https://canvas.liberty.edu/courses/639581/pages/watch-introductionto-cognitive-psychology?module_item_id=67326535 Welcome to Introduction to Psychology, also known as Psychology 310, which is offered by the Department Psychology at Liberty University. Chapter 1, overview. Cognitive psychology is the study of how our minds receive, store, and use information. This includes theory in research about perception, attention, memory, language use, decision-making and problem-solving. The primary development of Cognitive psychology began in part as a reaction to the behaviorist tradition within psychology. But it's also a reflection. Optimal developments within their discipline including biology, linguistics, and computer science. Explanations of cognitive processes have been developed within three general approaches: Representationalist, embodied, and biologically motivated. Three main types of research design are employed and research and cognition. Case studies, correlational studies, and experiments. There are a range of behaviors studied by cognitive psychologists. Some common measures. Our accuracy for a task, as well as a toll to go to sleep a task. They are also behavior specific to an area of cognitive psychologist. such as, measurement of brain activity and Page 1 of 165 cognitive neuroscience. The primary learning objectives for Chapter 1 include addressing what is cognitive psychology? How have cognitive psychologists approach the study of cognition? What types of research methods are useful in the study of cognition? What behaviors to psychologists observe to study cognition? Some of the concepts in Chapter 1 include defining what cognition is. Beyond just thinking about mental processes. For example, our perceptions of the world around us. To our senses. Attentional processes that allow us to focus on a particular stimulus while not attending to others. Things like how memory operates to allow us to remember information and intentions. Our language processes that help us communicate our thoughts and ideas, not just to others, but also within our own thinking. cognitive psychology also includes research and work in decision-making. And allowed this is underscored by arching brain activity that controls all of these processes. In many ways, cognitive psychology bridges cognition with all other areas of psychology to that and that this course has a real benefit to students in that not only are you learning a very, very important, important applied area of psychology, but the area of cognitive psychology civically should teach you and help you understand the ways in which you can be a better learner. And that includes being more efficient and more effective at what you do. So throughout this course, you should be able to apply core concepts and ideas to the work that you're doing in this class and other classes. Because cognitive psychology is all about our thinking, This also applies directly to you and your unique developmental point in time. So hopefully by the time this course is over, you will have in hand skills at understanding your own ways of understanding with approaches to improve them. And of course, the better you understand your own thinking, you'll be able to apply that to the thinking of others. There's, there's no area of psychology that's not Page 2 of 165 more beneficial than cognitive psychology, especially in the workforce. Now of course, the Bible has a lot to say about our thinking about what's in our hearts and what's in our minds, not as reflected in the way the Bible talks about. Do, DO ways in which we think and act. For example, Philippians four, for rejoice in the Lord always again, I will say rejoice that your reasonableness be known to everyone. The Lord is at hand. Do not be anxious about anything, but in everything by prayer, supplication with thanksgiving, let your requests be made known to God. And the peace of God, which surpasses all understanding, will guard your hearts and your minds. Christ, Jesus, bombing, brothers, whatever is true, whatever is honorable, whatever is just, whatever is pure, whatever is lovely, whatever is commendable. There's any excellence. If there's anything worthy of praise, think about these things. What you have learned and received and heard and seen in me, practice these things. And the God of peace will be with you. As you can see in this scripture, the entire focus of it is on our thinking, because our thinking becomes our reality. So the Bible asks us to focus on Christ and having a Christ-like mindset and to model that. Page 3 of 165 https://canvas.liberty.edu/courses/639581/pages/watch-howreliable-is-your-memory-dr-elizabeth-beth-loftus-ted-talk-on-falsememories?module_item_id=67326545 I'd like to tell you about a legal case that I worked on involving a man named Steve Titus. Titus was a restaurant manager. He was 31 years old. He lived in Seattle, Washington. He was engaged to Gretchen, about to be married. She was the love of his life. And one night the couple went out for a romantic restaurant meal. They were on their way home and they were pulled over by a police officer. You see Titus, his car sort of resembled a car that was driven earlier in the evening by a man who raped a female hitchhiker. And Titus kind of resembled that rapist. So the police took a picture of Titus. They put it in a photo lineup. They later showed it to the victim and she pointed to Titus his photos. She said That one's the closest. The police and the prosecution proceeded with a trial. And when Steve Titus was put on trial for rape, the rape victim gone on the stand and said, I'm absolutely positive, that's the man. And Titus was convicted. He proclaimed his innocence. His family screamed at the jury. His fiance collapsed on the floor sobbing, and Titus is taken away to jail. So what would you do at this point? What would you do? Well, Titus lost complete faith in the legal system, and yet he got an idea. He called up the local newspaper. He got the interests of an investigative journalist. And that journalist actually found the real rapist, a man who ultimately confessed to this rape. A man who was thought to have committed 50 rapes in that area. When this information was given to the judge, the judge set Titus free. And Page 4 of 165 really that that's where this case should've ended. That should have been over a Titus should have thought of this as a horrible year, a year of accusation and trial, but over, it didn't end that way. Titus was so bitter. He'd lost his job. He couldn't get it back. He lost his fiance. She couldn't put up with his persistent anger. He lost his entire savings, and so he decided to file a lawsuit against the police and others whom he felt were responsible for his suffering. And that's when I really started working on this case, trying to figure out how did that victim go from That one's the closest to I'm absolutely positive. That's the guy. While Titus was consumed with his civil cases. And he spent every waking moment thinking about it. And just days before he was to have his day in court, he woke up in the morning, doubled over in pain and died of a stress-related heart attack. He was 35 years old. So I was asked to work on Titus his case because I'm a psychological scientist. I study memory. I've studied memory for decades. And if I meet somebody on an airplane, it has happened on the way over to Scotland. If I meet somebody on an airplane and we ask each other, what do you do? What do you do? And I say I study memory. They usually want to tell me how they have trouble remembering names or they've got a relative who's got Alzheimer's or some kind of memory problem. But, but I have to tell them. I don't study. When people forget. I study the opposite when they remember, when they remember things that didn't happen, or remember things that were different from the way they really were. Study false memories. Unhappily. Steve Titus is not the only person to be convicted based on somebody's false memory. In one project in the United States, information has been gathered on 300 innocent people, 300 defendants who were convicted of crimes they didn't do. They spent 10, 20, 30 years in prison for these crimes. And now DNA testing has proven that they're actually innocent. And when those cases IBM and analyzed, three quarters of them are due to faulty memory, faulty eyewitness memory. Well, why? Like the jurors who Page 5 of 165 convicted those innocent people and the jurors who convicted Titus. Many people believe that that memory works like a recording device. You just record the information. Then you call it up and play it back when you want to answer questions or identify images. But decades of work in psychology has shown that this just isn't true. Our memories are constructive, they're reconstructive. Memory works a little bit more like a Wikipedia page. You can go in there and change it. But so can other people. I first started studying this constructive memory process in the 1970s. I did my experiments that involve showing people simulated crimes and accidents and asking them questions about what they remember. In one study, we showed people a simulated accident and we asked people how fast were the cars going when they hit each other. And we asked other people how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other? And if we asked the leading smashed question, the witness has told us the cars were going faster. And moreover, that leading smashed question caused people to be more likely to tell us that they saw broken glass in the accident scene when there wasn't any broken glass at all. In another study, we showed a simulated accident where a car went through an intersection with a stop sign. And if we asked a question that insinuated it was a yield sign. Many witnesses told us they remember seeing a yield sign at the intersection, not a stop sign. And you might be thinking, well, you know, these are filmed events. They're not particularly stressful with the same kind of mistakes be made with a really stressful event. In a study we published just a few months ago, we have an answer to this question. Because what was unusual about this study is we arranged for people to have a very stressful experience. The subjects in the study were members of the US military who were undergoing a harrowing training exercise to teach them what it's going to be like for them if they are ever captured as prisoners of war. And as part of this training exercise, these soldiers are interrogated in an Page 6 of 165 aggressive, hostile, physically abusive fashion for 30 minutes. And later on they have to try to identify the person who conducted that interrogation. And when we feed them suggestive information that insinuates it's a different person. Many of them misidentify their interrogator, often not identifying someone who doesn't even remotely resemble the real interrogator. And so what these studies are showing is that when you feed people misinformation about some experience that they may have had, you can distort or contaminate or change their memory. Well out there in the real-world, misinformation is everywhere we get miss information. Not only if we're question in a leading way, but if we talk to other witnesses who might consciously or inadvertently fetus some erroneous information. Or if we see media coverage about some event we might have experienced. All of these provide the opportunity for this kind of contamination of our memory. In the 1990s, we began to see an even more extreme kind of memory problem. Some patients we're going into therapy with one problem. Maybe they had depression, eating disorder, and they were coming out of therapy with a different problem. Extreme memories for her horrific brutalization, sometimes in satanic rituals, sometimes involving really bizarre and unusual elements. One woman came out of psychotherapy believing that she'd endured years of ritualistic abuse where she was forced into a pregnancy and that the baby was cut from her belly. But there were no physical scars or any kind of physical evidence that could have supported her story. And when I began looking into these cases, I was wondering where do these bizarre memories come from. And what I found is that most of the situations involved some particular form psychotherapy. And so I asked where some of the things going on in this psychotherapy, like the imagination exercises or dream interpretation, or in some cases hypnosis, or in some cases, exposure to false information where these leading these patients to develop these Page 7 of 165 very bizarre, unlikely memories. And I designed some experiments to try to study the processes that were being used in this psychotherapy. So I could study the development of these very rich false memories. In one of the first studies we did, we use suggestion, a method inspired by the psychotherapy we saw in these cases, we use this kind of suggestion and planted a false memory that when you were a kid five or six years old, you were lost in a shopping mall. You were frighten, you were crying. You were ultimately rescued by an elderly person and reunited with a family. And we succeeded and planting this memory in the minds of about a quarter of our subjects. And you might be thinking, well, that's not particularly stressful. But we and other investigators have planted rich false memories of things that were much more unusual and much more stressful. So when a study done in Tennessee, researchers planted the false memory that when you were a kid, you nearly drowned and had to be rescued by a lifeguard. And in a study done in Canada, researchers planted the false memory that when you were a kid, something as awful as being attacked by a vicious animal happened to you, succeeding with about half of their subjects. And in a study done in Italy, researchers planted the false memory. When you were a kid, you witnessed demonic possession. I do want to add that it might seem like we are traumatizing these experimental subjects in the name of science. But our studies have gone through thorough evaluation by research ethics boards that have made the decision that the temporary discomfort that some of these subjects might experience in these studies is outweighed by the importance of this problem for understanding memory processes and the abuse of memory that is going on in some places in the world. Well, to my surprise, when I publish this work and began to speak out against this particular brand of psychotherapy. It created some pretty bad problems for me. Hostilities primarily from the repressed memory therapists who felt under attack and by the patients whom they had Page 8 of 165 influenced. I had sometimes armed guards at speeches that I was invited to give people trying to drum up letter writing campaigns to get me fired. But probably the worst was I suspected that a woman was innocent of abuse. That was being claimed by her grown daughter. She accused her mother of sexual abuse based on a repressed memory. And this accusing daughter had actually allowed her story to be filmed and presented in public places. I was suspicious of this story, and so I started to investigate and eventually found information that convinced me that this mother was innocent. I published an expose a on the case. And a little while later, the accusing daughter filed a lawsuit. Even though I'd never mentioned her name. She sued me for defamation and invasion of privacy. And I went through nearly five years of dealing with this messy, an unpleasant litigation. But finally, finally it was over and I could really get back to my work in the process, however, I became part of a disturbing trend in America where scientists are being sued for simply speaking out on matters of great public controversy. When I got back to my work, I ask this question. If I plant a false memory in your mind, does it have repercussions? Does it affect your later thoughts, your later behaviors? Our first study planted a false memory that you've got sick because a child eating certain foods, hard-boiled eggs, dill pickles, strawberry ice cream. And we found that once we planted this false memory, people didn't want to eat the foods as much at an outdoor picnic. The false memories aren't necessarily bad or unpleasant. If we planted a warm, fuzzy memory involving a healthy food like asparagus, we could get people to want to eat experiments more. And so what these studies are showing is that you can plant false memories and they have repercussions that affect behavior long after the memories take hold. Well, along with this ability to plant memories and control behavior, obviously come some important ethical issues. Like when should we use this mine technology and should we ever ban its use? Therapists can't ethically plant false memories in the minds of their patients, even if Page 9 of 165 it would help the patient. But there's nothing to stop a parent from trying this out on their overweight or obese teenager. And when I suggested this publicly, it created outcry again. There she goes. She's advocating the parents lie to their children. Hello, Santa Claus. I mean, another way to do well, another way to think about this is which would you rather have a kid with obesity diabetes, shortened lifespan, all the things that go with it. Or a kid with one little extra bit of false memory. I know what I would choose for a kid of mine, but maybe my work has made me different from most people. Most people cherish their memories, know that they represent their identity, who they are, where they came from. And I appreciate that. I feel that way too. But I know from my work how much fiction is already in there. If I've learned anything from these decades of working on these problems, it's this just because somebody tells you something and they say it with confidence, just because they say it with lots of detail. Just because they express emotion when they say it. It doesn't mean that it really happened. We can't reliably distinguish true memories from false memories. We need independent corroboration. Such a discovery has made me more tolerant of them everyday memory mistakes that my friends and family members make. Such a discovery might have saved Steve Titus, the man whose whole future was snatched away by a false memory. But meanwhile, we should all keep in mind. We do well to that memory. Like liberty is a fragile thing. Thank you. Thank you very much. Page 10 of 165 https://canvas.liberty.edu/courses/639581/pages/watch-cognitionhow-your-mind-can-amaze-and-betray-you-crash-coursepsychology-number-15?module_item_id=67326555 Why do smart people make dumb decisions? Do conspiracy theorists think that we didn't land on the moon or that Hillary Clinton is a space alien. And why won't Bernice admit that the new Superman movie just isn't very good. And we've talked about cognition before. We usually refer to it as the process that we used to think and solve crossword puzzles and stuff. But really, cognition involves knowing, remembering, understanding, communicating, and to a certain extent, learning and as truly wonderful as our brains are, we can be spectacularly bad at all of these things. We used to think that our cognition worked like a computer logically processing information. But that cabin sized chunk of pink wet brain matter in your skull can do a lot more than math. And the things that it does are certainly not always logical. Many experts argue that it's cognition that makes us truly human. That everything that comes with it, our preferences, prejudices, fears and intuitions are what make us the individuals that we are not the only animals that show some evidence of cognition. Of course, Page 11 of 165 chimps and gorillas exhibit insight and planning. Crows use tools, elephants teach each other, but our capacity as humans to figure stuff out is matched only by our ability to totally misjudge stuff as prone as we are to brilliance and insight, we're equally likely to succumb to irrational thinking and false intuition. So to borrow a riff from Rene Descartes, you think therefore you are, which means you are brilliant a lot of the time. And sometimes you just kinda look stupid. We all want to make sense of the world. And one of the major ways our cognition allows us to do that is by forming concepts, mental groupings of similar objects, people, ideas or events, we like to lump things together. Concepts simplify our thinking in such a fundamental way that we usually don't have to stop and think about using them. They're just there. And yet without concepts, we'd need a unique name for everything. You couldn't just ask me to shake the angler fish because there'd be no concept of shake or fish, let alone stuffed blue angler fish. And if I told you that I was devastated that I lost my angler fish, which I probably would be also have to explain my emotions, their intensities, even the words themselves that I had to use. So basically, without concepts, no one would ever get anything done. We'd all be like a bunch of ants taking all morning to say. We often organize our concepts by forming prototypes, mental images or pinnacle examples of a certain thing. For example, if I say bird, the general shape of a songbird probably pops into your head before like a penguin or chicken or emu. Because robins and cardinals more closely resemble our bird prototype. Still, if I show you a picture of some crazy creature you've never seen before and you note that it has feathers and a beak. You'll probably file it under the bird category because it more closely resembles your concept of bird than your concept of rodent or overcoat or footstool. Concepts and prototypes speed up our thinking, but they also can box our thinking in then lead to prejudice if we see something that doesn't fit our prototypes. A 100 years ago, the sight of a female doctor might have caused Page 12 of 165 some heads to explode because in peoples tiny minds, the prototypes of doctor and women didn't have any overlap. And actually some people today still feel that way haters going to hate. So it's important to actively keep your mind open to make room for evolving concepts. And remember that concepts may sometimes hurt as much as they help. One of the biggest ways our cognition works to our benefit though, through our ability to solve problems, we use our problem-solving skills all the time How to assemble Scandinavian furniture, bake muffins with a missing ingredient or handle the crushing disappointment of the new Superman movie. And we approach problem-solving in different ways. Sometimes we value speed, other times accuracy, some problems we figure out using trial and error and try something and if it doesn't work, try a different way and keep at it until something works. Trial and error is slow and deliberate, which may be good or bad depending on the problem. We can also use algorithms and heuristics to come up with some solutions. Algorithms are logical, methodical, step-by-step procedures that guarantee an eventual solution. They may be slow to work through. Heuristics, on the other hand, are more like mental shortcuts, simple strategies that allow us to solve problems faster, although they're more error-prone than algorithms, say at a grocery store, I'm looking for a family size bottle of sriracha. You could use an algorithm and methodically check every shelf and aisle store or you could use heuristics and first search the Asian or condiment sections, the places that make the most sense based on what you already know. Heuristics may be way faster, but the algorithmic approach guarantees you won't overlook the sauce along the way because they stuck it in the deli or whatever dumb thing they did this week. So algorithms, heuristics and trial and error are problem-solving strategies that involve a plan of attack. But sometimes we get lucky while puzzling out a problem and out of nowhere, a sudden flash of insight that solve the problem. I'll use orange in the muffin recipe instead of Page 13 of 165 lemon or sriracha. It lives in the Mexican section. For some reason, neuroscientists have actually watched that kind of sudden happy brain flash on neuroimaging screens. In one experiment, they gave subjects a problem to solve What word can be added to the three words, crab, pine, and sauce to create a new compound word. Then they asked the subjects to press a button when they had the answer or the subjects thought about it, scans showed activity in their frontal lobes, the areas involved in the focused attention of typical problem-solving. But right at that aha moment, just as they pushed the button, there was a clear burst of activity. Just above the ear in the right temporal lobe, which among many other things, is involved with recognition. The answer, by the way, we already gave you the hint earlier in the episode. Where the fish, those sudden bursts of insight are awesome, but you can't always count on them to solve all your problems. And just because something feels right doesn't mean it's truly correct. Because as inventive and smarty pants as we may be, our cognition often leads us astray in all kinds of ways. For instance, we often look for and favor evidence that verifies our ideas. While we're more likely to avoid or ignore contradictory evidence, a tendency known as confirmation bias. This is really similar to the overconfidence we've talked about when you're basically more confident than you are correct, when this kind of cognitive bias takes hold, you might cling to your initial conceptions in a kind of belief perseverance, even in the face of clear proof to the contrary, this happens all the time and it can be maddening for people watching it happen. People still think that the earth is flat. It's like what, how theirs space pictures probably don't need to tell you. People can get really weird and defensive when they evade facts and choose to see only the information that confirms their beliefs, they may even become functionally fixed, unable to view a problem from a new perspective. Instead, they just keep approaching a situation with the same mental set, especially if it's worked in the past. So you gotta now thicken out from a board and you're like, I need to Page 14 of 165 take care of that. There's rocks and bricks all around you. But because of your functional fixedness on the idea that only hammers work on nails. You don't even consider hitting it with the brick, and instead you waste a bunch of time looking for the hammer in your garage and you're angry, frustrated, and there's still a nail sticking up the board. So our mental set predisposes how we think, just as you'll remember that our perceptual set predisposes how we perceive fence is what makes heuristics, those super convenient mental shortcuts that we all use so easily fallible. And the 19 seventies cognitive psychologists, Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman researched how we make snap judgments and discovered one way, smart people make dumb decisions. They found that people believe an event will be more likely to occur if they can conjure up examples or memories of it, especially if those examples are particularly vivid, scary, or awesome. So say you're in a casino and you win $2 at a slot machine, suddenly every flashing light and ringing bell in the place goes off but when you lose. The vast majority of the time. It's just crickets and all their lights and noise making the casino makes sure that wins are super vivid and memorable while losses just go away unacknowledged that way. The next time you're standing there with a 100 bucks in your pocket, you're more likely to overestimate your chances of winning because the memories of winning are more striking, the more mentally available those memories are, the more it seemed that it's going to happen. Again, this is known as the availability heuristic and it can warp our judgements of people too. If we keep remembering news footage that shows people of a given group shooting guns, that can shape our impression of the entire group, even if what we saw was only a tiny minority within that group. Essentially, we are great at fearing the wrong things. We worry about being killed in a plane crash or getting bitten in half by a shark or accidentally choking on a dumpling. Thanks to our brains be role of horrific images we come to fear what's actually very rare Page 15 of 165 instead of worrying about much more common but less memorable ends like car accidents, cancer and heart failure are thinking can also be swayed by framing or how an issue is presented. Imagine you're considering climbing Everest or getting a nose job or eating a bowl of raw Blowfish. I can frame the risks in different ways, telling you that you've got a 95 percent chance of survival sounds a lot different than saying five out of a 100 people die doing this activity. The information is the same. Our cognitive minds are capable of incredible intellectual feats and tremendous failures. We can solve problems better than any organism on the planet. But given the chance, we can also mess up a pretty simple judgment every day of the week. But if we're mindful of our capacity for air and if we honor our ingenuity and intellect, I think our ability to solve any problem is nearly infinite, and that gives me a lot, I hope seriously though, where is my fish today, you learned how we use concepts, prototypes, and our mental sets to think and communicate and how algorithms, heuristics and insight help us solve problems. You also learned about how fixation, the availability, heuristic, fear, overconfidence, and belief perseverance can get in the way of good decision-making and thinking. Thank you for watching, especially to our sub-level subscribers who make this whole channel possible. If you'd like to sponsor an episode of Crash Course, get a special Laptop Diego, or even be animated into an upcoming episode. Just go to separable.com slash Crash Course. This episode was written by Kathleen Yale, edited by Blake de best Dino and our consultant is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwati. Our director and editor is Nicholas Jenkins. The script supervisor is Michael Rhonda, who is also our sound designer. And the graphics team is Thought Cafe. Page 16 of 165 An Introduction to Applied Cognitive Psychology with Christian & Ethical Integration LEARNING OBJECTIVES ▸ Define cognitive psychology and review major areas of study within cognitive psychology. ▸ Explain why cognitive psychology is ideally suited for ethical integration. ▸ Explain why cognitive psychology is ideally suited for Christian integration. ▸ List and elaborate on the goals of psychology and cognitive psychology. ▸ Define and describe the importance of a worldview in the context of psychology. ▸ Differentiate between different religious worldviews, including their unique benefits and challenges. ▸ Identify major historical influences on the development of cognitive psychology. ▸ Compare and contrast various ethical positions and their relationship with science and cognitive psychology. Page 17 of 165 ▸ Explain the approach of the textbook and the topics to be covered. ▸ Discuss the importance of critical thinking, ethical reasoning, and scientific understanding. GOALS AND OBJECTIVES Course Description This course investigates the principal theories and contemporary scientific findings in perception, memory, and thought, including sensations, attention, knowledge representation, problemsolving, reasoning, language, information processing, decisionmaking, creativity, and intelligence. The course will also focus on disorders of memory, perception, and cognition as well as strategies for improving these processes across development. The neural basis underlying these processes will be emphasized in the context of real-life situations. An important addition to this course is the inclusion of Christian and ethical integration; such inclusion provides a more comprehensive approach to help address contemporary ethical debates. Course Learning Objectives 1.Describe the history, assumptions, and major concepts of cognitive psychology. 2.Explain how people interact with and process information. 3.Evaluate the current research in cognitive psychology from a biblical worldview. 4.Apply cognitive psychology to improving real-world challenges. Page 18 of 165 5.Think through and develop conclusions about contemporary ethical issues in cognitive psychology. APPROACH OF THE TEXTBOOK Writing a textbook is a difficult and time-consuming task. However, it is our position that a good textbook is critical to a student’s development. In this textbook, we have attempted to provide multiple tools for understanding and comprehending the material. First, throughout the book, bolded terms will be defined both in the context of the passage and independently of the passage to help build content knowledge. Additionally, tables summarizing concepts have been created as a guide for study and quick referencing. Each chapter will also contain five types of breakout boxes, which are described in more detail below. Each is intended to enhance core content in cognitive psychology as well as learn how to apply it to your personal and professional life. The book and its contents were developed around the current best practices in learning acquisition. Biblical Integration All biblical/Christian integration will be boxed and highlighted in yellow/ gold. These boxes will consist of relevant Bible verses and Christian commentary on relevant cognitive psychology topics. These sections are intended to help you connect your faith to important content in the broader field of cognitive psychology. For the most part, these sections include Scripture in its context, so it is less about a verse being added here or there and more about building a deeper, transformative understanding of biblical thinking. Cognitive psychology is about thinking, and much of the Bible references aligning our mind, heart, and spirit to our internal dialogs and metacognition. (That is our thinking about our own thinking.) These sections are intentional in terms of bringing the Word of God to the forefront of our thinking. In fact, even in this section, let’s take a moment and consider a prayer for spiritual strength. Ephesians 3:14-21 Page 19 of 165 14 For this reason I bow my knees before the Father, 15 from whom every family in heaven and on earth is named, 16 that according to the riches of his glory he may grant you to be strengthened with power through his Spirit in your inner being, 17 so that Christ may dwell in your hearts through faith—that you, being rooted and grounded in love, 18 may have strength to comprehend with all the saints what is the breadth and length and height and depth, 19 and to know the love of Christ that surpasses knowledge, that you may be filled with all the fullness of God. 20 Now to him who is able to do far more abundantly than all that we ask or think, according to the power at work within us, 21 to him be glory in the church and in Christ Jesus throughout all generations, forever and ever. Amen. Expert Commentaries All expert commentaries will be boxed and highlighted in blue. These sections highlight an expert’s thinking about a particular topic in cognitive psychology (e.g., AI, decision-making, loneliness, cognitive behavioral therapy, etc.). The goal of these commentaries is to help you understand how various experts consider topics in cognitive psychology and apply that content to their personal and professional lives. Personal Application Understanding cognitive psychology has the potential to truly be transformative for your personal and professional life. Throughout the book, practices, procedures, and processes for improving your cognitive skills will be highlighted in orange. These boxes will consist of questions and activities aimed at improving your understanding and application of the materials—these are intended to help you understand yourself in the context of this material. Page 20 of 165 Doing these brief active learning applications will help you learn the material more efficiently and effectively. Annotated Bibliographies This textbook will have numerous references, but too often, the importance or application of those referenced studies is not brought to life for readers. To enhance understanding of the science of cognitive psychology, select articles will be reviewed in more detail with emphasis on personal application. All article annotated bibliographies will be boxed and highlighted in light green. These boxes will consist of a paragraph summarizing the details of a journal article or study in cognitive psychology and the implications gained from the article. Brief Summary and Self-Assessment One proven strategy for knowledge and skill acquisition is real-time assessment of knowledge and skill gains. The more frequently you can assess your own learning, the more effectively you can adapt and ensure you are learning the material. These sections allow for rapid self-assessment of material. Be sure that you can not only select the correct answer but also explain why it is the correct answer. Furthermore, explain why each of the other options is not correct. WHY IS THIS IMPORTANT? What is the most valuable commodity (i.e., product, good, or service) in the world? While indispensable necessities like water, food, medicine, and gasoline might come to mind, I would argue that perhaps the most valuable commodity is information (and I would argue further still that one of the most important skills is how to use it). Industries demonstrating significant growth are often directly associated with the exchange of information (e.g., internet search engines, AI applications, email, social media, video sharing Page 21 of 165 websites, electronic medical records, online course management systems, etc.). Although it is possible to survive without these tools—humans did for many years—the question arises as to why one would choose to do so. Information, and whoever possesses it, becomes increasingly important in times of crisis. For example, who would visit you if you were sick? Where would you take your car if it broke down before an important trip? Who would you call first if you were arrested? While the answers to these questions are obvious—a physician if you were sick, a mechanic if your car was not working, and an attorney if you had legal troubles—we do not often pay attention to the reasoning behind these answers. We seek out highly trained people for specific problems because they possess the right information and experience. There is often a direct relationship between the cost of professional services and the relative scarcity of the information and skills. For this reason, taking your car to the neighborhood mechanic is generally cheaper than taking it to a transmission specialist. Similarly, seeing a family physician is less expensive than seeing a neurologist. Naturally, this arrangement is advantageous if one possesses the sought-after information and skills. For example, everyone seems to have funny and derogatory lawyer jokes, but such jokes cease to be funny when you need a lawyer; and you are certainly going to hire the very best one you can afford under the circumstances. There is a widely recognized axiom that states, “Knowledge is power.” While a person may still question the ultimate utility of information, contemplate the value one would assign to knowledge about the future. Imagine the significance of possessing future information when it comes to stock investments or predicting the success of future relationships, such as marriage. Page 22 of 165 Society generally rewards those who have information and use their skills and/or experience. Often this is the foundation for a better quality of life, or at least a better income. Ultimately, as a student, is this not one of the reasons why you are taking this class? Similarly, are you not taking this class to satisfy a requirement while trying to earn a college degree? While I am sure there are students who take classes for the sheer joy of learning, most are considerably more practical. College graduates usually earn more money during their lifetime when compared to those individuals who do not attend college. Still, there is some confusion around this issue. People think having a degree is what provides the increased earning; when in reality, it is the information and skills that do. (So, if your goal is to just get a degree or “piece of paper,” then it is unlikely such an experience will be as transformative or result in high wages.) We tend to equate information with earning potential; hence, the more information you have, the greater the earning potential. This helps to explain why we have associates, bachelor’s, master’s, doctoral degrees, and post-doctoral fellowships as well as a variety of certificates, licenses, and continuing education options. It sounds so easy. “If I just earn a degree, then I will make more money.” However, earning that degree, or learning for that matter, is not so easy. It often takes a lot of dedication, effort, and creativity to make the most of a formal education. Learning and education, unfortunately, can be very different things. It takes even more work to translate education and learning into a successful career and life—to be clear, success is not defined as income, status, or power. The transformative potential of education and learning raises the question: “What is the most challenging aspect of a college course?” Is it the difficulty of acquiring countless concepts? Or is it simply memorizing the innumerable terms inherent to advanced areas of study? One of my greatest frustrations as a college student was Page 23 of 165 translating my efforts in studying into effective learning. I would study for many hours each day, for several days straight, and yet, while I was pleased with myself for being disciplined, I could not even remember the three most important points of what I was studying—or even recall the title of the chapter I was reading. I wondered, “Was all that effort for nothing?” Sadly, most of my study efforts did not immediately increase my clarity or lead to an epiphany. Instead, I simply realized I had a great deal more work to do. Learning is often hard, slow, and uncomfortable; while there are not any “shortcuts,” there are proven strategies to facilitate more effective and efficient learning. A significant portion of this textbook will be focused on helping you learn how to learn, which will have lifelong value. As a scientist, psychologist, professor, and public health expert, I know that engagement stands out as one of the most crucial aspects of learning. As student engagement increases, so does the capacity for learning. Furthermore, engagement is often directly connected with the practical value of the learning experience. Students tend to achieve higher grades in courses they perceive to provide practical knowledge and tangible skills. While this textbook offers comprehensive information and rich content on cognitive psychology, it also incorporates relevant examples, strategies to enhance personal learning and functioning, discussions on various topics, and considerations of ethical and biblical integration. The hope is that this content is engaging and transformative for your mind and soul. Annotated Bibliography* Do Students Accurately Perceive the Quality of Their Learning in Different Lecture Environments? A Review of Deslauriers et al. (2019). Page 24 of 165 Article Review by Liliana Grace Mejia Surprisingly, the amount we feel we learn in a course may not accurately represent how well we truly comprehend the course material as we learn it. Deslauriers and colleagues at Harvard University (2019) found students often misinterpret the feeling of initial difficulty when approaching new information as an indication of poor learning. This misconception could lead to a decreased motivation to engage in learning activities, especially those proven to be the most effective (i.e., active learning strategies). In this study, the researchers split college physics students into two groups using randomization and identical class resources. Their goal was to compare a passive with an active learning environment to determine if students ’self-perceived learning was accurate. The results demonstrated that students who are actively engaged in the classroom (rather than taking a passive role during a lecture) learned more, despite feeling they learned less. Active engagement included answering questions, working on problems in small groups, and speaking directly with the professor. The instructor provided targeted feedback to students after each group activity, which resulted in about half the class time consisting of relevant mini lectures. Conversely, passive engagement was considered sitting in a PowerPoint-led lecture without any overt student participation (i.e., conventional activities, like taking notes, is considered a passive activity). In the passive control group, the professor walked through the physics examples, and students observed while following along on their worksheets. The primary difference between the two groups was that the students were either told how to solve the problem (passive lecture) or offered the chance to try to find the answer themselves in small groups before the answers were explained (active lecture). Page 25 of 165 Deslauriers et al. (2019) elevated the methodological strength of the study by incorporating several control measures. The experiment was structured cohesively, with both the control and experimental groups using the same PowerPoint and materials. In contrast to previous research evaluating the efficacy of active learning, both groups within this study had proficient professors teaching the same information with different teaching styles (passive and active). The researchers also designed this study with a crossover design, allowing for each group of participants to experience both the active and passive lectures in various orders. This controlled for the potential confounding of active learning and varying class content. Students who asserted favoring one method of instruction over the other may have been reacting to variations in the course materials or topic, professor preference, or variations in the level of active participation. Thus, the rigorous methodology and precise controls used in this study allowed for high statistical power and consequently, reliable results. By assessing the discrepancies between the students ’feeling of learning (FOL) and objective tests of learning (TOL), this study revealed that all students felt they learned more in the passive lecture, despite the opposite being true. Within the active learning group, there were significantly higher TOL scores (P < 0.001), demonstrating a substantial increase in learning when active engagement was included in the classroom. (The researchers took an extra step to confirm that these significant results were unconfounded by student differences and varying levels of their background understanding in physics. Thus, three measures were provided to evaluate this concern, followed by descriptive statistics, which confirmed successful student randomization.) Other research also supports these findings, emphasizing that amateurs in a subject are poor judges of their own learning (Freeman et al., 2014; Nguyen et al., 2017). To compensate for their Page 26 of 165 inaccurate perception, many novice students use the “flow” of the lesson or the instructor’s fluency or expertise to estimate how much they learned. Students resist taking more responsibility for their own learning, as we all tend to favor the path of least effort. However, though cognitive effort is uncomfortable, research suggests it is vital for true learning to occur. So, what can you do with this information? When you are tempted to become distracted and disengaged, remember that your learning success depends on the cognitive effort you are willing to exert. The empirical evidence provided in this study supports the notion that students who are actively engaged during class learn more than those who are passive. Simply acknowledging that active learning leads to deeper understanding can greatly benefit your class experience (despite feeling as though the opposite is true) (Deslauriers et al., 2019). Your perception of how much you are learning is not a strong indicator of the depth of your understanding. References Deslauriers, L., McCarty, L. S., Miller, K., Callaghan, K., & Kestin, G. (2019). Measuring actual learning versus feeling of learning in response to being actively engaged in the classroom. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(39), 19251-19257. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1821936116 Freeman, S., Eddy, S. L., McDonough, M., Smith, M. K., Okoroafor, N., Jordt, H., & Wenderoth, M. P. (2014). Active learning increases student performance in science, engineering, and mathematics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(23), 8410-8415. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1319030111 Nguyen, K. A., Husman, J., Borrego, M., Shekhar, P., Prince, M. J., DeMonbrun, M., Finelli, C. J., Henderson, C. R., & Waters, C. (2017). Students ’expectations, types of instruction, and instructor Page 27 of 165 strategies predicting student response to active learning. International Journal of Engineering Education, 33, 2-18. *Contributed by Liliana Grace Mejia. © Kendall Hunt Publishing Company. THE BIG QUESTIONS [Do you ever stare toward the heavens and wonder if science can ultimately answer all our questions? Can science continue to transform mysteries into facts? Think about how much progress we have already made. Is it not rather amazing that we have detailed maps of distant planets and even know the content of their atmospheric gases? Robots are currently driving across Mars, taking pictures, and sending them to Earth. Farther away, distant galaxies have been identified, some ten thousand and counting, and the number of stars currently visible exceeds seventy sextillion (7 followed by 22 zeros). Scientists even determined the relative size of the universe and have it measured at approximately 93 billion light-years in diameter. Even now, plans are underway to develop interplanetary travel, a feat that is in the process of occurring during your lifetime. In many ways, what we used to call “science fiction” just a few years back is now a historical note or scientific fact. While some scientists gaze across the universe, others have recently delved deep into the human body, unlocking one of its greatest mysteries: the human genome. For the first time, the complete genetic blueprint for building a human being is available. The human genome is made of 46 chromosomes, between twenty thousand and twenty-five thousand genes, and over three billion DNA base pairs. In fact, it took a multinational effort over 13 years to sequence it. Knowledge of the very building blocks that provide structure, organization, and function to our bodies offers enormous potential for not just treating but curing diseases. Understanding DNA allowed scientists to clone a sheep known as Page 28 of 165 “Dolly.” Despite enormous controversy, people have continued to consider human cloning. What may be even more controversial is genetic engineering. If science has the capability to genetically engineer crops, then why not people? Honestly, who would not like to be just a little more intelligent, athletic, and/or attractive? From the universe that carries us to the molecules that bind us, advances within every scientific discipline continue to accelerate at astonishing, unprecedented rates. © vchal/Shutterstock.com The moral and ethical dilemmas extending from these advancements develop just as quickly. Yet, there is often a lack of attention to serious ethical discussions on new technologies. This may partly be because there are great financial incentives for technology innovation, while there is comparatively little for establishing a sophisticated ethical understanding of the new technologies that result. The potential benefits of an invention are often just too attractive for us to wait until we grasp its implications before blindly using it. But scientific and technological advances give us tools that can be used for evil or good—and the more powerful they are, the more powerfully evil or good they can be. Just because it can be done, does not mean it should be done. One of the most impactful moral effects of scientific and technological advancement is on our sense of self. As each new mystery is solved, do you ever feel increasingly indistinct, irrelevant, and uncertain? Many people do. While science does not set out to diminish our significance, it often seems to do just that. It appears the natural progression is that, as our scientific knowledge increases, our inherent self-worth decreases. Does the objective search for facts interfere with our subjective sense Page 29 of 165 of meaning? This might be inimitably true in cognitive psychology. I, as well as many others, would argue that the greatest scientific mystery, across all of time and space, is yet to be solved. That mystery resides in you. So, while there may be times when you feel unimportant, especially in comparison to the near-infinite expanse of the universe, one of the most significant questions of all time deals with the nature of your very own perceptions and conscious experiences. Therefore, this makes you and your very thoughts and perceptions exceedingly important, especially as an area of scientific study. Thus, while attempting to unravel the details of the universe, incredibly important advances like the human genome will eventually pale in comparison to comprehending the final frontier of consciousness. Understanding consciousness is foundational to cognitive psychology. Despite our prospects for dramatic scientific advancement, it is important to keep our limitations in perspective. Some parts of the human experience have always seemed accessible to observation and study. Human behavior, for all its complexity, seems easy to observe as long as we devote enough attention and care. Neurological activity, once seemingly as mysterious and inaccessible as the farthest reaches of the universe, now appears to be observable, even if we lack the tools to completely observe it at this time. But some parts of human experience, like first-person experience, remain unexplained and outside of scientific investigation. According to philosophy, the “hard problem of consciousness” has puzzled the most scientifically minded thinkers. We can know all there is to know about neurological activity, and yet there seems to be something more about consciousness that we still do not know. For example, we know Page 30 of 165 what it is like to have human consciousness, but can we know about bat consciousness? Even if we know everything about a bat’s neurological activity from the outside, is there not something additional the bat knows about having its mind that we could never know (Nagel, 1974)? So far, at least, science cannot give us an answer regarding this kind of question, and some wonder if it ever can. Yet, even if such a thing were true, the study of the brain is a step toward understanding the mind, our consciousness. The very nature of consciousness—the mind—is an area of study encompassing all others, which is why a cognitive psychology course is required in nearly every psychology program in the country (and the world). Since psychology and neuroscience deal with how we perceive and understand the universe, I would argue these fields of study combine all the other disciplines. Scientific advancements and all human achievements are housed in the mind. Where else could they be located? So, the study of the mind or thinking is one of the single most important areas of study and often one with the most directly transferable benefits.]3 Cognitive psychology is the study of stimulus-brain-behavior relationships and what happens in between processes. Advancements in this field have furthered our understanding of how we perceive, remember, and respond to the world around us and the changes within us. Considering how far our understanding of cognitive psychology has developed in the last 50 years, it raises the following question: How far will it take us, and do we really want to take that journey? There are important moral and ethical issues to such advances because who we are is essentially what we think. So, changing human thinking changes who we are by default—this is not a trivial issue. More specifically, imagine for a moment how far Page 31 of 165 science will advance in its understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying our every perception, memory, response, and, eventually, our most sacred beliefs like our understanding of “self.” Cognitive Psychology: The scientific study of information processing in the mind. Imagine that our understanding of the brain advances so much that we can program the brain as readily as we can program a computer—adding and deleting content at will. While this idea might seem ridiculous, consider how the idea of a mobile phone would have seemed 300 years ago. Scientists have already implanted electrodes in a pigeon’s brain to control its flight. The implants activate specific areas of the pigeon’s brain using electrical signals. These signals are sent by the scientists via computer, mimicking the natural signals generated by the brain. Even now, scientists are making enormous strides in understanding and replacing natural organs with artificial ones. For example, those who have lost their hearing can be fitted with an artificial cochlea. Those who have lost their sight can have an artificial retina implanted in their eye. People who have lost an arm or a leg can have a new, bionic one fitted to their body. Neural Link, a human computer brain implant, has been approved by the Food and Drug Administration for human trials. While these devices are relatively crude now, soon they may even surpass our body’s natural abilities. It is one thing to fix something that is not working but something else altogether to create or build a life. While controversial, scientists have for the first time created human embryo-like structures with just stem cells and let the embryo live up to 14 days (Oldak et al.; 2023 Weatherbee et al., 2023). Page 32 of 165 Personal Application Develop a short description of what you think humankind will be living like in 300 years. How will humans be using technology at that point in time? How will our daily living change? Do you see these advances ultimately improving our quality of life or worsening it? Explain your answer. Are you more concerned or excited about future technological advances? While much of our knowledge of stimulus-brain-behavior relationships can be used to improve the human condition (e.g., the point of this textbook), this knowledge can also detract from it. Advances in cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and biochemistry have enabled humankind to further develop and refine methods used to alter consciousness. Some have been for good, like treatments for children with autism, but also some for bad, like increasing the potency of addictive drugs. Scientists too often focus on what can be done and too infrequently consider the intended and unintended consequences of such advances. Scientists and clinicians should be given more training on formal ethics and given more opportunity to reflect upon their work—emphasizing this is a major point of this textbook. But how can a person think ethically if they have not been taught how to think? This is another major point of this textbook. To proactively move you toward more effective and efficient learning and thinking—which is a continuous and never-ending effort—this book will cover a variety of tangible strategies intended to improve content knowledge, critical thinking, and creative applications. The hope is that this book will help you construct a personalized, continuous learning method that will be useful beyond this course and its contents. Page 33 of 165 Biblical Integration Corinthians 1:18-31 18 For the word of the cross is folly to those who are perishing, but to us who are being saved, it is the power of God. 19 For it is written, “I will destroy the wisdom of the wise, and the discernment of the discerning I will thwart.” 20 Where is the one who is wise? Where is the scribe? Where is the debater of this age? Has not God made foolish the wisdom of the world? 21 For since, in the wisdom of God, the world did not know God through wisdom, it pleased God through the folly of what we preach to save those who believe. 22 For Jews demand signs and Greeks seek wisdom, 23 but we preach Christ crucified, a stumbling block to Jews and folly to Gentiles, 24 but to those who are called, both Jews and Greeks, Christ the power of God and the wisdom of God. 25 For the foolishness of God is wiser than men, and the weakness of God is stronger than men. 26 For consider your calling, brothers: not many of you were wise according to worldly standards, not many were powerful, not many were of noble birth. 27 But God chose what is foolish in the world to shame the wise; God chose what is weak in the world to shame the strong; 28 God chose what is low and despised in the world, even things that are not, to bring to nothing things that are, 29 so that no human being might boast in the presence of God. 30 And because of him you are in Christ Jesus, who became to us wisdom from God, righteousness and sanctification and redemption, 31 so that, as it is written, “Let the one who boasts, boast in the Lord.” Have you ever been in a professor’s, lawyer’s, or doctor’s office? I have many times over as well as in many different states and several countries. There is a generally established pattern to their office Page 34 of 165 décor—their diplomas, licenses, certificates, accolades, and honorable distinctions are ornately framed and displayed prominently, often on the wall behind their desk. This is a highly encouraged practice, and the argument goes it helps instill confidence when the student, client, or patient notices all the accomplishments. These professionals, as just examples, put significant dedication, education, and cost into their professional development and often have their identity joined to their occupation. In fact, in the United States, it is a matter of practice when you meet someone new to ask, “Who are you?” or “What do you do?” or “Tell me about yourself.” Regardless of how it is phrased, the question is almost always about what you do for a living and informing about your relative societal status. This is vanity. All the wisdom of the world will get you no closer to heaven and, in fact, can interfere with salvation. This Bible passage specifically notes that to the world, the gospel will seem strange and maybe even silly—especially, to the wise of the world. But, to those willing to humble themselves, they will recognize what is truly important. God is the source of life. God’s foolishness is wiser than the culmination of all human wisdom; God’s weakness is stronger than the totality of all human strength. This is why we need wisdom beyond what we can access on our own; this is why we need the Bible and the Holy Spirit. Judging knowledge or success from a worldly perspective will ultimately result in dissatisfaction. God will do what He promises to do, and if you let Him, He will bring you into this promise. What does such “folly” look like? In Charleston, SC on June 17, 2015, a deranged 21-year-old went into a historically Black church and shot and killed nine people during a Wednesday Bible Study. This is such a disgustingly grotesque act. There are really no words to fully describe it. Page 35 of 165 The Chief Magistrate stated, “Charleston is a very strong community. We have big hearts. We’re a very loving community. We’re going to reach out to everyone, all victims, and we will touch them” (Nahorniak, 2015). He went on further to note that there were victims on both sides of this tragedy, referring specifically to the young man’s family. “Nobody would have ever thrown them into the whirlwind of events that they have been thrown into. We must find it in our heart, at some point in time, not only to help those that are victims but to also help his family as well.” The families of the victims responded to these comments with unwavering clarity. The daughter of Ethel Lance, one of the nine church members murdered, stated, “I forgive you. You took something really precious away from me. I will never talk to her ever again. I will never be able to hold her again. But I forgive you and have mercy on your soul. It hurts me, it hurts a lot of people but God forgives you and I forgive you.” Anthony Thompson, speaking for the family of Myra Thompson, followed with this: “I forgive you, and my family forgives you. We would like you to take this opportunity to repent. Repent. Confess. Give your life to the one who matters the most, Christ, so he can change your ways no matter what happens to you, and you will be OK. Do that and you will be better off than you are right now.” Yet another speaker came forward, Felecia Sanders, and said, “We welcomed you Wednesday night in our Bible study with open arms. You have killed some of the most beautifullest people that I know. Every fiber in my body hurts, and I will never be the same... Tywanza was my hero. But as they say in the Bible study, we enjoyed you, but may God have mercy on your soul.” The news reporters and the rest of the world, for that matter, could hardly understand this reaction. This is beyond most people’s comprehension and compassion—to respond to such blind hate with such overwhelming faith, hope, and love. No amount of education Page 36 of 165 could prepare anyone for such a reaction, only Christ. This is what it means when the Bible says, “For the word of the cross is folly to those who are perishing, but to us who are being saved it is the power of God.” James 3:17 17 But the wisdom from above is first pure, then peaceable, gentle, open to reason, full of mercy and good fruits, impartial and sincere. Reference Nahorniak, M. (2015, June 19). Families to roof: May God ‘Have mercy on your soul’. USA Today. https://www.usatoday.com/story/news/2015/06/19/bond-courtdylann-roof-charleston/28991607/ PSYCHOLOGY DEFINED [From your general or introductory psychology course, you should have learned that psychology is defined as the scientific study of measurable mental activities and observable behaviors. Psychology initially distinguished itself from philosophy by applying the scientific method to understanding, predicting, and controlling behavior. This started rather rudimentarily by examining the relationship between sensations and perceptions but has now grown to include the study of motivation, development, emotion, cognition, learning, social relationships, and psychopathology, as well as how our biological, social, and psychological systems influence each other. Psychologists study every aspect of human experiences and often employ animal models to help explain biopsychosocial phenomena. Cognitive psychology is one part of psychology but in many ways should be seen as foundational to the field of psychology because it explains how we think and come to understand knowledge and convert our knowledge to skills. Page 37 of 165 Psychology: The scientific study of measurable mental processes and observable behaviors. Many people enter the study of psychology thinking it will mostly reveal or explain commonsense experiences or serve as foundational content for counseling or clinical psychology. These two expectations do not fully capture the field of psychology. Psychology is an increasingly complex area of study and is unique compared to other sciences because it integrates many subdisciplines and perspectives to help explain behavior and mental processes. For example, the study of psychology is simultaneously a biological, social, and cognitive-behavioral science. Biological psychologists are interested in how genes, the brain, the nervous system, and the endocrine system impact behavior. Because drugs influence one or more of these processes, they would be of interest to biological psychologists too. Social psychologists are interested in how interpersonal and group dynamics affect behavior as well as more specific topics like social media, prejudice, religion, bullying, and criminal activity. Cognitive-behavioral psychologists study important phenomena like learning, memory, decision-making, rewards, and behavioral modification. As you would imagine, none of these areas operate in isolation, so the study of human behavior involves addressing every psychological topic from a biological, social, and cognitive-behavioral (psychological) approach while simultaneously considering how people change and grow across a lifespan (Figure 1.1).]2 Source: Brian Kelley Figure 1.1 Page 38 of 165 Page 39 of 165 Areas of Science Within Psychology2 Biological psychologists: Psychologists interested in how genes, the brain, the nervous system, and the endocrine system impact behavior. Social psychologists: Psychologists interested in how interpersonal and group dynamics affect behavior, including specific topics like social media, prejudice, religion, bullying, and criminal activity. Cognitive-behavioral psychologists: Psychologists who study phenomena like learning, memory, decision-making, rewards, and behavioral modification. Personal Application Select a topic of interest to you in the field of psychology and list at least five factors for each of the major areas noted in the below diagram (Figure 1.1). Explain why you think these factors are important to the behavior of interest. How would these biopsychosocial factors change across the lifespan? Pick several developmental milestones and note the relation between the three areas of development. [While the science of psychology simultaneously considers biological, social, and psychological influences on behavior, psychology is also unique as a discipline because it is concurrently a basic science, translational science, clinical science, and prevention science. More specifically, even the sub-discipline of cognitive psychology addresses each of these levels of scientific inquiry. The goal of basic scientists is to describe, explain, predict, and control behavior with the aim of understanding phenomena; and this occurs without any clear clinical objective (e.g., What does this Page 40 of 165 neurotransmitter do?). Often, it is referred to as research for the sake of knowledge. Research in basic science helps explain the big “what” and “why” of a phenomenon. It is more focused on describing behavior and factors that cause things to occur. Translational science is an effort to build on basic scientific research to create new therapies, medical procedures, or diagnostics (e.g., How can we use our understanding of this neurotransmitter to positively impact autism?) The goal of translational scientists is to translate the basic facts about a phenomenon discovered by basic scientists into a treatment or procedure to help some human condition. Basic science: Science that aims to describe, explain, predict, and control behavior with the goal of understanding phenomena. Translational science: Science that builds upon the knowledge obtained in basic science to create new therapies, medical procedures, or diagnostics. Clinical science: Science that develops and improves specific treatments for conditions. Prevention science: Science focused on studying how to prevent and/or reduce negative medical, social, and emotional impacts of phenomena before they occur. Clinical scientists build upon this translational research to improve the human condition or a person’s quality of life, often through improving or creating specific treatments (e.g., What are the necessary parameters for changing neurotransmitter levels for symptom reduction in autism?). Page 41 of 165 Thus, clinical researchers are concerned with the big “how,” meaning how we can use the information to make treatments or interventions to help people. They are more concerned with solving practical problems. Prevention science is a framework for studies on how to prevent and/or reduce negative medical, social, and emotional impacts before they occur; it is often an extension of clinical work in the sense that scientists work to prevent the occurrence or reoccurrence of a particular problem like depression or substance abuse (e.g., How can our understanding of neurotransmitters and diet impact the reduction in children developing autism?). However, prevention science could also be focused on community issues such as human trafficking, gang activity, or teen pregnancy. Almost always, advances in basic science lay the groundwork for translational science that works to advance clinical science.]2 GOALS OF THE FIELD OF PSYCHOLOGY [The field of psychology is complex and includes the study of many different topic areas with the overarching goal of improving the human condition. As such, Page 42 of 165 there are two main goals of the field of psychology: 1. Conduct research in psychological phenomena for the purpose of understanding, explaining, predicting, and controlling human behavior, cognition, and experience. 2. Use the knowledge gained from psychological research to promote human well-being (through counseling, interventions, social programs, etc.). These two goals of the field of psychology can be seen in the goal statements of the leading professional organizations in the field. According to the Association for Psychological Science (APS), psychological science “has the ability to transform society for the better and must play a central role in advancing human welfare and the public interest” (Association for Psychological Science [APS], 2022, p. 1). Through promoting psychological science, the APS seeks to further our understanding of psychological principles for the purpose of improving the welfare of individuals. Similarly, the American Psychological Association (APA) states that they promote research in psychology for the purpose of making a positive impact on social and individual issues (American Psychological Association [APA], 2020). As reflected by both national organizations, the field of psychology aims to understand psychological phenomena for the purpose of improving the welfare of all people. As a student in psychology, you will learn about the major theories and principles that relate to a variety of human experiences as you grow in your understanding of how to use psychological knowledge to help others. Page 43 of 165 Consistent with the two overarching goals above, the APA (2020) has asserted five goals for you, a student in a psychology course. As you go through the material in each chapter, keep these goals in mind: Goal 1: Knowledge Base in Psychology You will demonstrate fundamental knowledge and comprehension of the major concepts, theoretical perspectives, historical trends, and empirical findings to discuss how psychological principles apply to behavioral problems. Goal 2: Scientific Inquiry and Critical Thinking You will demonstrate scientific reasoning and problem-solving, including effective research methods. Goal 3: Ethical and Social Responsibility in a Diverse World You will apply ethical standards to evaluate psychological science and practice, and you will develop ethically and socially responsible behaviors for professional and personal settings in a landscape that involves increasing diversity. Goal 4: Communication You will demonstrate competence in writing and oral and interpersonal communication skills. Goal 5: Professional Development You will apply psychological content and skills to career goals and develop a meaningful, professional direction for life after graduation. If you are interested in learning more about the mission and work of different organizations in psychology, please visit the following websites: •American Psychological Association (APA): www.apa.org Page 44 of 165 •Association for Psychological Science (APS): www.psychologicalscience.org •American Association of Christian Counselors (AACC): https://www.aacc.net •Christian Association for Psychological Studies (CAPS): https://new.caps.net]2 Personal Application Explain how your program of study is addressing or not addressing these five goals. Are there any important goals that are missing? If so, what would you add to this list? Go to the American Psychological Association’s website and review their Topics page which includes numerous topic areas in the field. Explain which interests you the most and why. How do the APA and APS differ in their overall mission and goals? Compare one of those organizations with one of the Christian organizations. How are they similar versus different in their goals and approach? Brief Summary and Self-Assessment Information and skills are crucial during crises; they enable individuals to make informed decisions and seek appropriate expertise. The value of information and skills is reflected in the cost of professional services and earning potential associated with different degrees and certifications. Possessing knowledge provides advantages in various aspects of life, including investments and relationships. Learning is challenging but can be facilitated through effective strategies that enhance engagement. This textbook on cognitive psychology aims to engage students by providing comprehensive information, practical examples, learning strategies, and Page 45 of 165 considerations of ethics and biblical integration. While scientific advancements continue, mysteries like the human genome and consciousness pose moral and ethical dilemmas. Cognitive psychology focuses on stimulus-brain-behavior relationships, perception, memory, and responses to the world, with the potential for further advancements. This textbook emphasizes strategies for learning and thinking, improving content knowledge, critical thinking, and creativity, extending beyond the course itself. Psychology integrates various subdisciplines and perspectives to understand behavior and mental processes, contributing to both basic and translational science. Cognitive psychology plays a fundamental role in understanding human thinking and knowledge acquisition. Ultimately, possessing information and skills enhances outcomes, and psychology, particularly cognitive psychology, offers valuable insights for personal and societal well-being. 1.What does the author argue is the most valuable commodity in the world? a.Water b.Food c.Medicine d.Information (Correct Answer) 2.Which industries are directly associated with the exchange of information? a.Agriculture and farming b.Manufacturing and production Page 46 of 165 c.Internet search engines, AI applications, email, social media, video sharing websites, electronic medical records, and online course management systems d.Transportation and logistics Answer: c) Internet search engines, AI applications, email, social media, video sharing websites, electronic medical records, and online course management systems 3.Why do we seek out highly trained people for specific problems? a.Because they are more expensive b.Because they have experience c.Because they possess the right information and skills d.Because they are more trustworthy Answer: c) Because they possess the right information and skills 4.What does cognitive psychology study? a.Learning, memory, and decision-making b.Social dynamics and interpersonal relationships c.Genetic engineering and biotechnology d.Stimulus-brain-behavior relationships and what happens in between Answer: d) Stimulus-brain-behavior relationships and what happens in between 5.What does the study of psychology integrate to explain behavior and mental processes? a.Biological, social, and cognitive perspectives b.Physical, chemical, and mathematical principles c.Economic, political, and historical factors d.Art, literature, and cultural practices Answer: a) Biological, social, and cognitive perspectives WHY ETHICAL INTEGRATION? Page 47 of 165 Among the sciences, psychology is relatively young, and like most sciences, it grew out of philosophical inquiry and matured into an independent discipline. Yet, it is important to remember the “why” of psychology that sparked its independence. The study of the human brain is crucial for our understanding of the meaning of human life and the nature of human flourishing, and these are questions to which full investigation still belongs to a significant degree to philosophy and theology. If we are to progress in our understanding of the human condition, we need to uncover not only the limits of human nature through the study of psychology but also the character of a good human life through the study of ethics. On a more practical level, there are many reasons why it is crucial that ethics and psychology are conversant with each other. Below are just a few of these. 1. Psychology deals with subject matter that has a uniquely powerful impact on persons (e.g., the mind, behavior, habits, inner compulsion, etc.). Since humans are the primary subjects of psychology, it is imperative that psychologists exercise caution in how they conduct their research and practice. Every human deserves to be treated with dignity, and that poses a unique constraint on what psychologists are permitted to do in their pursuit of truth. 2. Practitioners in any health science have a special relationship with vulnerable patients. Page 48 of 165 The health sciences are especially subjected to ethical constraints because of the vulnerability their practitioners encounter daily. Those who suffer from medical ailments are in need. Likewise, psychologists, along with any others in the health sciences, have a special mandate to not only attend to the needs of those who may feel neglected or shunned but also to maintain a strong commitment to seeing their dignity and treating them according to it. 3. Psychology has a heightened burden to avoid repeating the ethical abuses of the past. Psychologists can be fairly honest about their discipline’s ethical abuses from the past, but it is also easy to forget the possibility that if we are not careful, some current practices may one day be looked at with disdain and incredulity. The only way to avoid falling into similar patterns of unethical practice is to use heightened vigilance about ethics. 4. Public perceptions of psychology impact the effectiveness of professional interactions with the community and its members. Practitioners and scientists in psychology represent their field whether they like it or not. For that reason, at least some in the general public will remember the field only by the small interactions they have with the individuals who represent it. 5. Close historical and conceptual ties exist between psychology and moral philosophy (ethics). Philosophy begins with wonder, and humanity offers one of the richest sources of wonder that we may investigate. Even as psychology was in its infancy, philosophers around the world were discussing the meaning of our lives, the extent of our freedom to Page 49 of 165 choose, the weight of our responsibility, the limits of our capabilities, the rightness of our actions, the effectiveness of our social structures, etc. Psychology has taken some of the burdens once carried by philosophy alone, but the two fields remain mutually dependent in their pursuit of truth. 6. Christians have special ethical mandates. Jesus pushed ethical matters beyond the superficial conduct that was the focus of many of his contemporaries. For example, where the people were taught not to commit adultery or murder, Jesus taught them not to lust or hate. However we interpret the ethical burdens Jesus placed on us, he highlighted the importance and difficulty of ethics. Further, he made it clear that Christians must not only strive for what is right but must also consistently remember their inability to meet the standards requires on their own. “No one is righteous. No, not one” (English Standard Bible, Romans 3:10). Of course, ethics and psychology ought to interact for many reasons. Beyond those given above is perhaps the most important reason why ethics is important for psychology: The study of ethics is instrumental for the full life of any moral agent. Academic disciplines may produce discrete and insular conclusions about very different aspects of the universe, but their service to human life should not be understated. The ability to prioritize those conclusions for an ordered human life is one of the building blocks of a meaningful existence. Personal Application Explain why you think it is important to consider ethics and morals within the framework of studying a science. Explain further why you think this is even more important when studying cognitive psychology. Provide three descriptions of why you think cognitive Page 50 of 165 psychology and ethics relate. What do you think, right now, is the most important ethical issue in cognitive psychology? Biblical Integration What does it mean to act ethically or even morally? At the most foundational level, it would seem acting or behaving beyond yourself would be one of the most important features of ethics and morals. Most of the time, this would be hard to determine as we do not have direct access to people’s thoughts or feelings. We only see outward behavior and must make judgments about what a person is thinking based on these behaviors. Is a person appearing kind so they can impress others vs. behaving kindly because that is who that person is? We cannot tell. It sure does seem that, overwhelmingly, people’s first inclination is toward ugliness, not kindness. If this were not the case, why are children so mean to other children different than themselves, especially those children with disabilities? It is rare to see a child stand up against bullies or unkindness when they are not the direct target of it. Consider now that many years ago, children born with disabilities were thought to have been born as a punishment for their parents ’sins. Imagine how these children were treated, especially when the world was so much more difficult to navigate and survive. Mark 2:1-12 Jesus Heals a Paralytic 1 And when he returned to Capernaum after some days, it was reported that he was at home. 2 And many were gathered together, so that there was no more room, not even at the door. And he was preaching the word to them. 3 And they came, bringing to him a paralytic carried by four men. 4 And when they could not get near him because of the crowd, they removed the roof above him, and when they had made an opening, they let down the bed on which the paralytic lay. 5 And when Jesus saw their faith, he said to the Page 51 of 165 paralytic, “Son, your sins are forgiven.” 6 Now some of the scribes were sitting there, questioning in their hearts, 7“Why does this man speak like that? He is blaspheming! Who can forgive sins but God alone?” 8 And immediately Jesus, perceiving in his spirit that they thus questioned within themselves, said to them, “Why do you question these things in your hearts? 9 Which is easier, to say to the paralytic, ‘Your sins are forgiven, ’or to say, ‘Rise, take up your bed and walk’? 10 But that you may know that the Son of Man has authority on earth to forgive sins”—he said to the paralytic— 11 “I say to you, rise, pick up your bed, and go home.” 12 And he rose and immediately picked up his bed and went out before them all so that they were all amazed and glorified God, saying, “We never saw anything like this!” The paralyzed man, obviously unable to approach Jesus on his own, had to rely on four other men to bring him to Christ. Not only did those men bring the paralyzed man to Christ, but they also had to really put some work into the process, including climbing on a roof, breaking up the roof, getting rope, and lowering the paralyzed man to Jesus. What are the five miracles in this story? 1. The man had abundant faith. 2. Jesus forgave the man’s sins. 3. Jesus understood the thoughts of the paralyzed man and scribes. 4. Jesus healed the man. 5. A man in that period of time, paralyzed from birth, had four caring friends. Mother Teresa said, “The most terrible poverty is loneliness and the feeling of being unloved.” Psalm 68:3-6 3 But the righteous shall be glad; Page 52 of 165 they shall exult before God; they shall be jubilant with joy! 4 Sing to God, sing praises to his name; lift up a song to him who rides through the deserts; his name is the Lord; exult before him! 5 Father of the fatherless and protector of widows is God in his holy habitation. 6 God settles the solitary in a home; he leads out the prisoners to prosperity, but the rebellious dwell in a parched land. Let us all be the type of friends noted in the story and engage with those around us so that people can see the love of Christ. WHY CHRISTIAN INTEGRATION? [At this point, you may be wondering how the Bible fits into all of this. It seems from the definition of the field and the goals of the American Psychological Association presented so far that the field of psychology is a secular science, and researchers are focused on developing theories and treatments based on science and science alone. As for psychology, you would be correct to draw these conclusions about the current field as a whole. However, as Christian psychologists, through this textbook, we will be offering another perspective on the field of psychology and the study of psychological phenomena. In this text, you will learn about the field and science of psychology through a biblical worldview. You will learn what it means to integrate the Bible with psychological science to understand the truth about human phenomena and to get a more accurate understanding of human behavior. Consistent with the goals of the Christian Association for Psychological Studies (CAPS), Page 53 of 165 the purpose of this textbook is this: (1) to understand psychological topics from the integration of Christianity and the behavioral sciences at both theoretical and applied levels and (2) to promote research and educational opportunities advancing mental health are as means of ministry in and to the world (Christian Association for Psychological Studies [CAPS], 2020). As Christians in the field of psychology, it is our responsibility to understand the science and theories of the field while also understanding what God says about psychological concepts so we can most accurately discuss psychological topics and, therefore, fully help our neighbors. Throughout this textbook, you will learn about the theories and science of psychology while also investigating what the Bible says about different topics. You will be challenged to think about different areas of life and how both the field of cognitive psychology and the Bible talk about them. You will also be directed to search for truth on topics by integrating what God says in the Bible with what He reveals (and we study) in creation, especially across different contemporary ethical issues. As we begin, there are some important terms and foundations to set as we embark on an exploration of psychology from a biblical worldview. First and foremost, what is a biblical worldview? Biblical worldview: A worldview informed, constrained, and enhanced by the Bible as a guiding source. WHAT IS A WORLDVIEW? Page 54 of 165 [Simply stated, a worldview is the cognitive framework by which we organize our sensory information—hence, the preeminent emphasis in this chapter. While the sensory information our body detects is objective (e.g., quantitative and, as such, measurable), the perceptual processes making sense of all this disparate information are subjective (e.g., qualitative and, as such, distinctive). This is why two people can have nearly the same experience (i.e., sensory) and, yet, have very different interpretations (i.e., perception). Reacting to temperature is a simple example. Two people can be in the exact same room wearing nearly identical clothes, and one person complains it is too hot while the other complains it is too cold. Flavor is yet another example. What tastes good to one person compared to another is influenced by a variety of complex factors, most notably experience, context, and hunger. Two people eating the exact same food might come to opposite conclusions about the experience with one noting it is too spicy and the other saying it is not spicy enough. If you grew up in a home that ate spicy food frequently then you would be accustomed to it and find it enjoyable. The enjoyment is complex because it might be that eating such food occurred at a family dinner each night wherein the family had a chance to talk, laugh, and bond. So, food is not just food but part of a developmental and cultural experience. On the other hand, if you had limited exposure to such food, then it might literally be a painful experience with an upset stomach, burning mouth, and watering eyes and nose. In both cases though if you are hungry enough, it might change the overall food experience. So, how we experience the world is complicated. Page 55 of 165 Worldview: Frameworks of beliefs, values, expectations, etc., by which people operate to understand the world around them. Now, imagine trying to apply this process to developing a political position, a religious commitment, or a philosophical argument. How many factors would be involved in such a determination? So, in a more complex way, a worldview is how we make sense of and organize all our perceptual experiences. To this end, our worldview changes as we change and grows in complexity as we grow in complexity. Going back to a simple definition of a complex idea, a worldview can be described as the cumulative total of our beliefs about the world and our place in it—it is the lens through which we view and interpret the world. People do not have perfect lenses, nor do any two people have the same lens. Everyone has a worldview. While some may argue they do not have a worldview, such a claim is ingenuous, erroneous, and dangerous. It triggers the famous quote from Alexander Hamilton: “Those who stand for nothing fall for everything.” Our worldview serves several purposes, such as being a filter that allows us to screen and sift the world into things with which we agree or disagree. By extension, it helps us identify the differences between right and wrong or good and bad. To say you have no worldview effectively dampens the filter component of the worldview, and instead of discriminating and separating information, you just absorb it like a sponge. A sponge approach is dangerous because it just soaks up everything—the good and the bad. Perceptually, if we are sponges this allows all information to come in and build perceptions without needed understanding. Page 56 of 165 Related, it is the default position of our perceptual experience to build the world around ourselves, in our image, and with ourselves as the center of it. People who say they have no worldview typically just have a permissive one, where there is no critical way to separate truth from falsehood. Carrying this out consistently is impossible to sustain and irresponsible to attempt. Attempting to escape the difficulty of developing a mature and sophisticated worldview does not relieve you of the responsibility of interpreting the world well. It renders your interpretation ill-formed and simplistic. We all will have to actively work and struggle to build a cohesive and comprehensive worldview for which we are an integral part of the world but not the center of it. As Francis Schaeffer wrote, “Most people catch their presuppositions from their family and surrounding society the way a child catches measles. But people with more understanding realize that their presuppositions should be chosen after careful consideration of what worldview is true.” More positively, Frances Moore Lappe stated, “Each of us carries within us a worldview, a set of assumptions about how the world works—what some call a paradigm—that forms the very questions we allow ourselves to ask and determines our view of future possibilities.” One of the important factors in education, generally, and cognitive psychology, specifically, is to understand yourself and even more so to understand how you come to understand yourself. A major goal of this textbook is to provide the necessary content knowledge, critical analysis, and creative application of information for you to better understand yourself and, secondarily, understand cognitive psychology. It was eloquently stated by Alister McGrath: If worldviews or metanarratives can be compared to lenses, which of them brings things into the sharpest focus? This is not Page 57 of 165 an irrational retreat from reason. Rather, it is about grasping a deeper order of things which is more easily accessed by the imagination than by reason. Reflecting upon your worldview is the first and most important step in cognitive psychology because, if the above information is correct, it will serve as the lens or filter versus a sponge in terms of how the rest of the content is recounted, synthesized, and applied. Worldviews play a critical role in the simplest issues like defining what happiness might look like. People alive today are almost constantly bombarded with messages of happiness—mostly on how to access it. Most modern advertising is centered around this central premise. Does our worldview simply define happiness for us or is it more than that, and does ethics simply mean to consider others as you seek out your own happiness? This is especially important regarding the contemporary ethical issues presented throughout the book. How can you address and reach conclusions on complex ethical issues without considering your worldview? The diagram below (Figure 1.2) captures a simple way of considering what contributes to the development of a worldview. Worldview formation can be conceptualized into four broad categories: dispositional, ideological, spiritual, and philosophical. Each of the categories can be made up of many variables. Furthermore, many of these variables can fall along a continuum (e.g., mild, moderate, and severe; low to high; completely disagree Page 58 of 165 to completely agree, etc.). To make matters even more complex, not everyone has put considerable thought into their, well, thoughts. So, people might not fully appreciate the dispositional, ideological, spiritual, or philosophical underpinnings of their views or even daily decisions—do you? Much of the learning around these categories and related variables is also un- or sub-consciously being subtly influenced by culture and language. Any one of these categories and the related variables would give rise to a near-infinite number of permutations (i.e., consider just dispositional variables). Consequently, imagine how complex it would be to consider all of them and then to further consider all of them across time and space. This is the reason why no two people share the same exact worldview. There are just too many variables influencing worldview development, and we rarely have direct access to the cognitive processes involved in worldview development.]2 Contributing Factors to the Development of a Worldview Personal Application A worldview can be a great mechanism for interpreting and making sense of the world. Explain three potential benefits of having a welldefined worldview. How does this add value to your life? Explain three potential problems with a well-defined worldview. Lastly, a worldview can be conceptualized as a mission statement, much like for a business. Consider your worldview and in about 250 words, write your own personal mission statement. [While no people share the exact same worldview, there can be general agreement about important matters, and that agreement can serve as an opportunity for shared vision and values and, as such, norms, rules, and laws. Too often we focus on the minor areas where our worldview does not align and miss the larger areas where they do. Worldview development, for those and other reasons, is of critical importance. Socrates is known for saying, “True wisdom Page 59 of 165 comes to each of us when we realize how little we understand about life, ourselves, and the world around us.” Similarly, Aristotle noted that “knowing yourself is the beginning of wisdom.” King Solomon says in Proverbs 9:10 (English Standard Bible) that “The fear of the Lord is the beginning of wisdom, and the knowledge of the Holy One is insight.” Another good application is to consider Proverbs 3:5-6, “Trust in the Lord with all your heart, and do not lean on your own understanding. In all your ways acknowledge him, and he will make straight your paths.”]2 Annotated Bibliography* The Happiest States in the U.S. Article Review by Blake Fraser Does where people live have an impact on their perceived happiness? Similarly, what states are the happiest? As reported by numerous top news outlets, the top five happiest states are Massachusetts (most happy), Illinois, Connecticut, New Jersey, and New York while the bottom five happiest states are West Virginia (least happy), Arkansas, Mississippi, Kentucky, and Louisiana (KHQA Staff, 2023; Kyler, 2023; Lazar, 2023). Whether it is a scientific journal article, a news article, or something you see on social media, you should always think critically and ask how the definitions and data support the conclusions. In this example, these news reports simply repeated conclusions from a scholarship company named Scholaroo. In the data report, Scholaroo (2023) detailed the methods by which they gathered evidence and made conclusions. Happiness was scored using 7 categories: 1) personal finance, 2) leisure activities, 3) mental Page 60 of 165 health, 4) physical health, 5) employment, 6) personal relationships, and 7) social policies. There were 54 specific metrics across these categories that contributed to the happiness score, some of which included state productivity rate, movie theater visits, hotels and motels, beauty salon visits, depression rates, average income, divorce rates, dating app searches, friendship app searches, drug legalization and drug use, LGBT rights, LGBTQI+ youth protection, and cannabis ’legal status. For example, definitions and data included the frequency of people visiting a musical instrument store per capita, the percentage of households who have a pet, the frequency of people visiting airports per capita, and the frequency of people visiting cosmetic and perfume stores per capita. While some of these metrics may seem relevant for happiness, others seem irrelevant or political. Many of these metrics leaned heavily toward upscale urban environments and actively scored any more rural areas lower. Perhaps the worldview of the study’s authors impacted their ability to effectively manage such a study. Worldviews impact how people see the world and define their experiences. High-quality research requires abstract constructs, like happiness, to be transformed into something measurable, known as operationalization (Morling, 2017). However, sometimes these operationalizations are not a valid (i.e., accurate) measure of the construct; in fact, evidence is necessary to show an operationalization of a construct is valid. Scholaroo’s (2023) operationalization of happiness is not valid and has no support from scientific literature. Ironically, data show people are leaving the “most happy” states and migrating to the “least happy” states and doing so in large numbers (Atlas, 2023; North American Moving Services, 2023). When there is poor operationalization, the construct being measured (i.e., Page 61 of 165 happiness) is misrepresented for something else, likely whatever the researchers want it to be. This is the opposite goal of science, which aims to gain an accurate understanding of the natural world without bias or manipulation. So, next time you see a bold claim about a construct (e.g., happiness, loneliness, depression, love, etc.), figure out how the researchers operationally defined the construct of interest and if it has empirical support. References Atlas. (2023). 2022 migration patterns study. https://www.atlasvanlines.com/resources/migration-patterns KHQA Staff. (2023, May 15). Happy place: Illinois ranked 2nd happiest state in the country, per new study. News Channel ABC 20. https://newschannel20.com/news/local/happy-place-illinois-ranked2nd-happiest-state-in-the-country-per-new-study Kyler, E. (2023, May 15). Don’t worry, be happy—Maryland is 6th happiest state in the country, per new study. Fox 45 News. https://foxbaltimore.com/newsletter-daily/dont-worry-be-happymaryland-is-6th-happiest-state-in-the-country-per-new-study Lazar, K. (2023, May 29). Which U.S. states have the happiest residents? CBS Los Angeles. https://www.cbsnews.com/losangeles/news/which-u-s-states-havethe-happiest-residents/ Morling, B. (2017). Research methods in psychology (3rd ed.). W. W. Norton. North American Moving Services. (2023). Where did Americans move in 2022? https://www.northamerican.com/migration-map Scholaroo. (2023, May 11). Happiest states in the U.S. https://scholaroo.com/report/ happiest-states/. *Contributed by Blake Fraser. © Kendall Hunt Publishing Company. Page 62 of 165 Biblical Integration The Bible does not spend a lot of time focused on the broader concept of happiness, but it does spend significant time discussing joy. First, what is the difference? Happiness is typically explained in terms of a response to a pleasurable or satisfying experience while joy tends to capture a more enduring state of emotions resulting from well-being, success, or good fortune or by the prospect of getting what one hopes for. More simply, happiness tends to be controlled by external circumstances and is reactive while joy occurs despite external circumstances and is proactive. The Bible does capture this more critically with happiness being a more external and fleeting state brought about by bodily satisfaction—the Bible warns against focusing on this state. In contrast, joy is connected to a relationship with Jesus Christ. The former is unpredictable and fleeting while the latter is consistent and eternal. 1 John 2:15-16 15 Do not love the world or the things in the world. If anyone loves the world, the love of the Father is not in him. 16 For all that is in the world—the desires of the flesh and the desires of the eyes and pride of life—is not from the Father but is from the world. Matthew 6:19-21 19 “Do not lay up for yourselves treasures on earth, where moth and rust destroy and where thieves break in and steal, 20 but lay up for yourselves treasures in heaven, where neither moth nor rust destroys and where thieves do not break in and steal. 21 For where your treasure is, there your heart will be also. Galatians 5:16 Page 63 of 165 16 But I say, walk by the Spirit, and you will not gratify the desires of the flesh. 1 Thessalonians 5:16-18 16 Rejoice always, 17 pray without ceasing, 18 give thanks in all circumstances; for this is the will of God in Christ Jesus for you. Isaiah 61:10 I will greatly rejoice in the Lord; my soul shall exult in my God, for he has clothed me with the garments of salvation; he has covered me with the robe of righteousness, as a bridegroom decks himself like a priest with a beautiful headdress, and as a bride adorns herself with her jewels. Philippians 4:11-13 11 Not that I am speaking of being in need, for I have learned in whatever situation I am to be content. 12 I know how to be brought low, and I know how to abound. In any and every circumstance, I have learned the secret of facing plenty and hunger, abundance and need. 13 I can do all things through him who strengthens me. Romans 5:1-11 5 Therefore, since we have been justified by faith, we have peace with God through our Lord Jesus Christ. 2 Through him we have also obtained access by faith into this grace in which we stand, and we rejoice in hope of the glory of God. 3 Not only that, but we rejoice in our sufferings, knowing that suffering produces endurance, 4 and endurance produces character, and character produces hope, 5 and hope does not put us to shame, because God’s Page 64 of 165 love has been poured into our hearts through the Holy Spirit who has been given to us. 6 For while we were still weak, at the right time Christ died for the ungodly. 7 For one will scarcely die for a righteous person—though perhaps for a good person, one would dare even to die— 8 but God shows his love for us in that while we were still sinners, Christ died for us. 9 Since, therefore, we have now been justified by his blood, much more shall we be saved by him from the wrath of God. 10 For if while we were enemies we were reconciled to God by the death of his Son, much more, now that we are reconciled, shall we be saved by his life. 11 More than that, we also rejoice in God through our Lord Jesus Christ, through whom we have now received reconciliation. Religion, Morality, and Worldview Religion and morality have central positions in informing the character of our worldviews. Our values are placed and prioritized according to what has been called our “comprehensive conceptions of the good” (Rawls, 1996). “It is comprehensive when it includes conceptions of what is of value in human life, and ideals of personal character, as well as ideals of friendship and of familial and associational relationships, and much else that is to inform our conduct, and in the limit to our life as a whole” (p. 13). These comprehensive conceptions may include our religious views and any related or independent moral views about the good. “Many religious and philosophical doctrines aspire to be general and comprehensive” (p. 13). Since religion and morality have such an important place in our worldviews, it is important to survey a rough overview of the prominent categories available in the contemporary world. The suffix “ism” in the English language is a powerful one. “Isms” refers to powerful, persuasive, and pertinent worldviews having Page 65 of 165 widespread consequences across individuals, communities, and entire nations. An “ism” cannot be understood in isolation because each one is not completely mutually exclusive from other concepts, consequences, or cultures. But, for the purposes of simplification, it is a good starting point to consider the “ism” around belief in God or the lack thereof. Table 1.1 on the following page captures some of the critical components of various “isms.” You can tell from careful review that many have overlapping features, but it is the distinctions that make the “isms” more so than the similarities. Page 66 of 165 Page 67 of 165 Page 68 of 165 Page 69 of 165 Major Religious Worldviews Again, a worldview consists of a set of expectations one holds about reality; it is a lens through which you understand and translate everything around you (e.g., Why am I here?). Emerging from your worldview is a set of assumptions or beliefs about the basic constitution of reality, about your purpose, good versus evil, and the afterlife (e.g., How I should live?). Extending our definition further, a worldview is a collection of attitudes, values, histories, and expectations about the world around us, which inform our every thought and action. Worldview is expressed in ethics, religion, philosophy, scientific beliefs, and so on (Sire, 2004). Collectively and cumulatively, a worldview is how a culture works out in Page 70 of 165 individual practice and daily living. When you encounter a situation and think, “That is unfair, unjust, or just wrong,” your worldview is working. We have an innate inclination to assume what we believe is normal and natural. We too often view opposing positions as backward and superstitious. Your views are a result of how you were brought up (e.g., my views are rational, balanced, and accurate). Worldviews readily create in-groups and out-groups. If our experiences are limited and our exposure to different people and people groups is narrow, we often do not even realize the ubiquitous nature of our worldview in defining and interpreting our reality; in fact, it is not in our general nature to question our reality (Why would we?). Too often, we become aware of worldviews and their corresponding values only when there is a clash or crisis (Fulford, 2011). Now that people of different faiths can readily travel around the world and live in culturally mixed communities, there are increasing opportunities for such conflicts. This is most easily recognizable when it comes to a religious view or a political view. This is why it is said that “polite company” does not discuss such things. Page 71 of 165 Page 72 of 165 Biblical Integration Science has its overarching goals. Psychology, as a scientific discipline, has its goals. Most organizations, associations, or societies like the American Psychological Association, the Association for Psychological Science, the American Counseling Association, the National Alliance on Mental Illness, the Association for Behavioral Analysis International, the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, and the Society for Neuroscience each has their own focus area, their own charter, and Page 73 of 165 mission. To be a member of one of these groups, you will need to understand their area of focus and requirements for admission. What does it mean to be a member of the Christian faith? What does it mean to have a Christian or Biblical Worldview? Included below are some core scriptures regarding converting larger worldview issues into actionable items. John 3:16 16 “For God so loved the world, that he gave his only Son, that whoever believes in him should not perish but have eternal life. Romans 10:9-10 9 Because, if you confess with your mouth that Jesus is Lord and believe in your heart that God raised him from the dead, you will be saved. 10 For with the heart one believes and is justified, and with the mouth one confesses and is saved. Ephesians 2:4-5 4 But God, being rich in mercy, because of the great love with which he loved us, 5 even when we were dead in our trespasses, made us alive together with Christ—by grace you have been saved. When it comes to creating a charter and mission, we can learn a lot from two critical passages: one from the Old Testament and one from the New Testament. Exodus 20:1-17 The Ten Commandments 1 And God spoke all these words, saying, 2 “I am the Lord your God, who brought you out of the land of Egypt, out of the house of slavery. Page 74 of 165 3 “You shall have no other gods before me. 4 “You shall not make for yourself a carved image, or any likeness of anything that is in heaven above, or that is in the earth beneath, or that is in the water under the earth. 5 You shall not bow down to them or serve them, for I the Lord your God am a jealous God, visiting the iniquity of the fathers on the children to the third and the fourth generation of those who hate me, 6 but showing steadfast love to thousands of those who love me and keep my commandments. 7 “You shall not take the name of the Lord your God in vain, for the Lord will not hold him guiltless who takes his name in vain. 8 “Remember the Sabbath day, to keep it holy. 9 Six days you shall labor, and do all your work, 10 but the seventh day is a Sabbath to the Lord your God. On it you shall not do any work, you, or your son, or your daughter, your male servant, or your female servant, or your livestock, or the sojourner who is within your gates. 11 For in six days the Lord made heaven and earth, the sea, and all that is in them, and rested on the seventh day. Therefore, the Lord blessed the Sabbath day and made it holy. 12 “Honor your father and your mother, that your days may be long in the land that the Lord your God is giving you. 13 “You shall not murder. 14 “You shall not commit adultery. 15 “You shall not steal. 16 “You shall not bear false witness against your neighbor. 17 “You shall not covet your neighbor’s house; you shall not covet your neighbor’s wife, or his male servant, or his female servant, or his ox, or his donkey, or anything that is your neighbor’s.” Mathew 5:1-11 The Sermon on the Mount, the Beatitudes Page 75 of 165 1 Seeing the crowds, he went up on the mountain, and when he sat down, his disciples came to him. 2 And he opened his mouth and taught them, saying: 3 “Blessed are the poor in spirit, for theirs is the kingdom of heaven. 4 “Blessed are those who mourn, for they shall be comforted. 5 “Blessed are the meek, for they shall inherit the earth. 6 “Blessed are those who hunger and thirst for righteousness, for they shall be satisfied. 7 “Blessed are the merciful, for they shall receive mercy. 8 “Blessed are the pure in heart, for they shall see God. 9 “Blessed are the peacemakers, for they shall be called sons of God. 10 “Blessed are those who are persecuted for righteousness ’sake, for theirs is the kingdom of heaven. 11 “Blessed are you when others revile you and persecute you and utter all kinds of evil against you falsely on my account. The Ten Commandments serve as a moral framework, providing guidance on righteous conduct. Jesus, as the Son of God, directly imparts the Beatitudes to the New Testament children of God. The Beatitudes present a spiritual roadmap for attaining eternal life by fostering a closer relationship with God. Subsequently, Jesus ’entire Sermon on the Mount and his ministry over the following three years demonstrate how Christians should embody this spiritual guidance through acts of love. Every individual is called to strive for sanctity. The Ten Commandments are universally acknowledged as the fundamental moral laws to which all Christians must adhere. They instruct us on how to remain steadfast in our devotion to God on physical, emotional, mental, and spiritual levels. Conversely, the Beatitudes elucidate how we can establish the kingdom of God on Earth and partake in the heavenly realm. They reveal the values that hold significance to Jesus. In accordance with the catechism, the Page 76 of 165 Beatitudes occupy a central position in Jesus ’teachings. They demonstrate that those who are marginalized and overlooked in society are, in fact, blessed by God and will receive His eternal love. Both the Ten Commandments and the Beatitudes direct Christians to love God and their fellow human beings. Love is the central theme in Christian values, for God Himself is love. This love fosters the presence of God’s kingdom on Earth, as authentic Christianity revolves around the love we share with one another. The Beatitudes encapsulate the essence of the Ten Commandments; they express what is implicit in the commandments. This interrelation signifies that God’s laws (commandments) are indications of His love and the happiness that stems from abiding by them (beatitudes). Personal Application Regardless of worldview, people can and should focus on kindness. Mohamad Safa is recognized for saying the following: “Our world is not divided by race, color, gender, or religion. Our world is divided into wise people and fools. And fools divide themselves by race, color, gender, or religion.” Nelson Mandela stated, “No one is born hating another person because of the color of his skin or his background or his religion. People must learn to hate, and if they can learn to hate, they can be taught to love. For love comes more naturally to the human heart than its opposite.” Use the internet to search and find 5 inspirational quotes that will help you stay motivated throughout this course. Explaining a Biblical Worldview [To understand what a biblical worldview is, we need to briefly revisit the definition of a worldview. Everybody has a certain way they look at the world—certain truths and frameworks they use, consciously or unconsciously, to understand the world around them. Page 77 of 165 According to Wolters (Lee 2011), a worldview is “the comprehensive framework of one’s basic beliefs about things.” It may sound broad, but that is truly the purpose of this definition. Your worldview, according to Wolters, “speaks centrally to everything in your life and world, including technology, economics, and science” (p. 2). It is your basic framework of everything. It is what you believe to be true about everything and anything in life. It includes from where you believe truth comes, who decides what is true, and how you think about ideas and concepts.]2 We take a strong view of a “biblical worldview,” meaning that we assume some propositions about the Bible’s place in such a worldview that some who use it as a casual guide or just “one of many sources” may reject. [A biblical worldview asserts that truth originates with God, and God shares truth about the world, us, and Himself through the Bible and the world around us. When you hold a biblical worldview, you believe the Bible is the foundation for understanding what is true about the world. Someone adhering to a biblical worldview believes God created the world and the order by which it operates, and He has revealed truth through His Word (the Bible, or Scripture) and through His Works (the world around us that can be studied systematically to understand the truth about the world). A biblical worldview is a framework of what is true for mankind as outlined in the Bible and begins with God being the creator of the world (Genesis 1:1) and mankind (Genesis 2:7-25). © joshimerbin/Shutterstock.com In Genesis 1:1, we see the creation story begin, and in Genesis 1 and 2, we see the creation of mankind in Adam and Eve. We see that they were created in the image of God and were placed in the Garden of Eden where they were given dominion over the rest of Page 78 of 165 creation, and they were in perfect relationship with God (Genesis 1:26-27). God gave them freedom in the garden to live and thrive perfectly; God also gave them a command to not eat of a certain tree in the garden (Genesis 2:16-17). Unfortunately, Adam and Eve disobeyed God and chose to do exactly what He commanded them not to do, and as a result, sin entered the world. This sin broke the perfect relationship between mankind and God and introduced brokenness and death into everything. Because of sin, our bodies, our minds, our relationships, our psyches, our churches, our organizations, our countries, and ourselves are all broken. Nothing exists in this world or in our human experience that has not been touched and broken in some way by sin. Further, we know from Romans 3:23 that the penalty for sin is death and eternal separation from God. Fortunately, however, the story does not end there. The Bible says God had a plan before the foundation of the world to fix this brokenness due to sin and to restore us to perfect relationship with Himself again (Ephesians 1). His plan was to send his son, Jesus Christ, to pay the price for sin through his own death on the cross and resurrection from the dead so that we could be freed from the penalty of death and Hell for our sins (John 3:16-18; 1 Corinthians 15:3-4; Ephesians 2:1-6). Because of Jesus ’death and resurrection, our sins are forgiven and paid for, and we can be restored to perfect relationship with God in Heaven for all of eternity. All we need to do is accept this gift from God (John 3:16-18; Ephesians 2:8-9). The drama of Scripture can be divided into the four-act structure of creation, sin, redemption, and restoration. This Grand Narrative helps us see the bigger story of what God is doing throughout history to bring about His purposes, which culminate in the person and work of Jesus Christ. While many may see the Bible as just a collection of individual stories or moral guidelines, the Page 79 of 165 Bible is actually one continuous story of God’s love for His creation and His active work to restore His creation to Himself. The Bible reminds us how sin and brokenness have affected all things on this earth (ourselves included) but that Jesus has come to bring hope to our brokenness. This Grand Narrative is the basis of a biblical worldview and is a valuable perspective from which we can view all things in the field of psychology and the world. Our understanding of psychology is informed by our biblical understanding of God being the creator of the world, of sin entering the world and creating brokenness in all things, of Jesus coming to redeem us and give us hope for the future, and of our future restoration to God. The Bible contains the truth about human nature, God, and our call as Christians to help others. We can trust the Bible as a legitimate source of truth as we are studying biological, psychological, and social phenomena. We can think through any concept in psychology (and in this textbook) using the creation, fall, redemption, and restoration narrative. Grand Narrative: The metanarrative of the Bible that tells the story of mankind including Creation, Fall due to sin, Redemption through Jesus Christ, and Restoration of mankind to God. This contrasts with the dominant worldview today. Many believe truth only comes from science, logic, and critical thinking. Someone with a modern, secular worldview values data we collect from the natural world over spiritual authority. Someone who would say they had a modern, secular worldview would emphasize empirical, evidence-based ways of justifying beliefs about what is real. They would claim beliefs based on evidence (or science) are more reliable and objective compared to those based on uncorroborated intuition, revelation, religious authority, or sacred texts like the Bible. This belief in science as the Page 80 of 165 foundation for truth has truly dominated the modern era. Chances are, even if you come from a Christian background, you have modern, secular thinking about the authority of research in your thinking patterns. As a Christian in psychology, it is important to recognize God has shared many truths about human behavior in the Bible. Due to our belief in the authority of God and His Word, we can receive those truths with confidence. We can also study His creation with good science and see how He has created us and the truths in human behavior. As a Christian student studying psychology, knowing what the Bible says and what good research shows about human behavior is important to understanding psychological phenomena. For the Christian student, it is important to understand what the field presents about human behavior, but you also need to be aware that science is not the ultimate source of truth. Science is the study of the world around us and is dependent on measuring constructs well, analyzing data well, and coming to the correct conclusions (e.g., the flaw in the happiness study noted previously). Because science is a human phenomenon, it is prone to error. As you will notice, scientific findings and conclusions on topics frequently change as new evidence emerges or as new theoretical perspectives emerge. Therefore, we must always be cautious and think critically when evaluating research. One way we can think critically is by comparing it to the Word of God. Using the “Biblical Integration” boxes throughout the textbook, you will be shown how to accomplish this in various areas of cognitive psychology.]2 Personal Application How is a Christian or biblical worldview different from the other worldviews presented so far? Explain the most important factors in a biblical worldview and their impact on your daily living. If not a Page 81 of 165 biblical worldview, then select another and explain that view’s impact on your daily living. Lastly, explain how your worldview has changed across your development thus far. What life events have impacted your worldview development (e.g., graduating from high school, getting married, having children, becoming ill, starting your career, etc.)? [As we start our exploration of the field of psychology, we want you to keep the following things in mind: 1. As a human endeavor, there is always a chance in psychology that we are wrong in the conclusions we draw from the science we are conducting. We need to be humble in our science and cautious when we draw conclusions about various topics in the field. 2. Additionally, while the Bible is infallible (i.e., has no errors), sometimes man’s interpretation of the Bible can have errors. Therefore, we need to be careful in our interpretation of the Bible as well and make sure we apply good study techniques when understanding what God is saying in His Word about human behavior and the world. 3. Whenever we begin comparing the science of psychology to what God says in the Bible, we can sometimes find areas that do not seem to agree. That can lead us to wonder what is true. Keep in mind that God’s truth is always superior to anything we may determine in our own logic or study. God is the creator of the world, and He knows how His creation works best. We can believe Him even when our minds cannot understand how it all fits together. As we begin our study of cognitive psychology, we will review the history of the field and some of the founding theorists and researchers in the field. We will then discuss the science of psychology and things to be aware of when learning about cognitive Page 82 of 165 psychology. Moreover, we will conclude this introductory chapter with an overview of the major ethics positions. Additionally, in this chapter and subsequent chapters, we will guide you to expand your understanding of cognitive psychology to include a biblical worldview. As noted in the diagram above (Figure 1.3) and throughout our text, we will not just consider complex behaviors from the biological, social, and cognitive-behavioral perspectives across the life- span (as is typically done in our field), but we also will ground it all in the truth of the Bible. Throughout our examination of different topics, we will show you that our present reality and the study of human behavior must consider the divine creation and redemption story outlined in the Bible in addition to any biological, social, psychological, or developmental frameworks. Having a biblical worldview or framework states that we ground all our biological, psychological, social, and developmental theories in the truth (and story) of the Bible. As noted previously, the Bible tells us much about the origin of man, the nature of man, and how we operate; it provides valuable insight to us as we search for truth about human behavior, specifically about areas relevant to cognitive psychology. As we go through various topics in cognitive psychology, we will show you how to integrate biblical truth with psychological science to help you grow in the field and your understanding of human behavior.]2 Figure 1.3 Overview of the Overlapping Components of a Biblical Worldview Across the Lifespan The Effect of a Biblical Christian Paradigm* Pastor Scott Budde Pastor at Mount Carmel United Brethren in Christ, Fulks Run, VA Page 83 of 165 How you perceive reality colors information. If your reality is foundationally confrontational, even cornflake cereal can be a potential enemy to your very existence, but if reality is foundationally an adventure to be experienced, even a skiing accident can be an enchanting and wonderful journey of discovery. The Bible puts it this way: “To the pure, You show Yourself pure” (New International Bible, Psalm 18:25-26). An easily frightened person will be scared of a beetle, and a laid-back person will barely be affected by a near-death experience. Paradigm is the color of your world. What if the color of your world was not consistent? What if the foundation of your reality was not a cornerstone, but a prism: fractured, indistinguishable, and shifting like the sand of the seashore? Jesus contrasted the two models—the cornerstone and the sand—in His parable in Matthew 7:21-27 (New International Bible). He begins by warning the religious crowd that not everyone building on religion would be received by Him in the Judgment: “Many will say to me on that day, ‘Lord, Lord, did we not prophesy in your name and in your name drive out demons and in your name perform many miracles? ’Then I will tell them plainly, ‘I never knew you. Away from me, you evildoers!’” (v. 22-23). Next, He picks up the beginning of the parable by saying, “Therefore,” meaning the reason for the parable is found in the warning about the Judgment. In short, Jesus warns them to build on the Cornerstone, not on man’s fractured teachings about religion. For centuries, possibly millennia, secularists have tried to reconcile the fractured religions of the world through force, coercion, compromise, manipulation, and even treaty. All have failed, on the macro and micro scale. Sure, there have been individuals who changed their hearts, but the fallout was usually more fractures, not fewer. The inconsistency of a worldview within larger, less-defined worldviews continues to ruin minds and divide brothers and countrymen. But what of the Cornerstone, Jesus? Page 84 of 165 Despite the shifting sands of fractured thinking, there are really only two paradigms: Biblical and everything else. The Biblical paradigm calls all other views of reality into account as the offspring of a primordial rebellion against God called “sin.” It rests in a rejection of God as the definition of reality Himself. The Bible testifies against man that he has rejected the reality that IS for a reality of his own invention. As Stephen pointed out to the Sanhedrin in Acts 7:51-53 (New International Bible), they resist God as reality Himself by rejecting His attempts to bring them to a proper ontological foundation through law, prophecy, and even angels. These religious men were so enraged that they killed him (Stephen) who dared to suggest that they had no right to their own version of reality. Men are violent when it comes to defending their foundational concepts. These are more personal and carefully guarded than all other treasures, but the Bible testifies that those concepts are “filthy rags” (Isaiah 64:6). Therefore, we will either be working with those who have courageously embraced the Bible as reality or those who have rejected God and are attempting to twist the fabric of reality and bend it to their own will. The Bible gives us three groupings to expect from society: 1) those who will reject God’s Word outright, 2) those who see God as a means to an end, and 3) those who respect God’s Word but view it as essentially powerless to aid them in their goals (See the parable of the Sower, [New International Bible, Matthew 13:18-22]). However, there is a 4) fourth kind: a people intentionally transformed by God, plowed and cultivated through heartache and suffering to receive the seed of His Word into the deepest parts of their hearts (Matthew 13:23). Page 85 of 165 The first three are on a common quest to invent reality for themselves. The fourth kind is disciples of the works of God and His Son, Jesus (John 17:3). It is the first three groups that need morals— guidelines to keep them governed—so that in their pursuit of a tailored, fictitious reality, they do not destroy everyone around them. These are capable of morality, mutual respect, and patience but cannot and will not become Christlike (Romans 9:16). The value of transformative thinking—God supernaturally bringing the dead in trespasses and sin to life (New International Bible, Ephesians 2:1)—is found in the Cornerstone, Jesus, and finding a foundation already laid in what is true. The born-again disciple is then built as a living stone into the building—the temple—that relies upon the sure foundation. That Christian also builds a life on Christ using sometimes the materials of the flesh (i.e., wood, hay, and stubble) and sometimes the materials of the Spirit (i.e., gold, silver, precious stones). Each round of discipleship is tested with fire, and God brings greater heat as He refines them. The end result is promised in God’s glory—that they will shine like the stars in the heavens, with an incorruptible and immortal glory. This is the ultimate reward for repentance from that primordial rebellion into which they were born (1 Corinthians 3:11-15; 1 Peter 2:5; 1 Peter 1:7; Philippians 2:15; 1 Corinthians 15:52-53; Genesis 15:1). So then, Biblical Christian thought is transformative in nature. It brings you into the reality that IS and denies you the right to all other experiments with reality. While the sciences try to make sense out of the fracturing of the mind racked with pride and despair, there is an explanation as to the cause and even hope for those who have rejected God. However difficult it may be, the doctrines, morals, ethics, and even laws of the Bible can give those who embrace and respect religion at least a means to manage sin and live a good life, even if they ultimately refuse to ask God for mercy and Page 86 of 165 transformation. But, the polarization of the gospel aptly preached will cause them a constant baring of the soul and put most out to invent a compromised religion with which they can live. As Paul said, “I pray to God that not only you but all who are listening to me today may become what I am, except for these chains” (New International Bible, Acts 26:29). As you study through this textbook, you will likely confront many fractures of reality and how they misinform decision-making. Christian thinking counters all attempts at localized ontology. Part of the futility of good work with damaged materials is that the end result can only stand up under so much pressure before it again gives way to its sandy foundations. Brief Summary and Self-Assessment This text discusses the integration of psychology and a biblical worldview. It acknowledges that mainstream psychology is a secular science focused on developing theories and treatments based on scientific principles. However, it emphasizes the perspective of Christian psychologists who seek to understand psychological phenomena through the lens of the Bible. The text aims to provide an understanding of psychology from a biblical worldview by exploring the intersection of psychological science and biblical teachings. It highlights the importance of worldview in shaping our perception of the world and how it influences our beliefs, values, and interpretations. The text also explains that everyone has a worldview and that it plays a crucial role in filtering and making sense of information. It further discusses various “isms” and their impact on worldview formation, including religious and political beliefs. The concept of a biblical worldview is introduced, emphasizing that truth originates from God and is revealed through the Bible and the world. The text presents the biblical creation story, the introduction of sin, and the plan of redemption through Jesus Page 87 of 165 Christ. Overall, the text encourages readers to explore the integration of psychology and a biblical worldview to gain a deeper understanding of human behavior and promote mental health from a Christian perspective. 1.According to the text, what is a worldview? a.A comprehensive framework of one’s basic beliefs about things b.A set of rules and regulations for understanding the world c.A religious doctrine that dictates one’s actions and beliefs d.An individual’s perspective on a specific topic Answer: a) A comprehensive framework of one’s basic beliefs about things 2.According to Socrates, when does true wisdom come to us? a. When we understand everything about life and the world b. When we realize our limitations in understanding c. When we have a well-defined worldview d. When we have knowledge of the Holy One Answer: b) When we realize our limitations in understanding 3.What is the four-act structure that defines the biblical narrative? a. Creation, sin, redemption, and restoration b.Birth, growth, decline, and rebirth c.Observation, hypothesis, experimentation, and conclusion d.Perception, cognition, behavior, and motivation Answer: a) Creation, sin, redemption, and restoration 4.According to the text, what should be kept in mind when studying psychology? a.Psychology is a flawless science with infallible conclusions. b. The Bible discusses all important scientific findings. c.Human interpretations of the Bible are always correct. d.Psychology and the Bible can provide complementary insights. Page 88 of 165 Answer: d) Psychology and the Bible can provide complementary insights 5.How does a biblical worldview inform the study of cognitive psychology? a.By emphasizing biological and social factors over psychological ones b. By excluding developmental frameworks from the study of cognition c. By grounding all theories in the truth and story of the Bible d.By dismissing scientific research as irrelevant to cognitive processes Answer: By grounding all theories in the truth and story of the Bible BRIEF HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Beginnings of Psychology [Psychology is a field that combines both ancient and modern elements. Its origins can be traced back to classic philosophers who explored topics relevant to human nature and behavior. However, psychology as a formal discipline emerged in the late 19th century with the work of Wilhelm Wundt and Sigmund Freud. To understand the origins of psychology, it is important to review the contributions of key philosophers and physiologists who laid the groundwork for the field.]2 Philosophers like Plato and Aristotle made significant contributions to psychology. [ Plato believed that all knowledge and understanding could be attained through introspection and looking inward. He proposed a model of the human psyche consisting of impulses, reasoning, and behaviors. Page 89 of 165 Aristotle, on the other hand, focused on the external influences on human behavior and argued that the mind was shaped by experience and external factors. His ideas influenced the naturenurture debate and shaped our understanding of the structure of the mind. In the 17th century, Rene Descartes introduced the concept of mind-body dualism, suggesting that the mind and body are distinct but interconnected. He proposed a bidirectional relationship between the mind and body, where each can influence the other. Descartes ’ideas on the mind-body connection and involuntary actions have had lasting influences on theories of perception, emotion, and behavior.]1 While philosophy provided a historical foundation for psychology, physiology also played a crucial role in its development. In the late 19th and early 20th centuries, medical doctors trained in physiological research began applying scientific principles to psychological topics. This led to a shift in psychology toward systematic study and the use of the scientific method. Researchers began examining sensation, memory, and other mental processes through empirical observation. The integration of physiological research methods transformed psychology into a field that relied on systematic testing and empirical evidence to understand human behavior. In summary, psychology has a rich historical background rooted in philosophy, with contributions from philosophers such as Plato, Aristotle, and Descartes. However, it was the incorporation of physiological research methods in the late 19th century that paved the way for the modern field of psychology. This interdisciplinary Page 90 of 165 approach, combining philosophy and physiology, allowed for a more rigorous and scientific understanding of human behavior. Early Schools of Thought Wilhelm Wundt (1832-1920) was one of the very first scientists to study psychological principles. Following graduation from medical school in 1855, Wundt worked as an assistant in many early psychophysics and physiology research laboratories in Western Europe. It was during this time that Wundt began to develop his ideas for a new scientific discipline: psychology. Although others helped to contribute along the way, Wundt is considered the founder of modern psychology due to his publication of the first psychology book, Principles of Physiological Psychology, in 1874. Five years later, Wundt established the world’s first psychological laboratory at the University of Leipzig in 1879, which remained active until 1910. It was from this laboratory that Wundt trained the first generation of psychologists who would go out into the world and make psychology the discipline it is today (Benjamin Jr., 2008). © Timofeeff/Shutterstock.com In addition, while establishing the field of psychology, Wundt also made several contributions, which led to the separation of psychology from philosophy. To make this separation complete, Wundt stressed the scientific exploration of conscious experience through the use of empirical methods. To do this, Wundt developed an empirical method, which he called introspection. Introspection is the examination of one’s own mind to inspect and report on personal thoughts or feelings about conscious experiences. In order to properly use this technique, Wundt would rigorously train individuals over months and sometimes years. During the introspective process, individuals were presented with a physical stimulus and were to report on the Page 91 of 165 size, intensity, and duration of their conscious experience. Wundt used the results of each experiment to draw inferences about the elements and processes of conscious experience. Introspection: This school of thought emphasized the examination of one’s own mind to inspect and report on personal thoughts or feelings about conscious experiences. [Can you see any problems with this technique? Unfortunately, much of the criticism of Wundt’s early work centered on his use of introspection. Many researchers disapproved of the introspective method. Specifically, researchers argued there were problems with a method in which results could vary by different observers for the same stimulus, thus leading people to wonder which introspection was correct. Furthermore, since introspection is a private, personal method of study, how can the results be replicated? Despite these issues, Wundt is still considered to be the most important figure in the history of psychology, primarily because of his founding psychology as a science, rejecting nonscientific thinking, publishing extensively on the topic, and training the first generation of psychologists.]1 Structuralism [Edward Titchener (1867-1927) established the first brand of psychology (or school of thought) known as structuralism. Titchener developed structuralism at Cornell University in New York shortly after receiving his Ph.D. from Wundt in 1892. Titchener believed psychology was to be used to study the structure of the mind. Structuralism is the breaking down of conscious experience into its fundamental elements: sensations, feelings, and images. As with Wundt, Titchener sought to use introspection to accomplish this task. However, unlike Wundt who was interested in the whole of the conscious experience, Page 92 of 165 Titchener was more concerned with the parts of the experience that made up the whole (Goodwin, 2008). Titchener suffered from the same criticism as Wundt and when Titchener died, so did structuralism.]1 Structuralism: This school of thought views understanding the mind as a breakdown of conscious experience into its fundamental elements: sensations, feelings, and images. Functionalism [Around the same time that Titchener was developing structuralism, William James (1842-1910), known equally well for his contributions to a philosophical school of thought called American Pragmatism, and colleagues at the University of Chicago were developing the second school of thought in psychology: functionalism. Functionalism was a deliberate protest against the work of Wundt and Titchener. Functionalists believed that the work of Wundt and Titchener was narrow in scope and could not be applied to the real world. To overcome these limitations, James and colleagues adapted their work from the work of Darwin and his principles of evolution. Specifically, rather than focusing on the conscious experience only, functionalism sought to study how the mind of an organism adapted to the current environment (Goodwin, 2008). This was a major change from Wundt and Titchener, who did not care about the outside environment, only the internal state. Functionalism: The school of thought seeks to study how the mind of an organism adapted to the current environment rather than just focusing on the conscious experience. With the advent of functionalism, psychologists were able to expand the scope of psychology to children, the mentally Page 93 of 165 impaired, and even animals. These populations were not used with structuralism because it was believed they were unable to be trained in introspection. Unfortunately, as with structuralism, functionalism lost ground around the 1920s with the death of James and the introduction of behaviorism. However, some similarities do exist between functionalism and a contemporary perspective known as evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary psychology analyzes an information processing system as a biological system evolves through natural selection. In this model, the system will encounter environmental selection pressure and experience cognitive adaptation (e.g., Cosmides & Tooby, 1995).]1 Behaviorism [In the early 1920s, concurrent with the popularity of a philosophical school of thought called logical positivism, a major shift occurred in psychology with the introduction of behaviorism by John Watson (1878-1958). Watson and colleagues (such as B. F. Skinner, 1904-1990) believed that psychology should move away from using subjective procedures (i.e., introspection) and begin to engage in objective procedures. Watson’s solution was behaviorism, which is the scientific study of the prediction and control of behavior. In behaviorism, anything that could not be observed—such as mental processes, thoughts, and feelings— could not be studied. Behaviorism: A scientific study of the prediction and control of behavior. Behaviorists were focused on how humans and animals acquired and modified behavior, which they called learning, because of their environment. Behaviorists believe people’s development is little more than a result of learning that occurs in their world. While Page 94 of 165 seemingly like functionalism, behaviorism focused solely on behavior and ignored the mind or mental interaction with the environment. However, behaviorists such as Watson and Skinner differed on the type of learning that led to the development of people (Goodwin, 2008). Behaviorism was the dominant theory in American psychology until the early 1960s when other contemporary ideas began reshaping the psychological landscape.]1 Reductionism The world around us, how we perceive it, how we react to it, as well as every thought we have is solely dependent upon the way in which the neurons/cells in our brains are organized. This view is called reductionism. From a scientific viewpoint there is no such thing as the “mind,” only a complicated network of cells, which when working properly allow us to think, speak, read, walk, and perform other complex tasks. Consciousness is merely a naturally occurring byproduct of our brain’s neural activity. Based on these assumptions concerning the brain, and given enough time and resources, scientists should be able to determine exactly how the brain functions, even to the point of manipulating thought processes (i.e., with drugs, electric current, and artificial sensory experiences). Reductionism: The scientific position that all behaviors will eventually be explained by smaller and smaller particles and processes. HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY The Cognitive Revolution Page 95 of 165 Before we move forward, it is important to understand exactly what cognitive psychology is. The term cognitive psychology was first coined by Ulric Neisser (1928-2012) in his 1967 book, Cognitive Psychology. Cognitive psychology is defined as the scientific study of information processing in the mind. It assumes three things: (1) mental processes exist, (2) mental processes can be studied scientifically, and (3) people are active processors of information. [Outside of the United States, psychologists in Germany and Austria were forming a new school of thought, partially in opposition to ideas such as structuralism and functionalism. Gestalt psychology emphasized the role of patterns and structure in perception and was known for stating that the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. What is important to note right now is that ideas outside of behaviorism were occurring, and as World War II (WWII) forced many psychologists to relocate to the United States (such as Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Kohler, and Kurt Koffka), they brought these ideas with them. There were others in psychology looking at cognitive-type ideas before the so-called revolution occurred. For example, in 1932, Frederick Barlett was exploring how memory is reconstructive in nature and how existing schemas shape how information is recalled. In addition, even in the late 1800s, Hermann Ebbinghaus (1885) was using himself as a participant to study learning and forgetting. Gestalt psychology: The school of thought that emphasized the role of patterns and structure in perception. It is known for stating that the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. Within the United States following WWII, the dominant issue in psychology was the search for generalized laws of human behavior. Page 96 of 165 There was a small group of researchers who found this debate increasingly irrelevant to understanding human cognition. This group was influenced by several factors: 1. Page 97 of 165 Page 98 of 165 1. Dissatisfaction with stimulus-response tradition as it was too limited to understand complex behavior (e.g., see Breland & Breland, 1961). 2.The inability of academic psychologists involved in WWII efforts to apply behaviorism concepts and principles to solve problems of attention and vigilance in military personnel (for review, see Lachman et al., 1979). 3. Research by George Miller (1956) demonstrating limitations in information processing. 4. Chomsky’s (1959) review of B. F. Skinner’s work on language acquisition, dispelling the notion that stimulus-response psychology can explain language acquisition and usage. 5.Use of computer systems as a metaphor for how the human mind operates.]1 All these factors created a perfect storm if you will. Psychologists were ready to explore other explanations for human behavior, outside of those provided in behaviorism’s stimulus-response paradigm. Many of the historically significant psychologists are included below in Table 1.3. Understanding the history of science and scientists adds value in terms of understanding not just their Page 99 of 165 specific contribution in terms of content but the context in which it occurred. Page 100 of 165 Page 101 of 165 Page 102 of 165 Key Christian Contributors [In addition to the field of psychology tracing back to philosophers like Plato and Aristotle or more recent theorists and researchers such as Wundt, Freud, and Watson, several key individuals have contributed to the investigation of psychology from a Christian worldview (Table 1.4). These Christian contributors include classic philosophers and more recent researchers and theologians. As secular philosophers and researchers were presenting their ideas explaining human behavior and existence, these Christian philosophers and researchers were also presenting ideas of how the Bible helps inform our understanding of human life and how we can discern and know truth. Table 1.4 Overview of Key Christian Contributors to Cognitive Psychology Page 103 of 165 Page 104 of 165 Augustine (354-430) was a prominent Christian philosopher of the first millennium who exerted a profound influence on Western philosophy and Christianity. Born to a pagan father and devout Christian mother in Thagaste, Augustine displayed a fervent passion for knowledge from an early age and engaged with various philosophers of his time. In contrast to the skeptics of his era, Augustine asserted that truth was eternal and unchanging. Central to his theory of knowledge was the concept of divine revelation, which he believed was conveyed through both the Bible and natural phenomena. Augustine maintained that all truth originated from God and relied on His revelation to humans, including scientific truth. The famous phrase, “All truth is God’s truth” originated from Augustine, encapsulating his belief that truth emanates from God and is disclosed to humanity. Present-day Christian researchers continue to echo Augustine’s convictions, affirming the existence of absolute truth that can be discovered through God’s revelation in both the Bible and the study of creation, including scientific endeavors. Thomas Aquinas (1225-1274), known for merging the rational and supernatural realms, wielded a significant influence in both secular and sacred domains. By synthesizing Greek philosophy with Christian doctrine, Aquinas proposed that rationality and the study of nature constituted valid pathways to comprehend truths pertaining to God. According to Aquinas, God revealed Himself through the natural world, prompting the study of nature as an avenue to explore and understand the divine. In contrast to the dichotomy often perceived between philosophy and theology, as well as science and theology, Aquinas perceived these disciplines as complementary in the pursuit of truth. His perspective fostered the integration of seemingly disparate fields, fostering a Page 105 of 165 more harmonious relationship. Similar to Augustine, Aquinas held that all knowledge was contingent upon divine revelation from God. Richard Baxter (1615-1691), an English Puritan pastor and theologian, made substantial contributions to comprehending psychological ailments from biological, psychological, and spiritual perspectives. In his writings and teachings on depression, Baxter delineated various factors that could contribute to the experience of depression. He emphasized the need for individual evaluation to identify the specific causes underlying an individual’s depression, thereby enabling effective treatment. Baxter posited that depression could stem from both biological disturbances and behaviors inconsistent with God’s will, emphasizing a balanced approach that accounted for both physical and spiritual aspects. As one of several influential Puritans, Baxter significantly contributed to the development of Christian psychology and biblical counseling. Their reliance on Scripture’s authority, systematic diagnostic approaches to identify root causes (spiritual, temperamental, and physical), and utilization of the Gospel in treatment laid a solid foundation for subsequent Christian scholars and counselors. Fritz Kunkel (1889-1956), a German-born psychiatrist, played a pioneering role in modern psychology by integrating psychological and Christian perspectives. Collaborating with Alfred Adler in Vienna, Kunkel emerged as one of the earliest proponents of integrating psychology and theology. He coined the term “integration” in 1953 to describe his work, which aimed to combine psychological and theological principles. Kunkel is widely regarded as a founding figure of the modern integration movement. His research and practice emphasized the essential connection to God for mental well-being. Kunkel established the Foundation for the Advancement of Religious Psychology in California in 1951 and Page 106 of 165 founded a Christian counseling center in Los Angeles, further promoting the integration of psychology and theology. Since the introduction of the term “integration,” it has acquired various connotations, encompassing the integration of psychology and theology in the pursuit of understanding human behavior and incorporating theological truths into counseling practice to enhance well-being. Eric Johnson, a contemporary theologian and Christian psychologist, is renowned for his book, Psychology and Christianity: Five Views (Johnson, 2010). Johnson explores the growth of the field of Christian integration in psychology since the term’s initial usage in 1953. He elucidates several perspectives regarding the intersection of psychology and Christianity. These views encompass a spectrum ranging from the belief that secular psychology, due to its inherent secular bias, should not be employed in conjunction with the Bible to understand human behavior, to the conviction that integrating secular scientific findings with biblical teachings can be accomplished successfully under the guidance of the Holy Spirit. This approach involves allowing divine revelation to inform the interpretation of both Scripture and scientific knowledge. Johnson’s ongoing work in communicating diverse perspectives on how psychology and Christianity can be employed to comprehend human behavior continues to shape the field.]2 IDEA DEVELOPMENT: REACTIONARY OR REVOLUTIONARY In many ways, humans, especially scientists, are more reactionary than revolutionary. Often, it is easier to be critical of another person’s model or theory compared to developing one of your own. It is not surprising, then, that much of the advancement we see in the sciences occurs through one person or group reacting critically to the theories of others. Interestingly, as one person or group reacts to Page 107 of 165 the prevailing view, over time, those reactions build momentum and eventually bring forth new hypotheses, models, theories, and practices. Those then-new hypotheses, models, theories, and practices then get challenged, resulting in a continuous positive feedback loop, often resulting in an overall advancement of the field. However, holding too tightly to certain schools of thought can impede progress. For example, psychology emerged from dissatisfaction with philosophy because philosophy lacked any reproducibility. This gave rise to more laboratory-based work, but much of that work was based too heavily on observation and lacked appropriate controls. The reaction was to create a stimulus-response model based on rewards and punishments, which was highly controlled. The stimulus-response model lacked generalizability. The reaction to this lack of generalizability was to consider organisms, especially people, as thinkers, creators, and problem solvers, which gave rise to cognitive psychology. But, even still, cognitive psychology was criticized for being unable to explain the fundamental mechanisms of thought. This gave rise to cognitive neuroscience, which, in the simplest terms, allows scientists to develop cognitive models (e.g., addressing the big “why”) and utilize neuroscience to help explain the underlying physiology and chemistry (e.g., addressing the big “how”). Over longer periods of time, scientists can begin to synthesize and integrate the best ideas from the various schools of thought and provide a more comprehensive understanding of human behavior— like a biopsychosocial model across the lifespan. This is why being a good critical thinker is exceedingly important as well as having some perseverance, but neither of those will serve you well if you do not have considerable content knowledge. Page 108 of 165 Personal Application Explain in what ways specifically you are dissatisfied with the field of psychology. What improvements would you like to see in the field and why? How will you help advance the field of psychology? Explain how you see cognitive psychology adding value to this process. Considering the schools of thought discussed thus far, which best aligns with your thoughts on psychology, and which is the most different? Page 109 of 165 CONTEMPORARY COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Cognitive psychology is an area of scientific study that examines complex psychological phenomena such as perception, attention, memory, learning, reasoning, problem-solving, decisionmaking, language, and creativity. While cognitive psychology is still a relatively new field, it has been charged with undertaking some of the most significant and complicated questions of our time. For example, how the brain produces a conscious experience is one of the most fundamental questions of human existence. Other unanswered questions include how does the brain create a perception from simple isolated stimuli? How are memories stored in and subsequently retrieved from the brain? What is selfawareness and are people unique in their ability to consider who they are? How are decisions made and how do decisions become actions? Cognitive psychology: The scientific study of information processing in the mind. As you move through this course, you will consider these questions and many more and likely several of your own questions (curiosity and introspection are also perplexing cognitive qualities). Due to the complex and diverse nature of these questions, many different scientists within psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, computer science, and linguistics, contribute to our understanding of cognitive processes. This diversity lends itself to many different areas of study within the field of cognitive psychology, including but not limited to these, which will be covered throughout this textbook: Page 110 of 165 1.The influence of marketing on consumers ’spending. 2.How individuals decide whether to purchase a product online or in stores. 3.Composition of police lineups and implications for identified suspects. 4.Creating effective learning strategies. 5.Techniques for questioning eyewitnesses or victims of crimes. 6.Formation of false memories. 7.Second language acquisition during development. 8.How experts solve problems. 9.How to identify and combat misinformation. 10.Development and improvement of artificial intelligence. Such diversity of perspectives can provide additional challenges for students studying cognitive psychology; however, when you consider that, ultimately, nearly every behavior is derived from a cognitive process, it immediately brings to life the importance and inherent value of developing a better understanding of how people think. Throughout the textbook, we will explore these areas together. Therefore, you might find cognitive psychology to be surprisingly intellectually stimulating because it is very diverse in scope, and, more than likely, there is at least one area of cognitive psychology closely related to your primary interest in the more general field of psychology. Concepts, terms, and theories from Page 111 of 165 cognitive psychology surround you every day. It is just not always apparent they are grounded in cognitive psychology. However, once you see the connections, you will be able to relate what you are reading to your personal and professional life. For example, of great interest right now is the development and use of artificial intelligence. Have you considered the impact of artificial intelligence on our personal and professional lives? Do you think artificial intelligence could replace professionals such as physicians and psychologists? Professionals in these fields certainly do not think so. While artificial intelligence might do more in the near future along the lines of diagnosing and treatment planning, it would not replace the importance of human-to-human interaction, especially with important human traits like empathy. However, a recent study showed this is exactly where artificial intelligence does well. More specifically, ChatGPT outperformed physicians in not only diagnosing but also with higher empathy (Ayers et al., 2023). ChatGPT is an AI language model developed by OpenAI capable of understanding and generating human-like text responses to a wide range of queries, making it an advanced conversational agent with diverse applications such as chatbot interactions, content creation, and language understanding tasks. (This sentence was written by ChatGPT.) Annotated Bibliography* AI Outperforms Doctors? A Review of Ayers et al. (2023). Article Review by Virginia G. Cramer As artificial intelligence (AI) technology continues to advance, AI tools like ChatGPT have been shown to read, analyze, and generate information with amazing speed and accuracy. This technology is already being used to supplement (or even replace) the human Page 112 of 165 workforce in data analysis, finance, advertising, graphic design, education, and other fields (Abril, 2023). However, is it possible that AI could take on the jobs of nurses, doctors, psychologists, and therapists, whose jobs rely on ‘real ’interactions with patients? For better or worse, this may soon become a reality. In their crosssectional study, researchers Ayers et al. (2023) demonstrated that AI chatbot technology could provide more accurate and empathetic responses than real doctors. Using 195 randomly drawn questions from an online Reddit forum, r/AskDocs, physician answers were compared to responses generated by ChatGPT. The quality and empathy of each response were evaluated by three separate licensed healthcare professionals. Not only did the evaluators prefer the ChatGPT responses to the physician responses 78.6% of the time, but the ChatGPT responses were also rated as significantly higher in quality (t =13.3; P < .001) and empathy (t =18.9; P < .001). The average rating of the physicians ’responses was 21% lower in quality and 41% less empathetic than the ChatGPT responses. These findings suggest that AI may outperform real doctors at responding to patient questions submitted through online portals. In their conclusion, Ayers and colleagues (2023) suggest that utilizing AI assistants to respond to patient questions may be able to lighten the burden of busy healthcare workers. Additionally, it could provide patients with responses that are more detailed, accurate, and empathetic than an overworked human doctor may be able to offer. While the real-life performance of ChatGPT in clinical settings has not yet been tested, it might not be long before patients speak with AI assistants rather than doctors and nurses. References Page 113 of 165 Abril, D. (2023, March 20). AI isn’t yet going to take your job — but you may have to work with it. The Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/technology/ interactive/2023/aijobs-workplace/ Ayers, J. W., Poliak, A., Dredze, M., Leas, E. C., Zhu, Z., Kelley, J. B., Faix, D. J., Goodman, A. M., Longhurst, C. A., Hogarth, M., & Smith, D. M. (2023). Comparing physician and artificial intelligence chatbot responses to patient questions posted to a public social media forum. JAMA Internal Medicine, 183(6), 589-596. https://doi.org/0.1001/jamainternmed.2023.1838 *Contributed by Virginia Cramer. © Kendall Hunt Publishing Company. Cognitive psychology asks and answers many important questions in the field of psychology. Cognitive science interfaces with a broad range of other fields, especially artificial intelligence, making cognitive psychology a highly sought-after area of knowledge. The first step in building important content knowledge is being able to ask the right questions because questions drive curiosity, and curiosity drives innovation. Cognitive psychology is a broad field covering many topic areas related to perceiving, attending, thinking, and decision-making. Table 1.5 below captures many of the major areas of focus when it comes to understanding cognitive psychology. The domains below encompass the core areas of this textbook. The table below provides an excellent conceptual outline of the book along with definitions and examples. Table 1.5 Page 114 of 165 Major Areas of Study within Cognitive Psychology Page 115 of 165 Personal Application Looking at the top ten list above, what domain/sub-field is most interesting to you and why? Write one extra question for each of the ten domains/sub-fields within cognitive psychology. Explain how each of these areas can be applied to your personal and professional life. Lastly, explain what you think will be a prevailing ethical issue in three of the areas. Brief Summary and Self-Assessment Psychology is a multifaceted field that has evolved through contributions from ancient philosophers, key figures in the late 19th century, and subsequent schools of thought. The origins of psychology can be traced back to philosophers such as Plato and Aristotle, who explored topics related to human nature and behavior. Plato emphasized introspection and looking inward as a means to attain knowledge, while Aristotle focused on external influences and the role of experience in shaping behavior. In the 17th century, Rene Descartes introduced mind-body dualism, proposing that the mind and body are distinct yet interconnected. This concept influenced theories of perception, emotion, and behavior, highlighting the bidirectional relationship between the mind and body. The integration of physiological research methods in the late 19th and early 20th centuries marked a significant shift in psychology. Medical doctors with training in physiology began applying scientific principles to psychological topics, leading to a more systematic and empirical approach. This transformation allowed researchers to study sensation, memory, and other mental processes Page 116 of 165 through empirical observation, contributing to the emergence of psychology as a scientific discipline. Wilhelm Wundt is considered the founder of modern psychology. He published the first psychology book, Principles of Physiological Psychology, in 1874 and established the first psychological laboratory in 1879 at the University of Leipzig. Wundt emphasized the scientific exploration of conscious experience through introspection, a method where individuals examine and report on their own thoughts and feelings about conscious experiences. However, introspection faced criticism due to its subjective nature and varying results among different observers. Edward Titchener developed structuralism, which aimed to break down conscious experience into its fundamental elements such as sensations, feelings, and images. Titchener also relied on introspection as a method of study. William James and colleagues developed functionalism as a response to the narrow scope of Wundt and Titchener’s work. Functionalism focused on how the mind of an organism adapted to the environment, expanding the scope of psychology to include children, the mentally impaired, and animals. In the early 1920s, behaviorism emerged as a major shift in psychology, led by John Watson. Behaviorists emphasized the scientific study of behavior and rejected subjective procedures like introspection. They focused on how behavior is acquired and modified through learning, disregarding the role of the mind or mental processes. The reductionist view suggests that the brain is solely responsible for all thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. It posits that understanding the brain’s functioning will eventually lead to manipulating thought processes through drugs, electric currents, and artificial sensory Page 117 of 165 experiences. Reductionism aligns with the belief that psychology will be reduced to biology and biology to chemistry as our understanding of the brain advances. Overall, psychology has a rich history intertwined with philosophy and physiology. From its ancient roots to modern schools of thought, the field continues to evolve, incorporating interdisciplinary approaches and scientific methods to gain a deeper understanding of human behavior. 1.Who is considered the founder of modern psychology? a. Plato b. Aristotle c. Wilhelm Wundt d. Sigmund Freud Answer: c) Wilhelm Wundt 2. What method did Wundt develop to examine conscious experience? a) Behaviorism b) Introspection c) Structuralism d) Functionalism Answer: b) Introspection 3.Which school of thought focused on breaking down conscious experience into its fundamental elements? a. Behaviorism b. Introspection c. Functionalism d. Structuralism Answer: d) Structuralism Page 118 of 165 4. Which school of thought emphasized the study of how the mind adapts to the environment? a. Structuralism b. Introspection c. Behaviorism d. Functionalism Answer: d) Functionalism 5. Reductionism in psychology suggests that: a. The mind and brain are two different kinds of substances. b. Consciousness is a byproduct of neural activity. c. Psychology will eventually be reduced to biology. d. Behavior is shaped by experience and external factors. Answer: c) Psychology will eventually be reduced to biology. AN ETHICS PRIMER Scientific Practice and Ethics Ethics is the study of right and wrong, or good and bad. Culture sends mixed messages about what that means. On the one hand, people tend to think right and wrong are just personal choices based entirely on a person’s culture, upbringing, internal feelings, or preferences. Is abortion okay? “Well, some people think it is and others do not.” Is polygamy wrong? “It depends on your moral code.” These kinds of responses make it seem as if there are no real answers to questions about right and wrong. In short, they imply that ethical answers are relative or subjective. On the other hand, if we ask the right questions, we get very different responses. Is racism okay? “No, absolutely not. We should all be inclusive.” Is pedophilia wrong? “Yes, pedophiles should not be able to act at will with children!” These kinds of responses make it seem as if there are real answers. Ethical answers with this view Page 119 of 165 are objective—they are true or false regardless of whether people believe or practice them (Shafer-Landau, 2017, p. 597). In this book, we work on the assumption that ethical answers are objectively true or false, and they are not relative to individuals or cultures. For example, even if an entire society and culture believed that slavery was okay, we still think it would be unethical, or wrong. Even if we assume ethics is objective, that does not make finding ethical answers easy. Think about how we treat science. Science, as most people believe, has objective answers. Earth is spherical even if some (or even many) people believe it is flat. We believe, similarly, in many scientific truths, but that does not mean that it has been easy to find them. Some scientific truths have taken thousands of years of investigation. (For example, for over 1,500 years, most of the European civilization believed the sun orbited the earth.) Even though we sometimes treat science as if we have discovered everything there is to discover, this is far from the truth. We are continually searching for new scientific truths, and we believe they are “out there” for us to find. In this book, we treat ethics in a parallel way. Right and wrong are not things we simply decide or things our families and cultures can just tell us. We must investigate; we have to work hard; and we have to discover what is ethically true. Ethical truth, in other words, is out there in the world, even though it can be really difficult to find it. This is why ethics is so important in the study of psychology. All forms of science can be useful and powerful. That we have such power in our hands gives us the opportunity to do incredible good or incredible evil. We will discuss three main applications of ethics in psychology: scientific discovery, professional practice, and Page 120 of 165 personal virtue. Scientific Discovery and Ethical Practice Most of the greatest atrocities of modern times have relied on the tools of modern science. Modern weaponry alone has changed the landscape of killing in war abroad and crime in our streets. We are all familiar with the ways nuclear technology can be used as a threat to humanity or how sophisticated weapons make it easier for those with evil intent to commit terrible crimes. While those dangers are important, they have become too familiar, especially in Hollywood movies, and in many of our imaginations, they involve fields like physics or chemistry—often portraying an evil scientist with diabolical and criminal practices at the helm of a well-funded lab. So, what do any of these dangers have to do with psychology? Well, psychology has a checkered past, much like any other science. Psychologists have conducted research in unethical ways. Experiments with electroshock therapy, attempting to elicit reactions by displaying progressively horrific images, lobotomy, or even the use of orphans and other vulnerable populations are all good reminders that the quest for discovery can sometimes lead even well-meaning scientists astray. For example, in experiments known as “Operation Midnight Climax,” the CIA employed prostitutes in the 1950s and 60s to lure people into safehouses where they would be administered drugs and interrogated in the name of psychological discovery for the sake of national security. As another example, for over three decades, Dr. Walter Freeman performed lobotomies by sticking a long narrow instrument (like an ice pick) up through a patient’s eye socket to sever the connection between the frontal lobes and the thalamus (Caruso and Sheehan, 2017). Page 121 of 165 You might further wonder what unethical experiments have to do with more practices within the field of psychology. Most people who study psychology will not end up conducting Nobel-prizewinning experiments. Even fewer will have the hubris to violate experimental subjects ’rights just to produce novel results, but the unethical behavior of individual researchers is not our only concern. Anyone in the field is a representative of their community. Psychology’s problems are problems for psychologists, psychiatrists, neuroscientists, and counselors, at least to some extent. Public distrust in the sciences, for example, may affect public opinion about psychology or general confidence in the effectiveness of counseling. Trust in scientists has gone down in recent years (Kennedy, 2022). It does not help that there has been a recent high-profile “replication crisis” in psychology specifically that has caused many to have negative perspectives about the reliability of psychological science (Open Science Collaboration, 2015, p. 943). Anyone wishing to pursue a career in psychology should be equipped with understanding and the ability to put unethical conduct into perspective. What causes psychologists to push the boundaries? Can we still use important data that is acquired through unethical conduct? How do we talk about giants in the field whose behavior raises serious ethical questions? We need to take a sober look at unethical behavior from the past to understand how we can proceed in a better way. This can only be done if we have taken the time to consider some of these past mistakes. For example, it is difficult to understand African Americans ’distrust of general medical institutions unless you look at the long history of mistreatment from medical professionals toward the African American community in the last three centuries. Page 122 of 165 In the twentieth century alone, there are shameful examples of experimentation, neglect, and the violation of basic rights. According to Pew Research, only 30% of Democrat or Democratleaning African Americans have “a great deal” of confidence in medical scientists compared to 52% of Democrat or Democratleaning white Americans (Kennedy, 2022, p. 10). Unethical behavior has long ripple effects in the sentiments of cultures across the world. Deciding when to trust medical authorities or scientists can turn on rumors and traditions that reverberate through affected communities for decades or even centuries. This is why ethics in scientific discovery is important for any practitioners of the field, whether they work in a lab or with the daily concerns of a steady clientele. There are many mundane ethical decisions practitioners of psychology must face on a regular basis. Sadly, many do not even recognize them as ethical decisions at all. Because we worry so much about high-stakes situations, like the patient who wishes to self-harm, we forget to think through the things we encounter every day. This brings us to the final application of ethics in psychology, one that everyone faces inside or outside the field. Personal Virtue We all imagine ourselves on the right side of history when we are not experiencing it in real time. Would you be a Nazi in 1930s Germany? Of course not! If you were a physician treating a patient from the Tuskegee Syphilis experiment (where researchers persuaded physicians not to treat African American men who had the disease) would you have the courage to stand up for them? Yes! Who would not? Yet, we can say we would act ethically all we want, and it means nothing until we are honest enough to admit our weaknesses. Page 123 of 165 The fact is people act according to their character. If you have a habit of lying, you will continue to lie. That does not mean you will lie all the time, or that you will not have occasional moments of candor. However, it does mean that consistent lying leads to greater ease of lying in the future (Garrett et al., 2016, p. 1727). There is nothing surprising about this. The implications of this fact are immense. We often think of ourselves as “the good guys.” We imagine ourselves as having morally virtuous character, even though the objective evidence is to the contrary (Tappin & McKay, 2016, p. 630). Numbers do not lie. If even slightly more than half of people believe they are better than average, then at least some of them must be wrong. It is up to psychology to figure out why we self-inflate our character. However, as moral agents, it is up to us to work harder to counteract that tendency and to be more honest with ourselves about our vices (i.e., our moral weaknesses). Here, an old principle from Aristotle is helpful. If you lack virtue in some area, such as courage, then the world will look different for you than it should. If you are a coward, then bravery will look excessive to you (Aristotle, 1999, p. 28). You might see someone bravely rush into a burning building to save lives and think, “That person is rash! He is going to get himself killed!” When we have a vice, virtue looks out of place. That is why it is so important to think hard about our own character. Do you merely assume you are brave, or do you have actual evidence of it? Look at your own behavior. Do you merely assume you are honest, or have you actually told the truth, even when it was hard? Aristotle’s insight is that bad habits change everything about the way we see the world. The only way to make progress is to first admit to ourselves the uncomfortable truth that in some respects, we are not good people. Page 124 of 165 It is incredibly important to recognize our weaknesses before we are tested with moral choices. Perhaps we think of ourselves as good people. By an assumed transitivity, we believe we will be good counselors, scientists, or teachers. This is a basic mistake. Instead of thinking about whether we meet the minimum threshold for membership in the “good person” club, it may be more beneficial to assume we have at least some bad habits. What are they? If we recognize those bad habits early, then we can work on them before we are burdened with great professional responsibility. We will be ready when we get to a place where our behaviors will hugely affect patients, coworkers, or subordinates who encounter our professional activity. And this is the point. Whether we like it or not, who we are as professionals cannot be completely separated from who we are as private people. Transitivity: A property or relationship in which if one thing is related to a second thing, and the second thing is related to a third thing, then the first thing is also related to the third thing. Personal Application Reflect upon your life so far and consider what your greatest strengths and weaknesses are. How has experiencing your strengths and weaknesses impacted your development and, specifically, your ethical and moral development? If you could send a 300-word moral or ethical message back in time to your younger self, what would that message be? Consider this example. In 2008, a study was conducted on the publication of data relating to the effectiveness of antidepressants through clinical trials (Turner et al., 2008). The authors found that 94% of the trials published conveyed a positive result. However, the FDA registry for such studies indicated that only 51% of trials related to these approved antidepressants reflected a Page 125 of 165 positive result. The discrepancy is accounted for by the fact that 31% of drug trials registered by the FDA for these antidepressants were never published. Since publication is the only way such data are typically communicated to the general public as well as to medical professionals, the effectiveness of the antidepressants in question was inaccurately perceived by many who would rely on the data in making important decisions. We do not know, of course, what motives come from authors, funding sources, journal editors, or others involved in the publication process, but we can point out some ways in which such a lack of full knowledge about pharmaceutical effectiveness applies to ethics. First, even if everyone in the publication process has good motives, the system itself may cause problems in the administration of prescription drugs. Who is at fault if a medical professional prescribes such drugs inappropriately (or without conveying an accurate measure of their effectiveness to the patient)? Should they be checking the FDA database regularly to see whether the published material is reflective of the full set of results? Should the drug companies promoting the drug go out of their way to point out the danger of misperceptions about its drug? Should they act contrary to what is best for the shareholders, even if the misperception is unrelated to any of their positive actions? A second problem relates to ethics codes and legality. Professions that require a lot of training often also place a heavier burden of responsibility on their practitioners. So, legal requirements and ethics codes can seem oppressive and difficult. They are also quite helpful for keeping professionals within the scope of good practice. In fact, there is no indication that the selective publishing described above violates any law or ethical code. However, ethics is Page 126 of 165 ultimately in the hands of each individual moral agent. Whether we like it or not, we are responsible for the choices we make, and even when we follow the guidelines before us, such as the law or ethics codes, we still have not fulfilled our moral burden. We must think beyond these to determine the right course of action. Finally, public perception is important in ethical practice. We may not be misrepresenting the effectiveness of most drugs, but for psychological treatments to be effective, there must be enough public trust in the practice. If there is not, then whatever effective treatments exist are useless if no one is willing to utilize them. There are some fairly subtle ways that industrial interests can undermine public trust. They may encourage consent or dissent that is favorable to those interests even when they are not reliable, or they may rely on a narrow understanding of trustworthiness (e.g., complying with formalized/standardized procedures) when we have good reason to suspect the process is unreliable in any particular instance (Pinto, 2020). Biblical Integration One of the important attributes of critical thinking is critical feedback. What is the point of seeing a situation that is not optimal and then keeping it to yourself? But unfortunately, that is the most common response, it seems. For example, in nearly every new hire onboarding training are processes and procedures for ethical conduct. What those trainings do not actually accomplish is helping people create effective strategies for critical feedback as well as navigate intrapersonal and interpersonal discomfort. Ethical behavior and thoughtful reflection are qualities that improve with practice and feedback. If unethical behavior, even small, was challenged earlier on, then it is very likely that many people would behave more ethically. Just building a culture of pro-ethics instead Page 127 of 165 of anti-wrong would help. People who engage in unethical behavior do so, in part, because they have not been challenged on it or have not been challenged on it in ways that are effective. Foundational to academic and scientific work are critical feedback and the peerreview process. Doing this correctly starts with a change in spirit from one focused on discipline to one focused on discipleship. The Bible has a lot to say about ethical behavior and provides strong recommendations for engaging with others. One of the reasons people find it hard to correct others is because they do not want to be corrected themselves. So, one of the best approaches if you are a leader is to create an environment where feedback—critical feedback—is the norm. Another reason people find it hard to correct others is because of the pervasive notion of not judging others. So, one of the approaches is for leadership to create an environment where they model the highest ethical standards. This is not to say that if you are not doing either of those activities, you should not address unethical, immoral, or illegal behavior—you should. It will be easier to do so though if you are living according to the Scriptures. The first set of Scriptures is for you to help reset yourself and the second set of Scriptures is for you to help reset others. Psalm 25 Teach Me Your Paths 1 To you, O Lord, I lift up my soul. 2 O my God, in you I trust; let me not be put to shame; let not my enemies exult over me. 3 Indeed, none who wait for you shall be put to shame; they shall be ashamed who are wantonly treacherous. 4 Make me to know your ways, O Lord; Page 128 of 165 teach me your paths. 5 Lead me in your truth and teach me, for you are the God of my salvation; for you I wait all the day long. 6 Remember your mercy, O Lord, and your steadfast love, for they have been from of old. 7 Remember not the sins of my youth or my transgressions; according to your steadfast love remember me, for the sake of your goodness, O Lord! 8 Good and upright is the Lord; therefore, he instructs sinners in the way. 9 He leads the humble in what is right, and teaches the humble his way. 10 All the paths of the Lord are steadfast love and faithfulness, for those who keep his covenant and his testimonies. Galatians 6:1-10 1 Brothers, if anyone is caught in any transgression, you who are spiritual should restore him in a spirit of gentleness. Keep watch on yourself, lest you too be tempted. 2 Bear one another’s burdens, and so fulfill the law of Christ. 3 For if anyone thinks he is something, when he is nothing, he deceives himself. 4 But let each one test his own work, and then his reason to boast will be in himself alone and not in his neighbor. 5 For each will have to bear his own load. 6 Let the one who is taught the word share all good things with the one who teaches. 7 Do not be deceived: God is not mocked, for whatever one sows, that will he also reap. 8 For the one who sows to his own flesh will from the flesh reap corruption, but the one who sows to the Spirit will from the Spirit reap eternal life. 9 And let us not grow weary of doing good, for in due season we will reap, if we do not give up. 10 So then, as we have opportunity, let us do good to everyone, and especially to those who are of the household of faith. Page 129 of 165 Setting yourself right before God is important but so is helping set someone else right before God. The person you challenge may not thank you at that moment, but if they follow your correction, they surely will eventually. Even if you are not thanked and their behavior is unchanged, it is important to practice humble, critical feedback. Page 130 of 165 ETHICS OVERVIEW Introduction Traditionally, ethics (i.e., moral philosophy) is split into three categories: metaethics, normative ethics, and practical ethics (also sometimes referred to as applied ethics). Metaethics asks the most basic questions about the nature of ethics. Metaethicists (i.e., people who practice metaethics) attempt to find answers about what moral judgments really are, how they function in linguistic expression, what their relationship to truth is, and other subject matter broadly related to ethics. Normative ethics moves the discussion to more fine-grained questions about how we determine the status of ethical claims, judgments, or principles. Ethicists on this level seek to find answers about the best ways to understand the basis of our firmly held moral judgments. What makes a person’s claim that “torture is wrong” true or effective? Is there an overlap in the features that ground that claim and those that ground other firmly held judgments? Various theories seek to answer these questions and thereby offer a practical guide for how to conduct oneself in a morally good way. Finally, practical ethics seeks to find answers to ethical questions at the most practical level. This usually involves addressing nuances of a particular arena of human action. For example, the decisions made by physicians or nurses might look very different from those made by a businessperson. Nurses do not typically have to think about the stakeholders of the organization they work for (though they are often limited in their capacity to act by decisions made by those who do consider stakeholders). Page 131 of 165 Metaethics: The study of the theoretical foundations of ethics, including the metaphysical, epistemological, psychological, and linguistic presuppositions of the study and practice of ethics. Normative ethics: The study of the criteria of right and wrong (or good and bad) actions, intentions, or character. Practical ethics: The study of right and wrong (or good and bad) actions, intentions, or character in particular circumstances, on particular subjects, or within particular areas of academic, professional, or personal life. The relationships between these three categories of ethics (Table 1.6) and the boundaries of each are quite controversial. Some ethical theories may transcend these boundaries. For example, Divine Command Theory rests on the claim that actions are right or wrong insofar as they are commanded by God (or loved or endorsed by God). This might be a normative theory because it points to a feature that unifies all moral judgments, thus giving us a guiding principle for morality. According to Divine Command Theory, why is it wrong to torture? Because as a principle, it is wrong to do what God commands us not to do, and God has commanded us not to torture. Divine Command Theory might also function on a different level as a metaethical theory because it explains the nature of morality. What do we mean by moral “rightness” or “goodness”? Just that those actions, intentions, or habits have the property of being in accordance with God’s commands. Table 1.6 Brief Overview of the Three Categories of Ethics Page 132 of 165 METAETHICS Some Questions in Metaethics 1.What are moral judgments? Are they statements of fact? Emotional responses? Commands? 2.Is there moral truth? Are there objectively true moral claims, independent of people’s preferences or beliefs? 3.What are the foundations of moral statements? God’s commands? Social agreement? Natural features of the universe? *There is a bit of controversy over whether social and political philosophy is a subdiscipline of ethics. Some consider it a sui generis or independent field of normative philosophy. It is also debatable how the three categories interact. There is undoubtedly some interaction among them, but is it a necessary interaction and under what conditions does one category affect another? One example of controversy is whether we should call the bottom level of ethics “practical” or “applied.” Some Page 133 of 165 philosophers object to the term “applied” because it indicates we are taking a theory from above—a normative theory—and simply using its principles in a particular arena of action down below, in a specific field of practical ethics. For example, if ethics in medicine (i.e., biomedical ethics) is “applied” in this way, we might conclude that doctors should just act according to our preferred normative theory. Therefore, biomedical ethics is dependent on and subservient to our normative theory. However, many practical ethicists object to the idea that there is any such dependency. In fact, some postulate that lower-level ethics, like biomedical ethics, might be just as influential, or more, on normative ethics than the other way around. For example, if we are very confident that a doctor should not do some action, X, but a normative theory says the doctor should do X, well, maybe that normative theory is not our best option. In any case, philosophers disagree on how the three categories of ethics interact. Nevertheless, these categories are widely used, at least for practical reasons. Most of the discussion in this book centers on normative and practical ethics. This is partly because cognitive psychology as taught is a practical endeavor. Given its practical nature, it is probably more fruitful to see connections between cognitive psychology and ethics on a practical level all around. Having said that, there will be some exploration into metaethics. In fact, there already has been some at the very beginning of this section of this chapter. The discussion of the “Scientific Practice and Ethics” section above begins with a metaethical question—whether there are objectively true moral standards. Below, we turn to normative ethical theories. Normative Ethics Since normative ethics plays a significant role in this textbook, it is important to give a brief overview of some of the major theories in Page 134 of 165 use today. In this chapter, we discuss all three of the normative ethics categories. First, utilitarianism in its modern form was most famously endorsed by Jeremy Bentham and John Stuart Mill, English philosophers of the 18th and 19th centuries. Bentham is famous for developing the hedonistic calculus, the basic principle underlying his form of utilitarianism, which says that pleasure determines moral good. Actions are right insofar as they promote “the greatest happiness of the greatest number [of people],” where happiness is just the presence of pleasure and the absence of pain (Bentham, 1891 p. 93). Thus, to determine the right act in a given situation, we must figure out the consequences of the act. This is where the term “consequentialism” applies. Consequentialism is a family of theories that define right action according to its consequences (Anscombe, 1958, p. 12). As a theory that determines right action solely by its production of pleasure, utilitarianism is a famous example of consequentialism. There are other consequentialist theories as well. For example, egoism may also take pleasure in being what makes actions right, but its concern is only with the actor’s pleasure, not the pleasure of the greatest number of people. If you are an egoist, you do not care about others ’pleasure, only your own. If you are a utilitarian, you care about your pleasure and everyone else’s as well. So, utilitarianism is one form of consequentialism that many ethicists have found to be appealing. Mill departed from Bentham in a few ways. First, he distinguished between quantity of pleasure and quality of pleasure. For Bentham, it seems the pleasure calculus is simple—the more the better. Mill noticed that there is something unique about different kinds of pleasures. Can we just compare the pleasure amounts of watching a well-crafted film and Page 135 of 165 eating cotton candy? Even if you get a lot of pleasure from eating cotton candy, is it really as good as watching a great film? Mill thought there were higher and lower pleasures (Mill, 2001, p. 89). This might explain why people can sometimes go for extended periods without food if they are engaged in a fulfilling activity. They would prefer to experience a more fulfilling pleasure, even if they get a lot of pleasure from eating. For Mill, the higher pleasures are associated with intellectual activities (i.e., those that engage the “higher faculties”) and the lower with non-intellectual activities. It is preferable, according to Mill, to experience the higher pleasures over the lower ones, though he did not work out in great detail how this works in practice. Another disagreement between Mill and Bentham is whether the focus of a utility calculation should be on individual acts or broader rules that govern individual acts. Act Utilitarianism, endorsed by Bentham, indicates that we should assess the morality of individual acts by each of their individual sets of consequences, by their ability to promote the greatest pleasure compared to alternatives. We should only care about this act in particular, not the general practice of acting this way. Rule Utilitarianism, arguably endorsed by Mill, indicates that actions are right if they comply with rules that would if followed, promote the greatest pleasure. Imagine a set of rules that would create the most pleasure for the greatest number. Does the act comply with that set of rules? Then it is right. Briefly, you can see how these come apart. You might think that as a policy it is bad to lie to your friends just for your own benefit. If you believe that this rule would produce the most pleasure compared to other rules about honesty and then you always follow the rule, even when you think it might produce more pleasure to violate it on occasion, then you are using Rule Utilitarianism. However, if you violate the rule at times when you know it will Page 136 of 165 produce more pleasure to violate it than to follow it, you are really (ideally) assessing each act individually and using Act Utilitarianism. There are some perceived problems with accounts like utilitarianism. Perhaps the most significant is that it is difficult for utilitarianism to account for justice, specifically the protection of rights (Rawls, 1999, p. 23-24). To use a famous example, is it right for a surgeon with five patients who are dying of organ failure to kill a healthy individual of the same blood type to harvest his organs and save the five? (Thomson, 1985, p. 1396). If you are an Act Utilitarian, as long as the conditions are right, it is difficult to say why the surgeon should not do it. Especially if the patients are contributing members of society, their lives will probably produce more pleasure overall than the one whose organs are harvested. The temptation is to say that it could never happen that way, and there are probably very few instances that are even close to something like that in real life. However, the point of the hypothetical is to expose a flaw in utilitarianism, whether anything like that would ever happen or not. The point is that you should feel a revulsion to the very idea of organ harvesting for the purpose of pleasure production. The fact that we are even considering it seems contrary to justice. Mill attempted to solve some of these problems with Rule Utilitarianism. Justice, he believed, could be accounted for by appealing to rules that would produce the most amount of pleasure. As a general rule, it is not pleasure-increasing to go around harvesting the organs of innocent people so that others can be saved. In fact, that kind of practice sounds dystopian and intensely frightening. So, according to Mill, we can come up with principles of justice by thinking about the most pleasure-producing rules and following those without deviation. There are objections to Page 137 of 165 his view and much literature about whether Rule Utilitarianism is workable, but such matters are beyond the scope of this text. While concerns about justice were part of the motivation for Mill to develop Rule Utilitarianism, there is a more straightforward response in Deontology. Deontological ethics define right action in terms of what corresponds to duties, or moral requirements. This means that right or wrong action can sometimes be right or wrong entirely independent of the consequences. It might be wrong for you to take someone else’s property even if you would get much more pleasure from it than they do. The wrongness of your taking it might have nothing to do with the pleasure that would be gained or lost. It is just not right. Immanuel Kant was a German philosopher who did most of his work in the 18th century. He believed that moral duties could be derived from principles of rationality for any rational being, and his work produced a long line of adherents to what we now call Kantianism. He is famous for developing the idea of the Categorical Imperative—a requirement that is not dependent on your plans, desires, preferences, or whatever. It is a requirement for any rational being regardless of their particular goals. While he characterized the Categorical Imperative in a number of ways, here we just focus on two. First, he says you should “act only in accordance with that maxim through which you can at the same time will that it become a universal law” (Kant, 2008, p. 31). A “maxim” is just the rule by which you act, including your reasons for doing it. If you cannot rationally take that rule (i.e., the maxim) and have everyone else act similarly, it is morally prohibited. To take an example from Kant, can you borrow money from another with the intention of not paying it back? The rule or “maxim” of your action is just that it is Page 138 of 165 okay to borrow money with the intention of not paying it back on the basis of your self-benefit. Now, as long as you are the only one doing it, there are no problems with rationality. You get their money, and you do not even have to pay it back. But imagine if everyone did it. What would happen? Kant points out two problems. First, your main motivation for borrowing with the intention of not paying it back is to benefit yourself at the expense of others. However, if everyone acted that way, then anytime you lent money out, you would also not get paid back. Thus, universalizing that rule would undermine your self-benefit, which was the very motivation for doing it in the first place. Additionally, the very notion of borrowing would be undermined if everyone acted that way. The whole basis for the social practice of borrowing money is the promise and assurance that the lender will get it back. Yet, if everyone “borrows” money with the intention of not paying it back, it is not really borrowing at all. In fact, we would have completely eliminated the practice of borrowing from society. So, says Kant, it is wrong to borrow money with the intention of not paying it back. The maxim above also fails on another formulation of Kant’s Categorical Imperative. He writes, “So act that you use humanity...always at the same time as an end, never merely as a means” (Kant, 2008, p. 38). In other words, in any interaction you have with others, you must act consistently with their goals, not simply your own. When we borrow with the intention of not paying it back, we treat the lender as a means of achieving our own goals, but their welfare is completely ignored. If we were to imagine what they want, we would realize that they would not give us the money in the first place if they knew we did not plan to pay it back. If they did not expect it back, they would have just given it as a gift. So, by using the language of borrowing, we know that the person has as their goal to receive that money back. Thus, it would be wrong for us to borrow it and not pay it back. Page 139 of 165 There are, of course, problems with Kantianism as well as utilitarianism. Nonetheless, these theories make up the most widely defended versions of deontology and consequentialism, respectively. Kantianism’s focus is on moral duties that are not always determined by consequences. Utilitarianism’s focus is entirely on the consequences of our actions. Kantianism and utilitarianism dominated philosophy in the English-speaking world for much of the 19th and early 20th centuries. However, a revival of an ancient ethical framework occurred in the second half of the 20th century, to which we turn now. A third major category of ethics focuses on virtue. While there are many types of virtue ethics, the most widely known comes from the ancient Greek philosopher, Aristotle. Aristotle’s unique kind of virtue ethics is what we call Eudaimonism. The word comes from the Greek, “eudaimonia.” Eudaimonia, often translated simply as “happiness,” is a fuller sense of happiness, well-being, or thriving. Aristotle hypothesized that humans are directed toward a certain kind of life, and it is morally better to live consistently with that “end” or goal. The best kind of life for humans, what makes us unique from other creatures, is the life of virtue. The life of virtue is fine, pleasant, and beneficial. It is also characterized by patterns or habits of action, not just singular actions and not a person’s inherited capabilities. Just like you cannot be said to be a good guitar player without practice and continual play, you cannot be virtuous without practice and continual virtuous habits of action. What makes us able to do this is that we are creatures of reason and self-reflection; we do not act primarily on instinct like other creatures. Virtue is also inexact, according to Aristotle. Now, we must be careful not to read that to mean it is arbitrary because it is not. He Page 140 of 165 means it is inexact in the same way as medicine—where health looks different in different people and there may be slightly different ways to be healthy. Imagine two friends, one seven feet tall and 250 pounds, and the other five feet tall and 105 pounds. The doctor tells a seven-foot-tall person to consume around 3,500 calories per day. If that friend reports the recommendation to their five-foot-tall friend, should that friend also consume 3,500 calories per day? Of course not. Yet just because the amount they consume is different does not mean it is arbitrary. In fact, what makes us distinctive physically is what makes our standards of health appropriate. This is also true of virtues. If you encounter someone being mugged in an ally, should you intervene? Well, it depends. If you are very good at recognizing danger, if you have a strong ability to de-escalate stressful situations, if you are a good fighter, then perhaps you should intervene. If none of that characterizes you, then perhaps the virtuous thing is to call for help and just try to distract the mugger. But just because virtue calls one person to intervene and another to find other means does not make virtue arbitrary. Aristotle attempts to capture virtue by the Golden Mean. Every virtue, he says, lies between a vice of excess and a vice of deficiency. The excesses and deficiencies are identified relative to the specific element about which behavior is virtuous or vicious. For example, bravery is a virtue relating to courage. To be virtuous, you should neither have too much courage nor too little. You can have too much courage either by (1) having courage about the right kind of thing, but having too much of it, or (2) by having courage about the wrong kind of thing. It is virtuous to have courage in battle but not if you have so much that you rush irrationally into extremely dangerous situations. It would also be a mistake to have the courage to swim in a vat of hungry sharks just for the pure adrenaline rush. For bravery, then, we might call the vice of excess rashness and the vice of deficiency cowardice. You cannot live virtuously by Page 141 of 165 being rash or by being a coward. Instead, you should have measured courage in the right kinds of situations. The cultivation of virtues is also supposed to be action-guiding, according to Aristotle. If you want to know what you ought to do in a particular situation, try to imagine what a virtuous person would do. Do you want to know what is generous at this moment? Imagine a generous person. That person will not be stingy but will also not be wasteful. The person will probably not give money to anyone but will not hoard it either. That is how we determine the right action. While Aristotle gave examples of specific virtues, others have added virtues, such as simplicity (Gambrel & Cafaro, 2012, p. 324) or benevolence (Hursthouse, 1999, p. 8). Additionally, a tradition has emerged relating twentieth-century feminist critiques of deontological and consequentialist theories with a refocus on virtues of care. The ethics of care tradition focuses on virtues related specifically to relationships, such as sympathy, compassion, fidelity, love, and friendship. These virtues, according to ethics of care, have been seriously neglected in the literature on ethics (partly because ethics was dominated by masculine virtues leading up to the midtwentieth century) (see Gilligan, 1987). Personal Application Think about a fairly uncontroversial moral claim, such as, “It is usually wrong to torture, and always wrong to do it just for fun.” Think of why this claim might be true according to some of the major moral theories discussed above. Why might it be true according to utilitarianism? How about Kantianism? Virtue ethics? Are there any subtle differences between the way each of these might have to change the claim in order to accept it? Critical and Ethical Thinking and Science Page 142 of 165 To be clear, critical and ethical thinking cannot be separated from science. Effective learning and thinking often start with questions, not answers. An important way to create active engagement is to frequently ask questions about the content. So much of psychology and ethics is not well-defined or absolute. Science is imperfect and ethics is complex, so being able to think critically will do much to advance your learning and thinking. There are several strategies for critical thinking, but for now, let us keep it simple. You probably learned way back in grade school how to think like a journalist or detective, and this is a natural extension of that strategy. It is the Who, What, Where, When, Why, How, and Are approach (Figure 1.4 and Table 1.7). Page 143 of 165 Page 144 of 165 Figure 1.4 The Who, What, Where, When, Why, How, and Are Approach to Critical Thinking Table 1.7 Overview of the Who, What, Where, When, Why, How, and Are Approach to Critical Thinking Page 145 of 165 Page 146 of 165 Page 147 of 165 François-Marie Arouet, who wrote under the widely known pseudonym, Voltaire, was born on November 21, 1694, in Paris, France, and died on May 30, 1778. He was considered one of the greatest of all French writers. Voltaire had a famous quote that seems especially fitting. He said, “Judge a man by his questions rather than his answers.” It is impossible to know everything, and it is equally impossible to know if everything we know is correct— especially in science when facts evolve with improvements in research. Therefore, fundamental to advancing science and ethics is being able to ask good questions. Critical thinking might be one of the most important skills you can develop across your education and will serve you well, given how fast information is being presented across various mediums like radio, television, internet, apps, etc. The list of questions above should be very helpful as you go through this book, specifically in addressing many contemporary ethical issues. Page 148 of 165 COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY IN THE REAL WORLD Conceptual Approaches to Understanding VIDEO [In the 1970s and 1980s, a man named David Marr developed an interesting way of looking at phenomena in science. Marr, a neuroscientist interested in vision, was concerned with finding a great way to describe the entire process of vision, from the bottom to the very top. In order to describe the entire process, he soon realized that he needed to think about vision as a system. Specifically, it was an information processing system. Humans take in information from the environment (light waves), then process the information on biological structures in the eye (retinas), and then finally process the information in ways that can be useful for solving day-to-day problems (visual perception). To Marr, this was a clear case of something known as “multiple levels of explanation.” Basically, there are many ways of explaining the same phenomenon, and all are necessary to fully understand it. However, in the realm of science, a particular researcher may only focus on one level of explanation and then later collaborate with others who are approaching the same problem from a different level of explanation. Marr (1982) laid out his framework to include three different levels of explanation (Figure 1.5). Page 149 of 165 be a very complicated problem to solve; however, as you will discover in this book, the rewards of many experiments show us that the human mind is more fascinating than you can imagine! The lowest level of explanation, according to Marr, is the physical level. This is where we try to explain in a very concrete way where an event is occurring. For our example, this might involve neuroscientists exploring the literal changes in neuron structure and function (a process called long-term potentiation), which accompany the learning of new information. Marr’s tri-level hypothesis is one of several different ways to conceptualize the role of cognitive psychology in the larger field of psychological science, or even science in general. It also shows how different areas of psychology can help one another. In the example of memory, evolutionary psychologists, cognitive psychologists, and neuroscientists may all be studying the same problem but from different perspectives. More importantly, the framework also clearly points to the problem cognitive psychologists are uniquely trained to Page 150 of 165 solve: How does the human mind really work? Therefore, just like you may have been as a child who was curious about a new toy, cognitive psychologists like to carefully look at things and try to figure out how they work. Of course, unlike curious children, we do not pull apart our participants to see how they work! We will look at some examples of research from the past and present. Training and Automaticity Let us take a brief look at some examples of cognitive psychology research. The first example dates back to 1935. In a series of experiments, J. Ridley Stroop (1935) wanted to know what would occur when a person was presented with a color word (e.g., red) written in an incongruent ink color (e.g., green) (Figure 1.6). Would the participants be able to ignore the interfering task? It was considered an inference task because reading, after much training, Page 151 of 165 practice, and experience, becomes automatic, at least in terms of individual word identification. For the first experiment, Stroop (1935) had a group of participants read the words for Stimuli 1 (black ink) and Stimuli 2 (incongruent ink). Interestingly, results showed no difference between these two types of stimuli in terms of reaction time. That is, participants did not demonstrate any challenges reading the color words when printed in black or in a color inconsistent with the semantic meaning of the word. Subsequently, Stroop had a group of participants indicate the color of the ink used for each word (Stimuli 2) and for Page 152 of 165 each set of blocks (Stimuli 3). The participants had to say the color independent of the word written for Stimuli 2 and indicate the color of the set of blocks. Unlike the first task, results for the second experiment showed a large difference between Stimuli 2 and Stimuli 3 in terms of reaction time. Specifically, participants had a difficult time saying the color of the ink for Stimuli 2 as compared to saying the color of the ink for Stimuli 3, leading to longer reaction times. Stroop reasoned that the trained, automatic response of reading was interfering with the identification of the printed word’s ink color. Stroop’s study has seen many variations since its original publication almost 80 years ago. In fact, researchers have used a version of the Stroop Task in well over 700 research experiments! Framing and Decision-Making Another well-known study in cognitive psychology is on judgment and decision-making. Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky (1984) were interested in knowing if how a situation is presented to people will impact the decisions that people make. Specifically, they asked participants to imagine the United States was preparing for an outbreak of an unusual disease, which was expected to kill 600 people. Next, they presented participants with two alternative programs to combat the disease. 1.Option A: 200 lives will be saved. 2.Option B: 1/3 probability that 600 lives will be saved and a 2/3 probability that no people will be saved. Results indicated most participants preferred Option A. What is interesting, though, is the decision participants made when presented with Options C and D. 1.Option C: 400 people will die. Page 153 of 165 2.Option D: 1/3 probability that nobody will die and 2/3 probability that 600 people will die. In this situation, most participants preferred Option D. This is surprising because Options A and C are identical in terms of the number of lives saved. These results support the idea that individuals ’decisions are impacted depending on how the situation is framed. This is known as the framing effect, and we will examine it in more detail later in this book. A great example of the framing effect is noted in the picture below. If you had to select one of the three ice creams with overall health in mind, which one would you select and why? This is a powerful and frequently used marketing tool.]1 Fallibility of Memory Another example of research by cognitive psychologists explores how our memory of events can change over time and even how false memories can be created. Loftus and Pickrell (1995) asked 24 participants to recall childhood events. Participants were told the events were provided to the researchers by a close relative. Three of the four events were true while the fourth event was a false story created by the researchers, and this made-up story was about being lost in a shopping mall. The participants listened to all the stories and were asked to write as many details about all events as possible. Later, the participants were interviewed about the four events. When asked to write about the events, seven of the 24 participants remembered being lost in a shopping mall, although their relatives confirmed participants had never been lost. In subsequent interviews, six of the 24 participants continued to report details surrounding the memory of the false event. This was one of the first studies documenting the fallibility of memories, and this has Page 154 of 165 important implications for our legal system, which relies on eyewitness testimony. Eyewitness testimony is a hallmark in our collective cultural psyche. Just consider the relative comparison between how many times you have actually been in a courtroom versus the number of times you have seen it in a movie or on television. When we think about a courtroom criminal case, we often also think about a savvy attorney asking an eyewitness, on the stand and under oath, several questions about their memory of events for a crime. There are, of course, some discrepancies between real-life court cases and these fictionalized versions on television and other forms of media. However, we still have an uneasy relationship with eyewitness testimony in court cases. Robberies, assaults, and many other types of crimes occur quickly and often without any type of warning. They also often occur in the evening or at night and in areas with low levels of light. These qualities exacerbate the intensity of surprise the victim or bystander feels during the criminal act. While memory is obviously important to accurately recalling these events, an often missed and critical cognitive process that is missed is attention. A recent meta-analysis (Marr et al., 2021) demonstrated that high-stress situations may lead to lower identification accuracy in eyewitness memory for faces. This reduction in memory is also observed for eyewitness recall. Why? This has to do with the hijacking of attentional resources during a stressful event. When stress is at a very high level, this can reduce attentional resources, thus resulting in less focus on any single event in the surrounding environment. Since attention is an important component of the encoding process in memory, limiting attentional resources through stress can impair encoding and ultimately result in Page 155 of 165 a diminished ability to retrieve accurate and detailed memories of an event later. Why is this important? It is important because most situations involving robbery—and other crimes—also involve a high level of stress or anxiety on the part of victims or even bystanders. This high level of stress can have impairments on memory. Since eyewitness memory can be an important piece in locating and convicting a person in a criminal case, the accuracy of those memories should always be assessed. Psychologists have also used information about trauma and stress to create better treatments for those who have experienced trauma including cognitive behavioral therapy and, even more broadly, having a trauma-informed perspective of care. This is an example of how cognitive psychologists can contribute to serious discussions in the arenas normally reserved for experts in criminal justice. Crisis and Trauma Cognition* Kenyon Knapp, Ph.D., LPC Dean of the School of Behavioral Sciences, Liberty University A crisis is by definition when an event or situation exceeds the person’s current resources and coping mechanisms (Kleespies, 2009), and crises usually precede trauma. Trauma is “an experience that causes a person to feel afraid, overwhelmed, out of control, and broken. Trauma affects how people view themselves, others, and the world around them” (Corrado, 2020). Crises and traumas can come from multiple sources, such as a sudden health crisis, sexual assault, domestic violence, school violence, natural disasters such as an earthquake or hurricane, death of a loved one (particularly if it is a spouse or a child), or a host of other situations. It is significant that every year, millions of people experience the same crises and traumas Page 156 of 165 and yet have very different reactions and outcomes to these events. This is because how each of us perceives the crisis plays a major role in how detrimental or even beneficial the given crisis is for us (Callahan, 2009, p. 21). Think about a crisis or trauma in your life…how did you react to it? Did you believe that you were helpless, that this was a personal attack on you because of some characteristic about you, that the crisis would never end, and/or that this would ruin all your future dreams? Or, did you view this as bad but something you could learn from, something that was going to grow you as a person, and/or something that God must have allowed for some reason or greater purpose? This is the difference between those who suffer and then descend into depression and other diagnosable conditions and those who suffer and then experience“ Post-Traumatic Growth” (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 1998). This crucial difference in how people perceive a crisis or trauma is an illustration of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT), the most research-validated theoretical approach in the mental health field. CBT is very useful and effective in treating trauma survivors (Aten, 2012) by helping them cope with trauma symptoms, posttraumatic stress, anxiety, and depression (Hamblen et al., 2006; Taylor & Weems, 2011). Fortunately, the breadth, depth, and efficacy of trauma treatments have increased dramatically in the last decade, both in assisting people in finding meaning in their traumas and in symptom reduction. Any discussion of crisis and trauma cognition also needs to address how trauma burns the memory into our brains, and at the same time, often blurs or distorts the memory. When confronted by a threatening or actual traumatic situation, our brains pour epinephrine (also known as adrenaline) into our bodies, triggering fast increases in blood pressure, heart rate, and increased lung capacity, which Page 157 of 165 sends more oxygen to the brain and increases the sharpness of sight, hearing, and other senses (Harvard Medical School, 2020). It would seem logical to assume that increased oxygen to the brain would consistently produce more accurate cognitions, but this is often not the case. Loftus (2019) notes that when research participants watched a 30-second video of a bank robbery, the average participant said the robbery lasted 147 seconds, and a follow-up replication study found that 152 seconds was the participants ’ average estimation of the time. Traumatic moments often feel ‘slow motion ’in the memory of the traumatized person. Bornstein (2017) found that when a weapon was present during a crime, it decreased the ability of the witness to accurately identify a perpetrator. In more extreme abuse cases, victims frequently experience depersonalization (a sense of being separated from your body and watching your abuse occur, as if it were someone else), derealization (a sense that what is occurring must not be real), or other survival mechanism distortions such as Stockholm Syndrome (where the victim develops a strong emotional bond with the abuser [Brewster, 2002), such as the Elizabeth Smart case]. Trauma experts theorize that depersonalization and derealization are merciful, unconscious coping mechanisms that our brains shift to when the present moment is too painful for the person to fully experience in the moment. Later, with trauma-informed therapy, the victim is often able to consciously reintegrate these experiences that their brain segmented off in various ways. Traumatic events cause people to ruminate about them as they try to make sense of what happened. This rumination can be automatic in the form of PTSD-style uncontrolled flashbacks, or more controlled deliberate rumination where the person (often best done with a trauma-informed therapist) consciously chooses to think about the Page 158 of 165 trauma and cope with the memory in a better way. For many Christians, their faith serves as a strong resiliency factor (Pargament & Cummings, 2010) by helping them cope, make meaning, adapt, or even grow from a difficult experience. Some Christians take on the perspective of Paul, who said in 2 Corinthians 4:16-18 “Therefore we do not lose heart. Though outwardly we are wasting away, yet inwardly we are being renewed day by day. For our light and momentary troubles are achieving for us an eternal glory that far outweighs them all. So we fix our eyes not on what is seen, but on what is unseen, since what is seen is temporary, but what is unseen is eternal” (New International Bible). You can see Paul’s perception of the crisis here is in a posttraumatic growth perspective as he refers to “momentary troubles are achieving for us an eternal glory,” and later when he says, “we fix our eyes.” This is equivalent to deliberate rumination on the redemptive process in his troubles. For a Christian, faith in the love and purposes of God is also highly predictive of resiliency and posttraumatic growth, though in no way denying or diminishing the actual suffering. The common refrain from all those who suffer is, “Why?” The logical response for a person of faith is to develop their theodicy—the theological explanation for the problem of evil and suffering in the world (Aten et al., 2017). Developing a comprehensive, coherent, and durable theodicy is a necessary and bumpy process for most people, but many classic books have developed on this topic over the millennia. Reading those often helps people with this process. Having a ‘ministry of presence’—being with someone through a very dark period, not Page 159 of 165 with the ‘right words ’but with sincere empathy, listening, and time—often gives the support the struggling person needs. References Aten, J.D. (2012). Disaster spiritual and emotional care in professional psychology: A Christian integrative approach. Journal of Psychology & Theology, 40, 131-35. Aten, J.D., Schruba, A., Entwistle, D.N., Davis, E.B., Ranter, J., Hwang, J., Hook, J.N., Wang, D.C., Davis, D.E., & Van Tongeren, D.R. (2017). In Gingrich, H.D. & Gingrich, F.C. (Eds.) Treating trauma in Christian Counseling. Intervarsity Press Academic. Bornstein, B. (2017). Popular myths about memory. Lexington Books. Brewster, M.P. (2002). Domestic violence theories, research, and practice implications. In A.R. Roberts (Ed.) Handbook of domestic violence: Intervention strategies (pp. 23-48). Oxford University Press. Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (1998). Posttraumatic growth: Future directions. In R. G. Tedeschi, C. L. Park, & L. G. Calhoun (Eds.), Posttraumatic growth: Positive changes in the aftermath of crisis (pp. 215-238). Lawrence Erlbaum. Callahan, J. (2009). Emergency intervention and crisis intervention. In P.M. Kleespies (Ed.), Behavioral emergencies: An evidencebased resource for evaluating and managing risk of suicide, violence, and victimization (pp.13-32). American Psychological Association. Corrado, M. (2020). Stress, Crisis & Trauma: Supporting Individuals in Distress. Campaign for Trauma-Informed Policy and Practice. https://www.ctipp.org/post/stress-crisis-and-traumasupporting-individuals-in-distress Hamblen, J.L., Gibson, L.E., Mueser, K.T., & Norris, F.H. (2006). Cognitive behavior therapy for prolonged disaster distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1043-52. Page 160 of 165 Harvard Medical School (2020, July 6). Understanding the stress response. Harvard Health Publishing. https://www.health.harvard.edu/staying-healthy/understanding-thestress-response Kleespies, P.M. (2009). Behavioral emergencies: An evidencebased resources for evaluating and managing risk of suicide, violence, and victimization. American Psychological Association. Loftus, E.F. (2019). Eyewitness testimony. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 498-503. New International Bible. (2011). The NIV Bible. https://www.thenivbible.com (Original work published 1978) Pargament, K.I. & Cummings, J. (2010). Anchored by faith: Religion as a resilience factor. In J.W. Reich, A.J. Zautra, & J.S. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of adult resilience (pp.193-210). Guilford Press. Taylor, L.K. & Weems, C.F. (2011). Cognitive-behavior therapy for disaster-exposed youth with posttraumatic-stress: Results from a multiple-baseline examination. Behavior Therapy, 42, 349-63. *Contributed by Kenyon Knapp. © Kendall Hunt Publishing Company. WHY IS THIS STILL IMPORTANT? Considering how complex our cognitive processes are and how little insight we actually have into their underlying mechanisms, it makes sense to consider the nature and structure of cognition as well as the ethical and larger societal implications of understanding and using information. Just consider the select examples above and how easy it is to influence thinking. With the explosion of information sharing across technology platforms, it is even more important to consider what we think, what conclusions we reach about our thinking, and whether those conclusions are ethical, moral, and right. Biblical Integration Page 161 of 165 The language of sin, righteousness, condemnation, and more provide Biblical nuance to other moral concepts like wrong, justification, blame, etc. Those who believe in the Bible will always have their feet in two different worlds simultaneously. Christians must navigate the language of morality shared by broader society, even when they disagree with society’s values, in part so that they can bring their own values to bear on their relationship with their community and the ways they must be different from it. One vivid way this occurs is in the fact that Christians must have a different motivation for acting rightly than many others in their society. While some pursue the right action to make themselves righteous and justified or to retain their status as a “good person,” Christians know they cannot rely on these motivations. The Bible teaches that none are righteous by their own deeds. Right action is the result of a motivation of justice for its own sake and out of gratitude for the sacrifice Jesus made. We do right in the process of our sanctification, not our justification. Another vivid way is in the demands Christ gives to his followers. Some actions that society views as “supererogatory” (above and beyond the call of duty) are required by those who follow Jesus. For example, the Sermon on the Mount moves from a focus on outward actions to inward desires, intent, and character. The Bible illustrates the need for Christians to do more for their neighbors than non-believing members of society might demand. Luke 10:25-37 25 And behold, a lawyer stood up to put him to the test, saying, “Teacher, what shall I do to inherit eternal life?” 26 He said to him, “What is written in the Law? How do you read it?” 27 And he answered, “You shall love the Lord your God with all your heart and with all your soul and with all your strength and with all your Page 162 of 165 mind, and your neighbor as yourself.” 28 And he said to him, “You have answered correctly; do this, and you will live.” 29 But he, desiring to justify himself, said to Jesus, “And who is my neighbor?” 30 Jesus replied, “A man was going down from Jerusalem to Jericho, and he fell among robbers, who stripped him and beat him and departed, leaving him half dead. 31 Now by chance a priest was going down that road, and when he saw him he passed by on the other side. 32 So likewise a Levite, when he came to the place and saw him, passed by on the other side. 33 But a Samaritan, as he journeyed, came to where he was, and when he saw him, he had compassion. 34 He went to him and bound up his wounds, pouring on oil and wine. Then he set him on his own animal and brought him to an inn and took care of him. 35 And the next day he took out two denarii and gave them to the innkeeper, saying, ‘Take care of him, and whatever more you spend, I will repay you when I come back.’ 36 Which of these three, do you think, proved to be a neighbor to the man who fell among the robbers?” 37 He said, “The one who showed him mercy.” And Jesus said to him, “You go, and do likewise.” What does Jesus demand in this passage? Would someone who does not believe in the truth of the Bible require what Jesus requires here? Most likely not. A common understanding of our duties does not often include going out of our way or enduring great inconvenience to save someone that we do not know, especially if we have done nothing to place that person in danger. The legal demands of a country like the United States are even less onerous. In most states, you are not required to move your foot even an inch to nudge a life preserver off the edge of a pier to save a drowning person below. The message in Luke is very different and radical. Christians are expected to do more and for different reasons. Christ does not offer eternal justification by works of moral good. He offers himself as the justification and demands our whole person. Page 163 of 165 Brief Summary and Self-Assessment Ethics is the study of right and wrong, and it involves examining questions about morality and determining ethical truths. There are two contrasting views regarding ethical answers. The first view suggests that ethical answers are relative or subjective, influenced by culture, upbringing, and personal feelings. The second view posits that ethical answers are objective and independent of individual beliefs or cultural practices. While this book assumes that ethical answers are objectively true or false, finding these answers is not easy. It draws a parallel with science, which also seeks objective truths but requires extensive investigation and discovery. Similarly, ethics requires diligent inquiry and exploration to uncover what is ethically true. Psychology plays a crucial role in ethics because many modern atrocities have been facilitated by advancements in scientific tools. Psychology, like other sciences, has a history of unethical research practices, such as electroshock therapy, inhumane experiments, and the exploitation of vulnerable populations. The behavior of individual researchers can impact public trust in the field, and psychology has faced challenges due to a replication crisis and negative perceptions regarding its reliability. Ethical dilemmas are often discussed in ethics courses, with thought experiments exploring fringe issues. However, it is important not to overlook the ethical implications of everyday situations encountered in fields like counseling. Professionals must be prepared to handle ethical challenges, such as responding to patients expressing desires for self-harm or managing the number of clients seen in a week. The study of ethics also involves examining our own character and weaknesses. People tend to have an inflated perception of their moral virtues, but evidence suggests that individuals often act Page 164 of 165 according to their character. Acknowledging our weaknesses and striving for moral honesty is essential. Page 165 of 165