardization. The ball-rolling test requires subjects to dribble a ball around six objects laid out in a straight line on the floor, and the time taken in maneuvering the ball through this obstacle course is used as a measure. Penalties are It is common to see a child with a medulloblastoma (in the posterior vermis) walk and maintain balance very poorly, and yet when recumbent, so that the position of the body in space is maintained, or when the body is well braced, fail to show any evidence of the usual signs of cerebellar deficiency in the arms, legs or trunk. (Dow, 1958, p. 381) exacted if the ball touches the objects. This test is sensitive because it tests fine manipulation as well as balance. Standardized When considering the emphasis given lateral asymmetries, it data are not available. The only test of foot performance that may be surprising that in this section I have not commented on This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 190 MICHAEL PETERS the assessment of foot asymmetries relative to neuropsychological testing. The literature has little to say on this issue. A report by Strauss and Wada (1983), however, invites further investigation. They found in a sample of patients who had received a brain injury before the age of 1 year a significantly reduced prevalence of right-footedness, compared with a sample in which brain damage had occurred after the age of 1 year. If it is true that foot preference is generally less subject to cultural pressures than hand preference, the clinical interest in patterns of hand specialization and degree of lateral preferences should also be extended to a similar analysis for the feet. Summary The literature shows quite clearly that humans are rightfooted, if by footed one means the preferential use of one foot to act on and manipulate objects. By and large, footedness is determined not so much by a category of motor movement as by the extent to which a voluntary activity receives the weight of attention. Seen in this way, the specialization for the right foot can simply be seen within the larger context of a preference for focusing on the limbs of the right body half in the realization of a movement goal. The assessment of footedness in humans is complicated by the interactions between hand and feet, particularly those observed in athletics, dance, and the operation of machines and musical instruments. Assessment is further complicated by the fact that the activities of the feet are, relative to those of the hands, less complex and less often overlearned. Asa result, the lateral bias toward one or the other side may not be as compelling as for the hands. I have suggested that the assignment of limb-task combinations should, ideally, acknowledge the primacy of the hands over the feet and the right over the left in the average right-hander. This review also shows that the situation for left-handers is far from clear. Finally, I suggest that foot performance tests are useful adjuncts to neuropsychological testing because foot performance is a sensitive indicator of problems in motor control References Annett, M (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 303-321. Annett, M. (1978). A single gene explanation of right and left handedness and brainedness. Coventry, England: Lanchester Polytechnic. Annett, M., & Turner, A. (1974). Laterality and the growth of intellectual abilities. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 44, 37-46. Arnheim, D. D., & Sinclair, W. A. (1975). The clumsy child. St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby. Augustyn, C., & Peters, M. (1986a). Measures of foot preference and performance. Unpublished manuscript. Augustyn, C., & Peters, M. (1986b). On the relation between footedness and handedness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 1115-1118. Bakan, P., Dibb, G., & Reed, P. (1973). Handedness and birth stress. Neuropsychologia, 11, 363-366. Bell, C. (1870). The hand; its mechanism and vital endowments, as evincing design and illustrating the power, wisdom, and goodness of God. London: Bell & Daldy. Bernstein, S.(1981). With your own two hands. New York: G. Schirmer. Blau, A. (1946). The dominant hand (Research Monograph No. 5). New York: American Orthopsychiatric Association. Boschert, J., & Deecke, L. (1986). Handedness, footedness and finger and toe movement-related cerebral potentials. Human Neurobiology, 5, 235-243. Brodal, A. (1973). Self-observations and neuroanatomical considerations after a stroke. Brain, 96, 675-694. Brunia, C. H. M., Voom, E J., & Berger, M. P. F. (1985). Movement related to slow potentials. II. A contrast between finger and foot movements in left-handed subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60,135-145. Bryden, M. P. (1982). Laterality. New York: Academic Press. Burgoine, E., & Wing, L. (1983). Identical triplets with Asperger's syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 261-265. Carnahan, H., Elliott, D., & Lee, T. D. (1986). Dual-task interference between speaking and listening and a unipedal force production task. Neuropsychologia, 24, 582-586. Chapman, J. P., Chapman, L. J., & Allen, !. ). (1987). The measurement of foot preference. Neuropsychologia, 25, 579-584. Chaurasia, B. D. (1976). La position "Palthi" et ses relations avec les modes de croisement des mains et des bras. [The "Palthi" position and its relation to the crossing of hands and arms]. L'Anthropologie, 80, 325-334. Chibber, S. R., & Singh, I. (1970). Asymmetry in muscle weight and one sided dominance in the human lower limbs. Journal of Anatomy London, 106, 553-556. Chibber, S. R., & Singh, I. (1972). Asymmetry in muscle and bone weight in the lower limb of human foetuses. Journal of the Anatomical Society of India, 21, 33-39. Clymer, P. E., & Silva, P. A. (1985). Laterality, cognitive ability and motor performance in a sample of seven year olds. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 11, 59-68. Cohen, J. (1976). Handedness, speech pathology and hemispheric asymmetry. Triangle, 15, 93-101. Collins, R. L. (1977). Tbward an admissible genetic model for the inheritance of the degree and direction of asymmetry. In S. Harnad, R. W. Doty, J. Jaynes, L. Goldstein, & G. Krauthamer (Eds.), Lateralization in the nervous system (pp. 137-150). New \brk: Academic Press. Corballis, M. C. (1983). Human laterality. New \brk: Academic Press. Corballis, M. C., & Morgan, M. ). (1978). On the biological basis of human laterality: 1. Evidence for a maturational right-left gradient. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 261-269. Coryell, J., & Cardinal!, N. (1979). The asymmetric tonic neck reflex in normal full-term infants. Physical Therapy, 59, 747-753. Dare, M. T, & Gordon, N. (1970). Clumsy children: A disorder of perception and motor organization. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 12, 178-185. Delacato, C. H. (1963). The treatment and prevention of reading problems. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Dow, R. S. (1958). The physiology and pathology of the cerebellum. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Downey, J. (1927). Types of dextrality and their implications. American Journal of Psychology, 38, 317-367. Easton, T. A. (1972). On the normal use of reflexes. American Scientist, 60, 591-599. Eyre, M. B., & Schmeeckle, M. M. (1933). A study of handedness, eyedness and footedness. Child Development, 4,73-78. Gardner, L. P. (1941). Experimental data on the problem of motor lateral dominance in feet and hands. Psychological Records, 5, 1-63. Gardner, L. P. (1942). Experimental data on the problem of sensory lateral dominance in feet and hands. Psychological Records, 5, 66124. Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1984). Cerebral dominance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gould, G. M. (1908). Right-handedness and left-handedness. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott. Gretmer, W. F. (1947). A study of several design factors influencing hu- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. FOOTEDNESS man efficiency in hand and foot operation of controls. American Psychologist, 2, 340. Gubbay, S. (1975). The clumsy child. London: Saunders. Gubbay, S. (1978). The management of developmental apraxia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 20, 643-646. Gubbay, S. S., Ellis, E., Walton, J. N., & Court, S. D. M. (1965). Clumsy children, a study of apraxic and agnosic defects in 21 children. Brain, 88, 295-312. Haefner, R. (1930). The relation between hand and foot tendencies of children. Pedagogical Seminary, 38, 338. Harris, A. J. (1958). Harris Tests of Lateral Dominance. New York: Psychological Corporation. Harris, L. J. (1983). Laterality of function in the infant: Historical and contemporary trends in theory and research. In S. "Vfoung, S. J. Segalowitz, C. M. Corter, & S. Trehub (Eds.), Manual specialization and the developing brain (pp. 177-247). New York: Academic Press. Henderson, S. E. (1982). Concomitants of clumsiness in young schoolchildren. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 24, 448- 460. Henderson, S. E. (1984). Motor skill development. In D. Lane & B. Stratford (Eds.), Current approaches to Down's syndrome (pp. 187218). London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Hulme, C, Smart, A., & Moran. G. (1982). Visual perceptual deficits in clumsy children. Neuropsychologia, 20, 475-481. Hulme, C, Smart, A., Moran, G., & Raine, A. (1983). Visual, kinaesthetic and cross-modal development: Relationships to motor development. Perception, 12,477-483. Illingworth, R. S. (1968). The clumsy child. Clinical Pediatrics, 7, 539543. Ingelmark, B. E. (1947). Uber die Langenasymmetrien der Extremitaten und ihren Zusammenhang mit der Rechts-Links-handigkeit [Asymmetries in the length of the extremities and their relation to right- and left-handedness]. Upsala Laekarefoerening Forhandlingar, 52, 17-82. Irving, D., Rebeiz, J. J., & Tomlinson, B. E. (1974). The numbers of limb motor neurons in the individual segments of the human lumbo-sacral cord. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 21, 203212. Kamptner, N. L., Comwell, K. S., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Harris, L. J. (1985). Motor asymmetries in the human infant: Stepping movements. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6,145-157. Kimura, D., & Davison, W. (1975). Right arm superiority for tapping with distal and proximal joints. Journal of Motor Behavior, I, 199202. Komai, X, & Fukuoka, G. (1934). A study of the frequency of lefthandedness and left-footedness among Japanese school children. Human Biology, 6,33-42. Kovac, D. (1973). Measuring lateral preference on the basis of subjective data. Studio Psychologica, IS, 235-246. Kovac, D., & Horkovic, G. (1970). How to measure lateral preference: I. Studia Psychologica, 12,5-11. Lesny, I. A. (1980). Developmental dyspraxia-dysgnosia as a cause of congenital children's clumsiness. Brain Development, 2,69-11. Levy, J., & Levy, J. M. (1978). Human lateralization from head to foot: Sex-related factors. Science, 200, 1291 -1292. Levy, J., & Levy, J. M. (1981). Foot-length asymmetry, sex, and handedness. Science, 212, 1418-1419. Ludwig, W. (1970). Das Rechls-Links Problem im Tierreich und beim Menschen [The right-left problem in the animal kingdom and in man]. West Berlin, FRG: Springer-Verlag. Malmo, R. B., & Andrews, M. L. (1945). A recording device for foottapping, with results from polyneuropathic subjects. American Journal of Psychology, 58, 247-252. Mascie-Taylor, C., MacLarnon, A. M., Lanigan, P. M., & McManus, 191 I. C. (1981). Foot-length asymmetry, sex, and handedness. Science, 212, 1416-1417. Means, L. W, & Walters, R. E, (1982). Sex, handedness and asymmetry of hand and foot length. Neuropsychologia, 20,115-719. Melekian, B. (1981). Lateralization in the newborn at birth: Asymmetry of the stepping reflex. Neuropsychologia, 19, 707-711. Mongan, P. F. (1979). Tick toxicosis in North America. The Journal of Family Practice, 8, 939-944. Nachshon, I., & Denno, D. (1986). Birth order and laterality preferences. Cortex, 22, 567-578. Nachshon, I., Denno, D., & Aurand, S. (1983). Lateral preferences of hand, eye and foot: Relation to cerebral dominance. International Journal of Neurosciences, 18, 1-10. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburg Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113. Orsini, D. L., & Satz, P. (1985). Pedal asymmetry and handedness. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7,127-129. Penfield, W, & Jasper, H. (1954). Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain. Boston: Little, Brown. Penfield, W, & Rasmussen, A. T. (1950). Cerebral cortex of man. A clinical study of localization of function. New York: Macmillan. Penfield, W., & Robertson, J. S. (1943). Growth asymmetry due to lesions of the postcentral cerebral cortex. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, SO, 405-430. Peters, M. (1981). Attentional asymmetries during concurrent bimanual performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 95-103. Peters, M. (1983). Differentiation and lateral specialization in motor development. In G. Young, S. J. Segalowitz, C. M. Corter, & S. Trehub (Eds.), Manual specialization and the developing brain (pp. 141-159). New York: Academic Press. Peters, M. (1985). Constraints in the coordination of bimanual movements and their expression in skilled and unskilled subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 171-196. Peters, M. (1986). [Motor screening tests for a population of Canadian Indian children.] Unpublished raw data. Peters, M. (1987). A nontrivial difference in motor performance between righthanders and lefthanders: Attention as intervening variable in the expression of handedness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 91-99. Peters, M., & Durding, B. M. (1979a). Footedness of left- and righthanders. American Journal of Psychology, 92, 133-142. Peters, M., & Durding, B. M. (1979b). Lefthanders and righthanders compared on a motor task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 11, 103-111. Peters, M., & Petrie, B. F. (1979). Functional asymmetries in the stepping reflex of human neonates. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 198-200. Peters, M., Petrie, B., & Oddie, D. (1981). Foot-length asymmetry, sex, and handedness. Science, 212, 1417-1418. Pomerantz, A. P., & Harris, L. (1980). Are there sex-related asymmetries in foot size? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 675-678. Porac, C., &Coren, S. (1981). Lateral preferences and human behavior. New \brk: Springer-Verlag. Raczkowski, D., Kalat, J. W, & Nebes, R. (1974). Reliability and validity of some handedness questionnaire items. Neuropsychologia, 12, 43-47. Rasmussen, P., & Gillberg, C. (1983). Perceptual motor and attentional deficits in seven-year-old children. Ada Paediatrica Scandinavica, 72, 125-130. Reuben, R. N., & Bakwin, H. (1968). Developmental clumsiness. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 15,601 -611. Rogers, L. J. (1980). Lateralization in the avian brain. Bird Behavior, 2, 1-12. Rosenrot, P. (1980). Asymmetry of gait and lower limb dominance. Un- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 192 MICHAEL PETERS published master's thesis, Univeisity of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada. Ruisel, I. (1970). Crossed laterality of the upper and lower extremities. Stadia Psychologies 17, 71-73. Schonberg, H. C. (1970). The lives of the great composers. New York: W. W. Norton. Searleman, A. (1980). Subject variables and cerebral organization. Cortex, 16, 239-254. Shaw, L. S., Levine, M. D., & Belfer, M. (1982). Developmental double jeopardy: A study of clumsiness and self-esteem in children with learning problems. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 3,191196. Singh, I. (1970). Functional asymmetry in the lower limbs. Ada Anatomica. 77, 131-138. Smith, L. G. (1917). A brief survey of right- and left-handedness. Pedagogical Seminary, 24, 19-35. Stern, G., & Schilf, E. (1932). Uber Linksfussigkeit. [About left-footedness). Zeitschrift fur die gesamte Neurologic und Psychiatric, 139, 41-43. Stetson, R. H. (1905). A motor theory of rhythm and discrete succession. II. Psychological Review, 12, 293-350. Stott, D. H., Moyes, F. A., & Henderson, S. E. (1984). Manual of motor impairment, Guelph, Ontario, Canada: Brook Educational Publications. Strauss, E., & Wada, J. (1983). Lateral preference and cerebral speech dominance. Cortex, 19,165-177. Thelen, E., Ridley-Johnson, R., & Fisher, D. M. (1983). Shifting patterns of bilateral coordination and lateral dominance in the leg movements of young infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 29-46. Titchener, E. B. (1908). Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. New York: Macmillan. Trehub, S. E., Corter, C. M., & Shosenberg, N. (1983). Neonatal reflexes: A search for lateral asymmetry. In G. Tibung, S. i. Segalowitz, C. M. Corten & S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Manual specialization and the developing brain (pp. 257-274). New York: Academic Press. von Bonin, F. (1962). Anatomical asymmetries of the cerebral hemispheres. In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Yanowitz, P. G., Satz, P., & Heilman, K. M. (1981). Foot-length asymmetry, sex, and handedness. Science, 212,1418. Zelazo, P. R. (1983). The development of walking: New findings and old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 99-137. Received October 13,1986 Revision received March 27, 1987 Accepted July 15, 1987 •