Ethnolinguistic identity theory: a social psychological approach to language maintenance HOWARD GILES and PATRICIA JOHNSON The study of language maintenance and erosion has traditionally been approached from a sociological perspective, and this is understandable given that data are invariably collected at a macrolevel in terms of group tendencies. At the same time, the people who constitute the collectivities examined have of necessity to make up their own minds regarding whether to maintain their ethnic tongue or let it erode. In other words, given that personal decisions are being made and individual strategies enacted (albethey highly social), we feel that language-maintenance theory would be enriched by a social psychological input. As such, and in the context of language maintenance being an intergroup phenomenon to the extent that it is being fostered side by side or in conflict with another group's language, cognitive processes relating to social categorization, identity, comparison, attitude formation, attribution, and second-language acquisition (among many others) have an important part to play even at the macro-level. To this end, we argue that 'ethnolinguistic identity theory' can provide a valuable new direction for furthering our understanding of the variables and mechanisms involved in the maintenance of an ethnic language in different social settings. This theory was originally formulated to address the issue of who in an ethnic group uses what language strategy, when, and why, in interethnic encounters. More specifically, we were concerned with explaining why it was that in certain situations some members of a group accentuate their ethnolinguistic characteristics (be it by dialect, language, or whatever) when conversing with outgroup speakers, while others converge toward them by attenuating their linguistic distinctiveness. Now, the former divergent act can be considered a special case of language maintenance (short-term) at the micro-level. Indeed, this type of face-to-face strategy may arguably be an instance of language maintenance par excellence in the sense that when an outgroup language is the societal norm, ethnolinguistic differentiation can invoke considerable social sanctions as a consequence. Moreover, in some situations, little cognitive effort may be involved in maintaining one's own dialect or language within the private and 'safe' confines of the home and 0165-2516/87/0068-0069 $2.00 © Mouton de Gruyter, Amsterdam Int'l. J. Soc. Lang. 68 (1987), pp. 69-99 Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 70 H. Giles and P. Johnson other cohesive ingroup settings, but it is another matter as to whether this strategy will generalize to the immediacy of intergroup encounters in the wider society. In short, we believe that ethnolinguistic identity theory has value in fostering our understanding of when individuals maintain or sacrifice their ethnolinguistic identity in the short term of social interaction as well as in the longer term of the group retaining or relinquishing their language as a communicative code. This paper begins by outlining our approach to ethnolinguistics and in particular the psychological climate that is proposed to underlie language •maintenance in interethnic exchanges. Ethnolinguistic identity theory is then tested by means of a questionnaire methodology in Wales, and a number of its basic tenets are confirmed. However, the data demonstrated some unexpected complexities between variables not accounted for previously, and therefore the theory is reformulated as a consequence. In addition, it is revised in order to meet the demands of situational factors and extended so as to embrace recent developments in the fields of both second-language learning and ethnic language attitudes. Most importantly, however, in the context of this issue, we again argue that there is a conspicuous absence of a social psychological input to language-maintenance studies and theory. Therefore, we offer a revised and extended ethnolinguistic identity theory as a new direction to this end. Finally, priorities for future research are proffered and the notion of alternative conceptualizations of social identity discussed. Ethnolinguistic identity theory There has been a burgeoning of fruitful cross-disciplinary research examining the relationship between language and ethnicity (for example, Edwards 1985). Nevertheless, the multiplicity of interethnic situations, attitudes to the languages of ingroups and outgroups, and the ethnolinguistic strategies people consequently adopt have retarded the development of any overall theory which relates one situation to another and permits common explanation of the many aspects which are discernible. We have been developing an approach which seeks to clarify the empirical and theoretical confusion by identifying common social psychological processes underlying diverse speech strategies. This approach— 'ethnolinguistic identity theory' (see Beebe and Giles 1984; Hildebrandt and Giles 1983) —has the advantage of taking into account sociostructural influences on ethnic groups in contact and how they are perceived by group members. Ethnolinguistic identity theory draws heavily on the influential theory of intergroup behavior by Tajfel and Turner (1979) called, 'social identity theory', which is outlined as follows. We categorize the social world and, hence, perceive ourselves as members of various groups. Such knowledge of Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 11 ourselves as group members is defined as our social identity, and it may be positive or negative according to how our ingroups fare in social comparison with relevant outgroups. It is argued that we strive to achieve a positive identity by seeking dimensions which afford favorable comparison with outgroups; in other words, we strive to achieve positive 'psychological distinctiveness' (see Tajfel 1982). Language comes into the picture when a group regards its own language or speech variety as a dimension of comparison with outgroups. Hence, people who define an encounter with a member of another ethnic group as an intergroup one and value their language as a core aspect of its identity will wish to assume a positive identity by means of adopting various strategies of 'psycholinguistic distinctiveness' such as switching to ingroup language, accentuating ethnic dialect and slang, etc. (see for example, Bourhis et al. 1979; Taylor and Royer 1980). Indeed, it has been shown that taking the opportunity to adopt strategies of differentiation in an intergroup situation allows one a more positive self-concept as a consequence (Oakes and Turner 1980). Sometimes minority ethnic groups which possess a supposedly negative social identity vis-ä-vis the dominant group(s) do not seem to adopt language accentuation strategies. Tajfel and Turner (1979) suggest that social competition (which by implication would embrace ethnolinguistic differentiations) comes about when people (a) strongly identify with their social group, and (b) make insecure social comparisons between the positions of their group socially and that of the outgroup. Such 'insecure social comparisons' would be manifest in an awareness of cognitive alternatives to the extent that speakers believed their group's (low) status was unfair and potentially changeable. Ethnolinguistic identity theory goes on to suggest that although a person's membership groups are each part of his or her social identity, they will not all be equally salient at any one time. Moreover, it is only when groups' linguistic characteristics (oftentimes ethnic ones) are salient that we should expect the accentuation of ingroup speech markers as intergroup strategies. Three variables are suggested as relevant here (perceived vitality, perceived group boundaries, multiple group memberships) in that their nature can increase or decrease the level of a person's sense of ethnic belongingness * (Giles and Johnson 1981). Let us deal briefly with the three constructs in turn. Giles et al. (1977) suggested that ethnic groups could be compared in terms of 'ethnolinguistic vitality' toward which three main groups of factors contribute: status factors (such as economic, political, and linguistic prestige); demographic factors (such as absolute numbers, birthrate, geographical concentration); and institutional support (such as recognition of the group and its language in the media, education, government). They proposed that groups with high vitality are the ones most likely to thrive and remain distinct Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 72 H. Giles and P. Johnson (see Allard and Landry 1985; Saint-Blancat 1985). However, the actions of individual group members are likely to be governed less by the actual vitality of their groups than by their perceptions of it (Bourhis and Sachdev 1984; Giles et al. 1985; Young et al. 1986). Cues and Johnson (1981)suggested that a high level of perceived vitality increases the salience of group identity for members and thence intensifies their inclination to accentuate group speech markers in order to establish favorable psycholinguistic distinctiveness. Sociological and anthropological scholars have emphasized the importance of social 'boundaries' (Banto 1983). It is maintained that group members try to maintain a high level of perceived boundary hardness (Giles 1979; Huffines 1986) and this, when successful, clarifies ethnic categorization and norms for conducting intergroup encounters, thereby increasing the salience of group membership. Finally, those who see themselves as belonging to numerous different, overlapping groups should possess a more diffuse social identity than persons who view themselves as members of only one or two groups. In other words, ethnolinguistic attachment should be stronger for those who can identify with few other social categories. Moreover, the favorableness of intergroup comparisons offered by nonethnolinguistic membership groups will affect ethnolinguistic dimensions of identity, as will their status and involvement level within each membership group. The five major propositions relating to ethnolinguistic identity theory are presented below. They constitute the hypotheses which frame the empirical study conducted in Wales (see Williams 1978) which we report shortly as a first step toward testing this theory. The dependent variable of ethnolinguistic differentiation here was the extent to which Welsh-English bilinguals selfreported maintaining their use of the Welsh language in face-to-face encounters with monolingual English speakers. The theory claims that people will define an encounter in interethnic terms and strive for a positive ethnic identity by accentuating their ingroup speech style to the extent (we hypothesize) that they 1. identify themselves subjectively and strongly as members of a group which considers language an important symbol of their identity; 2. make insecure social comparisons with the outgroup (for example, regard their group's status as potentially changeable); 3. perceive their own group's vitality to be high; 4. perceive their ingroup boundaries to be hard and closed; and 5. identify strongly with few other social categories. It is our contention that ethnic minority groups experiencing such a psychological climate would not only be very likely to maintain their ethnolinguistic identity and diverge from an outgroup speaker, but would also be less disposed to acquiring nativelike proficiency in the dominant group's tongue (Giles and Byrne 1982) and be extremely keen to maintain use of the Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 73 ethnic tongue within the family context and beyond and expend energies in this direction. The converse set of conditions should be conducive to interpersonal convergence toward an outgroup speaker, promote a strong motivation for acquiring nativelike proficiency in the dominant group tongue, and ultimately foster ingroup language erosion or outgroup assimilation in the family context and within their close social networks. In other words, we argue that ethnolinguistic identity theory is relevant to short-term interactions at the individual level as well as long-term group tendencies of language maintenance and erosion. The study to be presented below is explicitly concerned with testing the theory in the former regard but has obvious implicit potency for the latter level of analysis too. Method Subjects Thirty-four bilingual adolescents served as subjects (Ss) for this study. They were 17 years of age, male, and used the self-referent 'Welsh' (rather than British or English), and their first tongue was Welsh. They attended a bilingual comprehensive school in Dyfed, a county characterized by a high proportion of Welsh bilingual speakers (Williams 1979). Materials The questionnaire consisted of five sections. The first examined Ss' identification with the ethnic group and consisted of three ΙΟ-cm lines marked at either end, 'extremely important to me' and 'not at all important to me'. Ss were asked to rate the importance to them of their Welsh identity, the Welsh language, and the learning of Welsh by their future offspring. As these items were found to intercorrelate very highly subsequently, they were summed in order to provide an overall identification score (ID). The second section examined Ss' perceptions of the degree of cognitive alternatives to the current position of Wales with respect to Britain. Ss were asked to rate on ΙΟ-cm, lines (marked appropriately at either end) the extent to which they felt that Wales would remain a small part of Britain (item C), and the extent to which they saw clear acceptable alternatives to Wales's historical position as a minor part of Britain (CC). Finally, Ss were asked the importance to them of the Welsh Nationalist Party (NAT). The third section consisted of the 22-item subjective vitality questionnaire Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 74 H. Giles and P. Johnson (VTT) compiled by Bourhis et al. (1981), which was adapted appropriately for use in a Welsh/English context. The fourth section was concerned with Ss' multiple group memberships and boundaries^ two questions each respectively. Ss were asked again on ΙΟ-cm lines (appropriately marked at either end) how many other groups they identified with which were as important to them as was their Welshness (item NO) — and to name examples of such groups — to what extent they saw themselves as firstly and most importantly in terms of their Welshness (FIR), the extent to which they agreed that Welsh speakers were the only true Welsh people (WSP), and the extent to which they agreed that Welsh and English people are clearly different with one never being able to become the other (BD). The fifth section was concerned with use of the Welsh language and attitudes to it (see Bourhis 1983a), and this provided us with our measure of linguistic differentiation from the English. These self-reported rating questions were When served by shop assistants who speak to you in English, how often do you reply in Welsh? When you are in shops, cinemas, pubs, clubs, etc., how often do you use Welsh when you speak to strangers? When you are in shops, cinemas, pubs, clubs, etc., how often do you speak to strangers in English? How often do you change from Welsh to English with someone who does not speak Welsh very well? How much do you enjoy switching to English in public places, like shops and cinemas, when you are not understood in Welsh? Finally, the following rating scales (agree-disagree) tapped bilinguals' attitudes to social norms prescribing the use of English: Welsh people should not speak Welsh in the presence of English people. If someone speaks to me in English, I should answer them in English. I feel completely at ease speaking Welsh in public places or wherever I want to. W'elsh people should not speak Welsh just for the sake of English people. The nine scales were summated for each subject in order to provide a composite differentiation score (DIFF). Ss were of course invited to append their comments throughout the questionnaire. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 75 Results Regarding the above measures, the sample as a whole can be characterized as follows (the figures in brackets subsequent to the means indicate the minimum and maximum scores). Ss identified very strongly with their Welsh group membership (ID X = 28.5 [10-30], perceived a great stability in the status quo position of Wales vis-ä-vis England (C and CC 3( = 7.4 and 7.0 respectively [1-10] and considered the Welsh Nationalist Party important to them (NAT X = 7.0 [1-10]). They also indicated that they saw themselves strongly as first and foremost 'Welsh', saw Welsh speakers as the only 'true Welsh', and perceived clear hard boundaries between the Welsh and English (FIR, WSP, and BD Xs=7.8, 7.3, 7.0 respectively [1-10]). While they responded by claiming that there were a few other groups with which they identified (NO X = 5.9 [1-10]), nearly all of those so generated related to Welsh youth groups, clubs, and choirs, with a number of Ss indicating that Welsh was salient to these groups by appending, 'in Welsh'; in other words, these other groups were in actuality highly ethnic in character. Finally, Ss perceived the vitality of their group to lie just below the midpoint of the scale (VIT X = 227 [100-400]) which is not surprising for a 'subordinate' ethnic group, but evinced a relatively high mean for the measure of likelihood of linguistic differentiation (DIFF X = 61.6 [9-90]). The first major analysis conducted was a multiple regression performed with all the variables on the DIFF measure as shown in Table 1. In other words, we wished to determine which variables were the best predictors of short-term language maintenance. The variables C and ID were entered in steps 1 and 2 respectively (ps < 0.001), but no further variables were involved Table 1. F values for the variables at each stage of stepping in the multiple regression on likelihood of linguistic differentiation (DIFF) Variable C ID CC VIT FIR WSP BD NO NAT Step no. 0 F to F to remove enter 21.20 14.60 0.93 2.18 2.62 11.40 5.82 0.09 9.70 Step no. 1 F to F to remove enter Step no. 2 Fto Fto remove enter 21.20 12.09 6.41 6.41 0.73 3.44 1.58 1.21 0.83 0.66 0.97 \ 1.85 1.63 0.99 0.98 0.76 0.14 0.02 Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 76 H. Giles and P. Johnson as the F levels were insufficient for further stepping. It can be seen from Table 1 that the variable accounting for the greatest amount of variance was C (that is, attitudes to the statement, 'Wales will always remain a small part of Britain') reflecting the perception of instability in the present intergroup hierarchy. When this variable was entered, the F values for WSP, NAT, and BD were noticeably reduced, indicating associations between these four items. The ID variable accounted for the second greatest amount of variance. The apparent associations between a number of the variables suggested that it would be useful to examine these further via a correlational analysis, and the results are shown in Table 2. As expected from the previous analysis, C, WSP, NAT and BD formed a positively correlated cluster. Although FIR and NO were not correlated with DIFF, they were with WSP, while FIR correlated with BD. ID was positively correlated with C, NAT, and DIFF. It can be seen from Table 2 that VIT was not correlated with any of the other variables. It seems possible, despite our original theoretical inclinations, that VIT might be orthogonal to ID, and that dividing Ss post hoc on the basis of their ID and VIT scores might yield interesting results for our language-maintenance measure, DIFF; accordingly, Ss were divided into these four quadrants. High identifiers were those who scored the maximum of 30 on the ID measure and low identifiers were those who scored less than this maximum. Examination of the data suggested that taking the median was the most appropriate dividing point for determining high and low vitality groups, t tests on both the ID and VIT scores between the quadrants verified that the four groups were statistically different. A 2 χ 2 ANOVA on these independent variables with DIFF as the dependent variable was then performed. While a main effect emerged for ID in the sense that high identifiers were more likely to differentiate than low scores (Fl.29 = 20.64, p. < 0.000Ϊ), a significant interaction qualified this pattern (F = 5.13; p < 0.03) with Newman-Keuls analyses being utilized to explore this effect further. For Ss who identified less strongly with the Welsh group, the perception of high vitality resulted in a higher DIFF score than those who perceived the group to have low vitality (Xs = 54.75 and 40.5 respectively, ρ < 0.01). However, for those who identified more strongly with their ethnic group, the perception of low vitality was associated with higher DIFF scores than for those who perceived vitality to be high (Xs = 64.75 and 70.38 respectively, ρ < 0.06). The results support the notion that VIT and ID are orthogonal here. Discussion The results support hypotheses 1 and 2 in that anticipated language maintenance was a function of ethnic identification and making insecure social Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 77 * * * * * * * * ** * * * o r-f-ioo^-ONONr^vo vo c o r - o o i o r ^ i - H O N » o »O V O * - H " * O e S « O C O C S vo VO *-H o CO τ!· es ^ es VO VO CS Q PQ es es ^· O «-H O CO CS ON VO 00 fH Ο O O CN oo «o co es Ό Ο VO Ο CO "* Ο »Ο VO l— O Tt CO r*· co oo vo oo v ** ** ON PH r- ON α es CO ^ T-H v—l g E O es g % vo »o es ** ** ** «ο ιο CO ON rι—ι VO *r> oo O ON co es es co es co o co oo T-H q V o. * * * oT q CO ON IO es V P, ON O •g B s CO V PH £ Β Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 78 H. Giles and P. Johnson comparisons. It may seem paradoxical at first sight that the latter variable accounted for more variance than the former, given that if individuals did not identify with the group presumably they would not care whether or not its low-status position was unstable or not. However, it must be recalled that all Ss identified very highly with the Welsh group and the fact that strength of identification accounted for any of the variance in this instance indicates the potency of its effect. The associations between a number of the variables suggested by the multiple-regression results were supported by examination of the correlations between all the variables. The 'perceived instability' variable, perception of the group boundaries as hard and closed, agreement that 'Welsh speakers are the only true Welsh', and allegiance to the Welsh Nationalist Party formed a positively correlated cluster and were also positively correlated with the likelihood of linguistic differentiation. These results offer some support for the association between hard and closed perceived boundaries and the likelihood of linguistic differentiation (that is, hypothesis 4). The association between C and NAT is unsurprising in view of the aims of this political party. Yet the lack of association between the latter variable and the perception of 'clear and acceptable alternatives to Wales's current position' suggests that these alternatives may include options other than political dissociation from England. The lack of any significant relations in this study between CC and DIFF is surprising in view of the tenets of social identity theory (Tajfel 1978). However, as indicated at the outset of this paper, the perception of instability and unfairness in the status hierarchy are considered to be precursors to the perception of cognitive alternatives, and these in turn are likely to precede awareness of concrete alternatives. It is possible that the variable relating to 'clear and acceptable alternatives' was tapping awareness of the latter rather than of more cognitive alternatives, and this may account for the lack of significant effects for this variable in the study. It is unfortunate that no measure was included herein of the Ss' perceptions of unfairness. Nevertheless, a number of Ss' comments (particularly of those who identified more strongly with the ingroup) indicated that they did perceive the low status of their group to be illegitimate. For example, over half of the stronger identifiers commented 'too much/not enough' alongside the vitality items referring to the power and wealth of the English and Welsh groups respectively. Hypothesis 5 relating to multiple group memberships was not supported in that neither FIR nor NO was related to DIFF. Nevertheless, these variables did yield some interesting effects. A weak positive correlation between them indicated that the more groups Ss identified with, the more they saw themselves firstly and most importantly as Welsh. This intuitive contradiction is dispelled once one recalls the nature of these other social groups, that is, they Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 79 were highly Welsh in cultural focus. The more Ss saw themselves as Welsh, the more groups they were likely to identify with which supported their allegiance with the Welsh group and language. This is supported in Ss' own comments: for instance, one informant wrote, 'Welshness is not a cult or companion but a way of life, a medium for all spheres of life'. Here then identification with a greater number of associated categories was related to seeing themselves firstly and most importantly as Welsh and with seeing Welsh speakers as the only 'true Welsh'. The latter two variables (see Table 2) were also associated with the perception of hard and closed boundaries, which in turn was associated with linguistic differentiation; causal pathway analyses could be fruitful in future research (see Gardner 1985). The above interpretation is supported by Milroy's (1982) findings of positive correlations between the level of social integration (in terms of multiple ties to other group members) and use of the nonstandard language variety of the group. She discussed this correlation 'by referring to the widely accepted anthropological view that a close-knit network has the capacity to function as a norm enforcement mechanism and insulate its members from the pressures of mainstream values - including linguistic values' (1982: 212). Other studies have supported the association between close-knit localized network structures and adherence to low-status language variety (Gal 1979). In addition, Wong-Reiger and Taylor (1981) suggest that one way by which individuals resolve the apparent paradox of achieving a unitary self-identity while being members of many different groups is through identifying with those which share the same values (for example, with respect to the use of a nonstandard language variety). The lack of any significant correlations between perceived vitality and any of the other variables and, in particular, strength of identification suggested that VIT was orthogonal to ID. This was found to be the case, with high VIT associated with greater DIFF for those who identified less strongly with the ethnic group, while the reverse was true for those who identified strongly with the group. Thus, hypothesis 3 was not supported but may usefully be considered in terms of the perceptions of group norms and the group's potential for applying sanctions against those who do not comply with these. Such a perspective is particularly relevant here where the DIFF measure included not only self-reports of language maintenance but also attitudes to social norms relating to the use of Welsh. Where language is a salient dimension of ethnic group identity, as is the case for our Welsh bilingual Ss, group norms are likely to prescribe use of the ingroup language. Furthermore, those who perceive the group as having high VIT are more likely to perceive the group norms in these terms, that is, as prescribing language loyalty. In contrast, those who identify less strongly with the ethnic group and perceive its vitality to be low may perceive the Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 80 H. Giles and P. Johnson group norm differently, as they may see the language as dying, if not dead. In addition, Bourhis (1979) suggests that the higher the vitality of a group, the more resources it has available for rewarding or sanctioning those who do or do not comply with group norms. Sanctioning may include the use of abuse, traitor labels, and negative evaluations, while rewards may include positive support and evaluations. Accordingly, it is suggested that the more vitality an individual perceives the group to have, the more resources it will be perceived to have for applying rewards/sanctions for (non)compliance with group norms relating to use of the ethnic language. On the other hand, those who perceive the ethnic group to have low vitality may not only perceive the group norms differently, but may also consider the group to have few resources available for enforcing its norms. These results may be considered in the light of Turner et al.'s (1984) work relating to the concepts of cognitive dissonance and commitment principles (Harvey and Smith 1977). They suggest that where there is voluntary group membership, group 'failure' (for instance, in our situation, as reflected in low vitality or low differentiation from the outgroup) will increase group cohesion (see Taylor et al. 1983). Where there is involuntary group membership, group cohesion will decrease when there is a perception of group 'failure' and increase where there is success. Furthermore, Giles (1979) suggested that in these situations, ethnic speech markers can be a more overt and flexible expression of social differentiation than their nonlinguistic counterparts (see Huffines 1986). Since high identifiers may be considered those with a greater degree of voluntary group membership and low vitality may be construed as group failure, then this perspective would lead one to expect greater group cohesion and linguistic differentiation among high ID and low VIT individuals. Conversely, low identifiers may be considered those with a greater degree of involuntary group membership and, correspondingly, would be expected to feel more group cohesion and evince language maintenance when they construe the group's vitality to be high rather than low. A number of Ss' comments supported the interpretation that in some cases low VIT may spur efforts to maintain the ethnic language and trigger actions to bolster its vitality. For example, one informant (high ID/low VIT) commented, Ί think that the Welsh language has a future if we all decide that the language is going to live and do everything possible to ensure that it has a future.' This type of attitude was reflected in the comment of another respondent: 'This [the survival of the Welsh group and language] will be up to us and the strength of activist movements, because the Welsh people and the Welsh language are one and once the latter disappears the former will also be lost.' These remarks also support Sweeting's (1982) interpretations of her study with Gaelic Scots learners. She found that Scots learning Gaelic perceived the vitality of Gaelic (as measured by the subjective vitality ques- Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistie identity theory 81 tionnaire) to be lower than did either other Scots or English controls (residing in Scotland) who were not learning this language. Sweeting suggests, in the light of her Ss' comments, that this perception (while possibly reflecting greater 'factual' knowledge about the language) may increase rather than decrease desires to learn and support the language as the Gaelic learners are striving to revive a 'dying language'. She also points out that this perception of the low VIT of the language may also represent a positive strategy for enhancement of self-identity in the contribution the learners feel they are making to revive Gaelic. A similar analysis can be applied at the societal level when one considers the resolve of Francophones in Quebec to ensure the vitality of the French language when they perceived it to be seriously threatened (Laporte 1983). This is underlined when one considers that for this group the Charter of the French Language remains the most popular legislative act promulgated by the Quebec government. It is still expected, however, that there exists a 'point of no return' at which very weak or threatened vitality virtually eliminates the likelihood that the language group will survive and act as a collective entity. Such a situation seems most likely when the group boundaries are soft, allowing those who identify weakly with the group to leave it. In this case, the 'point of no return' would be reached (albeit accepting future forces for 'language resurrection'; see Eastman 1979) when the number of highly committed (and Voluntary') group members decreased to the extent that they 'gave up'. Yet before this stage, the perception of low group vitality may stimulate efforts to support the language group among its members. A further analysis The findings above suggested that ethnic identification and perceived vitality were determining the self-reported strength of language maintenance independently. This was interpreted in terms of the group's perceived 'success/ failure', potential for applying rewards/sanctions for compliance/noncompliance, and the degree to which ethnic group membership was perceived to be voluntary. The notion of voluntary/involuntary membership and hence the perception that one can or cannot move easily between group memberships relates very well with the perception of ethnic boundaries as hard and closed. This interpretation, together with the lack of significant correlations between ID, BD, and VIT suggests that it might be worth exploring the possibility that identification, boundaries, and vitality interact in their influence on language maintenance (DIFF). To this end, further post hoc analyses were performed on the data. Unfortunately, there were insuffficient Ss for a 2x2x2 ANOVA (with ID, BD, and VIT as independent variables) and DIFF Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 82 H. Giles and P. Johnson as the dependent variable to make this ideal analysis viable. Instead, three separate 2x2 ANOVAs were performed with each of the three variables as the dependent variable in turn in the hope that they might cast some light on the influences of these variables on each other. The first of these analyses involved ID and BD as independent variables and VIT as the dependent variable. There emerged a significant main effect for ID, with stronger identifiers perceiving vitality to be lower than weaker identifiers (F1.30 = 4.04, ρ < 0.05), with an interactive tendency between them (F = 3.41, ρ < 0.07). Newman-Keuls analysis of the latter showed that for weak identifiers, vitality was perceived to be higher by those who perceived boundaries to be hard and closed (X = 254) rather than soft and open (X = 232; ρ < 0.05). For those who identified more strongly with the ethnic group, vitality is perceived to be higher by those who perceived boundaries to be soft and open (X = 230) rather than hard and closed (X = 214; ρ < 0.05). The second analysis involved ID and VIT as independent variables with BD as the dependent variable. The only effect to emerge was a main one for ID, with those who identified more strongly perceiving boundaries to be harder and more closed (X = 7.75) than those who identified less strongly with their ethnic group (X = 5.38; F = 6.49, ρ < 0.02). The final analysis examined BD and VIT with ID as the dependent variable but no significant effects emerged. The results of these ANOVAs can only be taken as tentative for a couple of reasons. First, the 'independent' variables have some degree of interdependence. Second, the small range of scores for ID are loaded at one end of the scale, with all Ss identifying relatively strongly with the ingroup; that is, they are asymmetrically distributed, and in addition the small range of scores suggests that significant effects of the BD and VIT variables are less likely to emerge. Nevertheless, with those cautionary notes in mind, the results do suggest that the three variables may interact and are mutually influential. In sum, they suggest that those who identify more strongly with the ethnic group may perceive group boundaries to be harder and more closed than those who identify less strongly. Furthermore, strong identifiers may perceive vitality to be lower than those who identify less strongly with the group, particularly when they perceive group boundaries to be hard and closed rather than soft and open. In contrast, weaker identifiers may perceive vitality to be higher, particularly when they perceive boundaries to be hard and closed rather than soft and open. These reanalyses may be interpreted once again in terms of commitment principles. In this vein, strong identifiers are likely to be very committed to the group when they perceive boundaries to be hard and closed, and to be even more so when they perceive their group's vitality to be low. As we saw Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 83 earlier, such a cognitive constellation is likely to be the breeding ground for language maintenance. It is suggested that they are likely to show ingroup solidarity and a low tolerance for what we shall call societal norms imposed by the dominant group, such as use of English in their presence (see Giles and Johnson 1986). Those who identify strongly with the ethnic group but perceive boundaries to be softer and more open may be more voluntarily committed to the group and perceive it to have higher vitality (that is, group 'success'). If also seems likely that their strong identification with the ingroup may lead to a hardening of group boundaries when interethnic discrimination occurs. This will facilitate the categorization of in- and outgroup members, allowing a more secure, clearer sense of identity and more firmly established group norms. For weaker identifiers, the perception of hard and closed boundaries may increase commitment to the group, but weaker identification suggests that this commitment is more involuntary than for those who strongly identify. They may therefore be more likely to perceive the group's vitality as higher, reflecting group 'success', and, as we indicated earlier, such individuals are more likely to differentiate linguistically from the outgroup than comparable others who perceive ingroup vitality to be low. This may be related to the group's perceived potential for applying sanctions against those who do not conform to ingroup norms, for example, regarding Welsh language use. It is suggested therefore that these individuals are likely to evidence lower solidarity with the group but relatively high conformity to group norms. In contrast, those who identify less strongly and perceive group boundaries to be softer and more open are likely to be less committed to the group and also to perceive the possibility of leaving it (Tajfel 1974). Analyses above suggest that these individuals are likely to perceive ingroup vitality as low as well and this perceived 'failure' may further weaken commitment to the group. The group may also be viewed as having less potential for applying real sanctions or rewards for (non)compliance with its norms, and these individuals are less likely to differentiate linguistically. It is suggested that they are likely to show less ingroup solidarity and greater tolerance for societal norms than the other subgroups above. Reformulations and extensions of ethnolinguistic identity theory Based on the above, albeit modest, data and interpretations, not to mention their consequential refutation of our hypotheses, the following are considered to improve upon the original propositions of ethnolinguistic identity theory. More specifically, the first three of these are considered as articulating the predispositional conditions under which individual ethnic group members Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 84 H. Giles and P. Johnson will be more or less likely to define many interethnic encounters in terms of ethnicity and to be resolute in their language-maintenance strategies. In this way, they highlight the heterogeneity of the ethnic group as well as indicating implicitly the dynamic nature of the processes contributing to this. The first set of propositions relates to those individuals who are predisposed to act in terms of ethnic solidarity: I. Members of a subordinate ethnic group which considers language to be an important dimension of its identity are likely to be predisposed to acting in terms of ethnic solidarity rather than conformity to societal norms in many interethnic situations, to define many interethnic situations in terms of their ethnicity, and to maintain their distinctive language features, when they (a) identify very strongly with their ethnic group; (b) make insecure social comparisons with the outgroup (for example, are aware of cognitive alternatives to their own group's status position); (c) identify with few other nonethnic social categories whose norms and values do not overlap with those of the ethnic group, perceive the social identities deriving from these as relatively inadequate, and perceive their status within the ethnic group to be higher than their intragroup status within their other social category memberships; and (di) perceive their ingroup boundaries to be hard and closed; while they (βχ) perceive their ethnic group to have low vitality; or (d2) perceive their ingroup boundaries to be soft and open; while they (e2 ) perceive their ethnic group to have high vitality. The second set of propositions relates to members of the ethnic group who identify less strongly with it but perceive the ethnic boundaries to be hard and closed; these individuals may be considered to be more involuntarily committed to the group. The perception of high vitality may reflect group 'success' and increase commitment to it, but it may also be seen as indicating the group's high potential for applying sanctions/rewards for (non)compliance to group language norms. They are likely to define fewer interethnic situations in terms of their ethnic identity than those depicted in the previous proposition and have the propensity to act in terms of conformity to group norms. Therefore, II. Members of a subordinate ethnic group which considers language an important dimension of its identity are likely to be predisposed to acting in terms of conformity to ingroup rather than societal norms in many inter- Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 85 ethnic situations, but to evidence only weak or moderate ethnic solidarity, to define fewer interethnic situations in terms of their ethnic identity, and to be less likely to maintain their distinctive language features, when they (a) identify only moderately with the ethnic group; (b) make insecure social comparisons with the outgroup; (c) identify with few other nonethnic social categories whose norms and values do not overlap with those of the ethnic group, perceive the social identities deriving from these as relatively inadequate, and perceive their status within the ethnic group to be higher than their intragroup status within other social category memberships; (d) perceive their ingroup boundaries to be hard and closed; (e) perceive their ethnic group to have high vitality. The third set of propositions relates to individuals who identify less strongly with the group and perceive its boundaries to be soft and open. They may be considered voluntarily and weakly committed to the group, and the perception of low ingroup vitality may reflect group 'failure', thereby further weakening commitment to it. In addition, it may reflect the group's low potential for applying sanctions/rewards for (non)compliance with group norms. Such persons are therefore considered likely to evidence weak ethnic solidarity and will likely conform more to societal norms rather than to ingroup ones in many situations. Therefore, III. Members of a subordinate group which considers language probably not that important a dimension of its group identity are likely to be predisposed to acting in terms of conformity to societal rather than ethnic ingroup norms and to evidence weak ethnic solidarity, to define few interethnic situations in terms of their ethnic identity, and to be less likely to maintain their distinctive language features, when they (a) identify only moderately or weakly with the ethnic group; (b) make secure social comparisons with the outgroup (for example, are not aware of cognitive alternatives to their own group's status position); (c) identify with a number of other nonethnic social categories whose norms and values do not overlap with those of the ethnic group, perceive the social identities deriving from these as relatively adequate, and perceive their status within the ethnic group to be lower than their intragroup status within their other social category memberships; (d) perceive their ingroup boundaries to be soft and open; (e) perceive their ethnic group to have low vitality. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 86 H. Giles and P. Johnson We would like to go beyond the data presented above and argue that there are situational variables which are likely to override these predispositional backdrops in some circumstances, such that either many or few ethnic group members are likely to act in terms of their ethnic identity and use the ethnic ingroup language. The findings of Giles and Johnson (1986), together with the tenets of speech-accommodation theory (Beebe and Giles 1984), suggest that predispositional tendencies of individuals to define interactions and act m terms of ethnic solidarity may be influenced by (a) the desire to maintain communicational efficiency, and (b) status-stressing features of the situation (Ball et al. 1984). The latter may highlight societal norms favoring use of the dominant group's speech style and/or increase the perceived costs in using their own lower-status ethnic ingroup speech. In addition, Giles and Johnson (1986) have shown that the perceived centrality of an ethnic threat may be a salient determinant of the likelihood that ethnic group members will anticipate maintaining their ingroup speech style in interethnic encounters (see Gallois et al. 1984; Taylor et al. 1983). It can be argued that, while those who are disposed to acting in terms of ethnic solidarity are likely to perceive more ethnic threats as central to their valued ethnic identity (and Overinclude'), those who are predisposed to acting in terms of conformity to ingroup norms or to societal norms may perceive fewer ethnic threats as central to their less positively valued sense of ethnicity (and Overexclude'). However, it is also suggested that there will be some ethnic threats which will be perceived by most ethnic group members to be central to their sense of ethnicity, when most ethnic group members will therefore be likely to differentiate from the threatening interactant and maintain their ethnic language. Conversely, there are likely to be some ethnic threats which few group members will perceive to be central to their sense of ethnic identity, when few ethnic group members will therefore be likely to differentiate linguistically. A further factor influencing the likelihood that an interaction will be defined in terms of ethnicity may be the degree of overlap with the other interactant in terms of shared (nonethnic) group memberships (Giles and Johnson 1981). If two individuals from different ethnic groups identify with a number of other common groups then the ethnolinguistic differences between them may have to be more salient for them to define the interaction in interethnic terms than if they shared fewer common membership groups, particularly if the shared group memberships are salient in the interaction. The following propositions, then, are meant to provide a useful complement to propositions I-III earlier, in that they identify the centrality of a perceived ethnic threat as a salient determinant of the likelihood of interethnic definitions and linguistic differentiation. They also highlight the ways in which situational factors override the predispositional tendencies of individual Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 87 group members to act in terms of ethnic solidarity. These propositions then are the following: IV. Few subordinate ethnic group members are likely to define interethnic situations in terms of their ethnic identity and to differentiate socially by language maintenance when they (a) are not strongly committed to an attitude position on which another interactant opposes them and they perceive the issue to be peripheral to their sense of ethnic identity; (b) perceive a number of features of the situation to emphasize statusstressing rather than solidarity-stressing dimensions (for example, the other interactant is of a higher status on a number of dimensions salient to the subordinate group members); (c) are concerned to maintain communicational efficiency; (d) perceive a large degree of overlap with their interactant in terms of other shared group memberships. Under these conditions, most ethnic group members are likely to attenuate their ethnic speech markers in order to preserve their value systems as 'good members of society'. Speech convergence toward the dominant outgroup member in this context may also function to categorize such accommodators as belonging to a common group with the other interactant. Such a strategy, if occurring frequently enough, is likely in the longer term to contribute to a decrease in ethnic ingroup identification, a weakening of the ethnic boundaries, and a lowering of group vitality, and hence it facilitates the climate for ingroup language suicide to occur (Denison 1977). V. Many subordinate group members are likely to define interethnic situations in terms of their ethnic identity and to differentiate socially by language maintenance, when they (a) are strongly committed to an attitude position on which another interactant opposes them and they perceive the issue to be central to their sense of ethnic identity; (b) perceive few features of the situation to emphasize status-stressing rather than solidarity-stressing dimensions or the other interactant is perceived to have violated social norms (for example, by making the issue more personal); (c) are not concerned to maintain communicational efficiency; (d) perceive little overlap with their interactant in terms of other shared group memberships. Accentuation of ethnic speech markers, or divergence here, may function to preserve value systems and to ideologize group action with respect to the outgroup. Such a strategy, if occurring frequently enough, is obviously Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 88 H. Giles and P. Johnson going to increase the likelihood of intercultural communication breakdown (Hewstone and Giles 1986), to increase ingroup identification, to strengthen group boundaries and vitality, and to promote the psychological climate necessary for language maintenance. This would be particularly so in cases where the dominant group showed no policy signs of offering genuine motivational support for the survival of the ethnic language institutionally (see Genesee et al. 1983). In sum, propositions IV and V are considered to identify when either few or many ethnic group members are likely to maintain their ethnic speech style. They are complemented by the earlier sets of propositions (I-III), specifying which members are most likely to use the ethnic speech style, but are considered to encapsulate conditions which may override the predispositional tendencies of individual group members described by the earlier propositions. Under these conditions, the functions of the language strategies individuals are likely to adopt were suggested above, indicating why the ethnic speech style may be used. These reformulated propositions of ethnolinguistic identity theory contribute more succinctly now to an understanding of who in an ethnic group is likely to maintain use of the ethnic speech style, when, and why. Figure 1 below is a model which attempts schematically to encapsulate in summary form the variables and processes operating in ethnolinguistic identity theory which we have articulated in this paper. Implications for related theoretical frameworks Reformulated ethnolinguistic identity theory has revisional implications not only for its precursor, speech-accommodation theory (Giles 1984), but also for its satellite models in second-language acquisition (Giles and Byrne 1982) and ethnic language attitudes (Ryan et al. 1984). Space of course precludes more than ä mere outline of these implications, which, in the case of accommodation theory, will be taken up elsewhere. Suffice it to say that the propositions above specify more concretely than hitherto the social psychological conditions under which speakers in intergroup contexts will converge to outgroup others or maintain their language features in defiance of them. In particular, notions of situational threat, message content, and the interrelationships between intergroup variables (that is, between identification, perceived vitality, and boundaries) will figure prominently in the revised formulation of accommodation theory. The intergroup model of second-language acquisition (Giles and Byrne 1982) suggested that many of the propositions inherent in ethnolinguistic identity theory were a key to understanding the factors which promote and Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 89 Ethnic identification (commitment) U Perceived id boui boundaries (hardness and and cl< closedness) Ο Subjective vitality (group's perceived 'success/ failure' and potential for rewarding/sanctioning) increases identification; strengthens boundaries; enhances vitality decreases · identification; weakens boundaries; decreases vitality Predisposition to act in terms of ethnic solidarity/higher status group memberships (and conformity to societal norms) define more situations in terms of ethnicity ('overinclude') many 'solidarity-stressing' inter ethnic situations s, more likelihood of ethnolinguistic differentiation define fewer situations in terms of ethnicity Coverexclude') \ many 'status-stressing' inter ethnic situations less likelihood ofv ethnolinguistic * differentiation Figure 1. Situational and predispositional influences on the likelihood of ethnolinguistic differentiation Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 90 H. Giles and P. Johnson impede the minority groups' learning of a dominant ethnic group's tongue. Subsequent to the appearance of this paper, research has supported as well as called for extensions to other social psychological models in second-language learning (Gardner 1985), yet none of these have addressed empirically the variables and processes inherent in the intergroup model. Hence, we can now suggest that a minority-group individual's lack of oral proficiency in a dominant-group language is not so much a function of the hypotheses which led to the present research (see subgroup A, Giles and Byrne 1982) as to the conditions laid down in proposition I above. Similarly, proficiency in such a second language is not so much the converse of this paper's hypotheses (see subgroup B, Giles and Byrne 1982) as it is a function of the psychological conditions specified in proposition III herein. It could be argued that the 'in-betweeners' highlighted in proposition II (see also Ball et al. 1984) would be those most susceptible to the particular pedagogical environment (that is, variations in methods and teaching skills) in which the learner acquires the language. Furthermore, the present data lead us to reappraise the notion of Oral proficiency' as represented in the intergroup model; therein it appears overly static and acontextual. Thus, apparent L2 proficiency is likely to depend on the nature of the situation along the lines of propositions IV and V above in which it is measured. For instance, those for whom even proposition I is 'true' might still evince a fair degree of proficiency under the contextual conditions of proposition IV, while those for whom proposition III is 'true' might be more reluctant to proffer L2 oral skills under conditions manifest in proposition V. Turning to language-attitudes theory, Ryan and associates (Ryan et al. 1982, 1984) identified sociostructural determinants considered to be salient to the type of language-attitude profiles in a speech community and the evaluative constructs which may be used in their assessment. The processes involved are considered in terms of a temporal cycle of intergroup relations (see Taylor and McKirnan 1984) and four language profiles are explicated in terms of the assessments of the higher-status and lower-status ethnic-group speech styles. It is suggested now that the language-attitude profiles of different members of a subordinate group are influenced by their perceptions of their identification with the ethnic group and the other variables identified in the revised propositions of ethnolinguistic identity theory above. Thus, language attitudes which different members of the subordinate group are likely to hold may be considered in terms of their predispositions to act in terms of ethnic solidarity/conformity to ingroup norms/conformity to societal norms. In addition, the findings of Giles and Johnson (1986) suggest that when individuals are acting in terms of ethnic solidarity rather than conformity to societal norms, they are likely to use more differentiated constructs in their evaluations of ethnically threatening and supportive Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 91 speakers. That is, they may be likely to up-/downgrade those who offer support for/threat to a valued aspect of their ethnic identity, whether they are defined as in- or outgroup members on the basis of noncontent speechstyle cues. Together these considerations result in a set of propositions relating to the language-attitude profiles different members of a subordinate group are likely to hold. At the present time, theories in the areas of ethnic language performance and ethnic language attitudes have been quite autonomous and reflected quite separate literatures. Below, then, we are not only proposing a modest extension to language-attitude theory but suggesting that both these fields of inquiry might profit from their being considered in tandem, particularly given that language attitudes oftentimes mediate speechstyle performance (Giles and Street 1985). In other words, we are extending ethnolinguistic identity theory herein to encompass related processes operating in ethnic language attitudes. The propositions therefore are as follows: VI. (a) (b) (c) Subordinate ethnic group members who are disposed to acting in terms of ethnic solidarity rather than conformity to societal norms (proposition I herein) are likely to upgrade those who support the ethnic group, whether in- or outgroup members, and the ingroup speech style, particularly on traits relating to the solidarity dimension (for example, kind) and other salient ingroup dimensions. (Their language attitude profile is therefore likely to resemble the appropriate component of pattern C described by Ryan et al. 1982,1984.) Subordinate ethnic group members who are disposed to acting in terms of conformity to ingroup norms (proposition II herein) are likely to upgrade the ethnic ingroup speech style on traits relating to the solidarity dimension while downgrading it on traits relating to the status dimension (for example, intelligent), and vice-versa for the higher-status ethnic speech style. (Their language attitude profile is therefore likely to resemble the appropriate component of pattern B as described by Ryan et al. 1982,1984.) Subordinate ethnic group members who are predisposed to acting in terms of conformity to societal norms (proposition III herein) are likely to upgrade the higher-status ethnic speech style relative to the lower-status ingroup speech style, particularly on traits relating to the status dimension. (Their language attitude profile is therefore likely to resemble the appropriate component of pattern A in Ryan et al. 1982, 1984.) Proposition VI then elaborates on previous language-attitude models in identifying more clearly the influences of ethnolinguistic identity theory Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 92 H. Giles and P. Johnson variables on the language attitudes subordinate ethnic group members are likely to hold. In addition, they complement the earlier propositions I-III in identifying the language attitudes which mediate the language strategies adopted. Finally, it is suggested that under the conditions defined by proposition IV, where few ethnic group members are likely to define interactions in terms of their ethnic identity, speech-style evaluations are likely to resemble those of proposition VI(c) above. Under these conditions, greater emphasis is likely to be given to societal norms than to ethnic solidarity, and speechstyle evaluations are likely to resemble those of traditional matched-guise studies. In contrast, under conditions specified by proposition V, where many subordinate ethnic group members are considered likely to define interactions in terms of their ethnic identity, speech-style evaluations are likely to resemble those of proposition VI(a) above. Under these conditions, greater emphasis is likely to be afforded ethnic solidarity than societal norms, and upgrading of the ingroup speech style and ethnically supportive speakers is likely to occur, particularly on solidarity traits. Thus, together with the preceding sets of jpropositions I-V, proposition VI may be seen as reflecting the ways in which language attitudes are influenced by both social and personal variables highlighting either ethnic ingroup solidarity or societal norms. Conclusions and future priorities On the basis of an admittedly small sample of young Welshmen using selfreport data (see Bourhis 1984), we have reformulated ethnolinguistic identity theory in modest ways for subordinate ethnic groups and extended it into the domain of ethnic language attitudes having implications for the intergroup model of second-language acquisition. All in all, the empirical data (and this is the first of its kind to be reported) supported some of the main original tenets of our theory and have demonstrated the complex relations between its core variables. Although this approach focuses upon language strategies in the immediacy of intergroup contact, we are now that much more confident that social psychological processes ought to feature in language-maintenance theory at the macro-level. As indicated at the outset, most research in the latter tradition has been macrosociological in orientation, and one recent study which included attitude measures in the design (McAllister and Mughan 1984) found no effect for their mediating role in language maintenance. The implicit conclusion from this study was that psychological variables play at best an extremely minor role in the context of structural forces at play. Now while we would not wish to elevate them to a primary status in this regard we would like Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 93 to point out that their relative status can only be gleaned from empirical investigations which examine variables and processes promulgated in ethnolinguistic identity theory. In other words, a recourse to gross variables such as ingroup-outgroup attitudes does not do *eal justice to the potential role of psychological processes in language maintenance/erosion. In sum, we would suggest that propositions I-III in particular are those which fashion the psychological climate necessary for language maintenance, language indifference, and language suicide, respectively. Obviously, their role at this macrolevel is likely to be a function of the historico-structural forces existing in particular language-conflict situations, and so we are simply scratching the obvious complexity of the theoretical surface at present. Nevertheless, as the title of this special issue implies, we are, and in concert with Bourhis (1983b), opening up what we believe to be fruitful areas of inquiry and suggesting that the balance is redressed that much more in the direction of a psychological input (see also Berry 1984). Indeed we are confident that such new directions (empirically and theoretically) will have important policy implications for language planning and multiculturalism subsequently. Finally, there are a number of areas worthy of immediate pursuance within the context of the empirical development of research relating to ethnolinguistic identity theory. First, larger-scale studies which examine the interrelations between identification, vitality, and group boundaries on the likelihood of language maintenance on the one hand and ethnolinguistic assimilation on the other are required in a diversity of multiethnic settings. Obviously, larger samples of group members who vary widely in their identification with their groups are required, with more detailed questionnaires designed. These could include an examination of (1) Ss' social attributions (Hewstone and Jaspars 1984) relating to perceived relative vitality of ingroup-outgroup and the degree to which the ingroup position is seen as reflecting group 'success/ failure', (2) perceptions of stability/legitimacy of the ingroup's position in the status hierarchy and an analysis as to what dimensions constitute ethnic threat/support, (3) the degree to which other groups with which the individual identifies contribute to or detract from the core values of ethnic identity in their self-concepts (see Wong-Reiger and Taylor 1981), and (4) the subjective nature of the solidarity, ingroup, and societal norms presumed as operative (see McKirnan and Hamayan 1984). It is also clearly important to move to an examination of linguistic behaviors, ultimately in naturally occurring situations so that 'ethnolinguistic differentiation' (not to mention ethnic attenuation) can be spelled out in linguistic, nonverbal, and discourse, etc., detail in a sequential process fashion. An exploration of the ways in which multiple (and sometimes, mixed) motives underlie speech strategies (see Street and Cappella 1985) will doubtless uncover the contextual fact that we may differentiate linguistically by means of certain linguistic features Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 94 H. Giles and P. Johnson and tactics while perhaps simultaneously converging on other dimensions (Coupland 1985). While we have emphasized ethnolinguistic identity theory with respect to ethnic minorities in this paper, our ultimate goal (as can be gleaned from Giles and Johnson 1981) is to consider dominant groups' language strategies in parallel also. For instance, it can often be in the interests of such collectivities to control information flow and the subjective realities of subordinate groups in a way that underscores the latter's low vitality, soft boundaries, and legitimate low status. While we do see the above multiple directions as priorities, we are also not blind to the problematic nature of some of the assumptions underlying ethnolinguistic identity theory, particularly with respect to its implications for language maintenance and policy. The theoretical underpinnings of our propositions rest in no small measure on a definition of social identity in terms of social comparisons with salient outgroups deriving from Tajfel and Turner's (1979) social identity theory; that is, social (and ethnic) identity has been construed in terms of valued differences between the ingroup and salient outgroups. We admit here that this theoretical basis is limited in its conceptualization of identity construction and evaluation, and that alternative conceptualizations may be usefully incorporated into future social psychological (as well as other) research into ethno- and sociolinguistics (see Dryden and Cues 1986). Our perspective has rested on what may be termed a competitive social identity. This type may be characterized as differentiating self (or group) from the field and considered in terms of the intellectual functions of separating and ordering, and the relationship styles of detachment and competition (Williams 1984). The analysis of intergroup relations which has followed from this conceptualization has made important contributions to the understanding of social conflict and strategies which may be adopted for social change. However, alternatives may be necessary in order to understand the ways in which some groups exist in contact while maintaining positive identities (Lambert et al. 1986). One such alternative conceptualization may be defined in terms of valued relationships with other groups, termed a communal social identity. It may be characterized as merging self (or group) with the field and considered in terms of the intellectual functions of unifying and communicating, and the relationship styles of acceptance and cooperation (Williams 1984). An example of the contrasts between competitive and communal social identities is provided by Williams (1980): Social workers can give meaning to their group by comparing themselves with Clinical Psychologists and Health Visitors on valued dimensions. They can also give meaning communally by stressing the valued aspects of their relationship with another group, in this instance, the client group. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 95 It is likely that groups will differ in the potential they offer for the construction of favorable competitive and communal social identities. While there is considerable evidence for the former (Tajfel 1978), there is less empirical evidence relating to the latter (see however Williams 1984). It can be suggested that this reflects in part the type of research which has been conducted, where little opportunity may have been offered for subjects to define their groups in terms of valued relationships with outgroups as well as or instead of valued differences from them. It may also be that where there are wide discrepancies in status between dominant and subordinate groups, positive communal social identities are derived mainly from valued relationships within the group. In situations where subordinate groups have attained greater parity in status and power with dominant groups, it can be proposed that harmonious intergroup relations may be maintained when group members focus on both valued differences and valued relationships between the groups. This notion is akin to the ideology of 'equal but different' (Taylor and Simard 1975) or 'common humanity' (Fishman 1982) and can be applied to Smolicz's (1983) discussion of pluralism. He suggests that stable pluralism may obtain when ethnic group members maintain interaction with other members of their ethnic group, thereby providing the necessary structural support for ethnicity. At the same time, economic and civil relations are defined in terms of individual's personal attributes rather than their ethnic background. Hence, a society which supports an ideology of cultural and social diversity encourages the development of bicultural individuals who can participate in the activities of more than one ethnic group while still maintaining their ethnic identity. As indicated above, and by both ethnolinguistic identity theory and research on second-language learning (for example, Gardner 1985), the conditions fostering such pluralism (cultural amd linguistic) are likely to depend on relative parity in status and power between the groups. In terms of our own theoretical perspective, stable pluralism is likely to maintain when ethnic group members identify relatively strongly with their ethnic group as well as the society of which they are a part; perceive the norms and values of their ethnic group to overlap with those of the society in significant ways; perceive their ethnic boundaries to be hard and closed; and perceive the vitality of their ethnic group to be relatively high. These conditions may be considered to provide relatively stable and secure support for ethnicity, enabling constructive contact with ethnic outgroups (see for example Clement et al. 1977). We argue here that in these circumstances, contact with outgroups is likely to focus on valued aspects of the ethnic outgroups and valued relationships with them and may be usefully conceptualized in terms of communal as well as competitive social identities. In conclusion, we have argued on the basis of exploratory data from Wales Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 96 H. Giles and P. Johnson for a reformulated social psychological theory of some of the relationships between language and ethnicity. This revised model begins to specify some of the complex conditions under which ethnic minorities accentuate and attenuate their distinctive ethnolinguistic styles when interacting with an outgroup speaker and has now been extended propositionally to take on board the domain of language attitudes. This theory not only is a starting point for considering the factors which promote and impede second-language acquisition but also specifies, we believe, some important psychological parameters of language maintenance and erosion. Department of Psychology University of Bristol References Allard, R., and Landry, R. (1985). Subjective ethnolinguistic vitality viewed as a belief system. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 7,1-12. Ball, P., Cues, H., Byrne, J., and Berechree, P. (1984). Situational constraints on the evaluative significance of speech accommodation: some Australian data. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46, 45-129. - Giles, H., and Hewstone, M. (1984). The intergroup theory of second language acquisition with catastrophic dimensions. In The Social Dimensions, vol. 2, H. Tajfel (ed.), 668-694. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Banton, M. (1983). Racial and Ethnic Competition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Beebe, L., and Giles, H. (1984). Speech accommodation theories: a discussion in terms of second language acquisition. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46, 5-32. Berry, J. W. (1984). Multicultural policy in Canada: a social psychological analysis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science 16, 353-370. Bourhis, R. Y. (1979). Language in ethnic interaction: a social psychological perspective. In Language and Ethnic Relations, H. Giles and B. Saint-Jacques (eds.), 117-142. Oxford: Pergamon. — (I983a). Language attitudes and self-reports of French-English usage in Quebec. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 4,163-180. -(ed.) (1983b). Conflict and'Language Planning in Quebec. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. - (1984). Cross-cultural communication in Montreal: two field studies since the Charter of the French Language. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46, 33-47. - , Giles, H., Leyens, J. P., and Tajfel, H. (1979). Psycholinguistic distinctiveness: language divergence in Belgium. In Language and Social Psychology, H. Giles and R. N. St Clair (eds.), 158-185. Oxford: Blackwell. — , Giles, H., and Rosenthal, D. (1981). Notes on the construction of a 'subjective vitality questionnaire' for ethnolinguistic groups. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 2,144-155. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 97 - , and Sachdev, I. (1984). Vitality perceptions and language attitudes: some Canadian data. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 3, 97-126. Clement, R., Gardner, R. C., and Smyth, P. C. (1977). Motivational variables in second language acquisition: a study of Francophones learning English. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science 9,122-133. Coupland, N. (1985). Hark, hark, the lark: social motivations for phonological styleshifting. Language and Communication 5, 151-171. Denison, N. (1977). Language death or language suicide? International Journal of the Sociology of Language 12,13-22. Dry den, C., and Giles H. (1986). Language, social identity and health. In Psychology Survey 6, H. Beloff and A. Coleman (eds.). Leicester: British Psychological Society Press. Eastman, C. M. (1979). Language resurrection. In Language and Ethnic Relations, H. Cues and B. Saint-Jacques (eds.), 215-227. Oxford: Pergamon. Edwards, J. R. (1985). Language, Society and Identity. Oxford: Blackwell. Fishman, J. A. (1982). Whorfianism of the third kind. Language in Society 11,1-14. Gal, S. (1979). Language Shift: Social Determinants of Linguistic Shift in Bilingual Austria. New York: Academic Press. Gallois, C., Callan, V. J., and Johnstone, M. (1984). Personality judgements of Australian Aborigine and White speakers: ethnicity, sex and context. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 3, 39-58. Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social Psychology and Second Language Learning. London: Edward Arnold. Genesee, F., Rogers, P., and Holobow, N. (1983). The social psychology of second language learning: another point of view. Language Learning 33, 209-224. Giles, H. (1979). Ethnicity markers in speech. In Social Markers in Speech, K. R. Scherer and H. Cues (eds.), 251-289. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. - (ed.) (1984). The dynamics of speech accommodation. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46 (special issue). - , Bourhis, R. Y., and Taylor, D. M. (1977). Towards a theory of language in ethnic group relations. In Language, Ethnicity and Intergroup Relations, H. Giles (ed.), 307-348. London: Academic Press. - and Byrne, J. (1982). The intergroup model of second language acquisition. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 3,17-40. - , and Johnson, P. (1981). The role of language in ethnic group relations. In Intergroup Behaviour, J. C. Turner and H. Cues (eds.), 199-243. Oxford: Blackwell. - , and Johnson, P. (1986). Perceived threat, ethnic commitment, and inter-ethnic language behaviour. In Interethnic Communication: Recent Research, Y. Kim (ed.), 91116. 10th Intercultural Communication Annual. Beverly Hills: Sage. - , Rosenthal, D., and Young, L. (1985). Perceived ethnolinguistic vitality: the Angloand Greek-Australian setting. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 6, 256-269. - , and Street, J. L., Jr. (1985). Communicator characteristics and behaviour: a review, generalizations, and model. In The Handbook of Interpersonal Communication, M. Knapp and G. Miller (eds.), 205-262. Beverly Hüls: Sage. Harvey, J. H., and Smith, W. P. (1977). Social Psychology: An Attributional Approach. St. Louis, Mo.: Mosby. Hewstone, M., and Giles, H. (1986). Social groups and social stereotypes in intergroup communication: a review and model of intergroup communication breakdown. In Intergroup Communication, W. B. Gudykunst (ed.), 10-26. London: Arnold. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM 98 H. Giles and P. Johnson - , and Jaspars, J. (1984). Social dimensions of attributions. In The Social Dimensions, vol. 2, H. Tajfel (ed.), 380-444. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hildebrandt, N., and Giles, H. (1983). The Japanese as subordinate group: ethnolinguistic identity theory in a foreign language context. Anthropological Linguistics 26, 436-466. Huffines, L. M. (1986). Strategies of language maintenance and ethnic marking among the Pennsylvanian Germans. Language Sciences 8,1-16. Lambert, W. E., Mermigis, L., and Taylor, D. M. (1986). Greek Canadians' attitudes toward own group and other Canadian ethnic groups: a test of the multiculturalism hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science 18, 35-51. Laporte, P. (1983). Status language planning in Quebec: an evaluation. In Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec, R. Y. Bourhis (ed.), 53-80. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. McAllister, I., and Mughan, A. (1984). The fate of the language: determinants of bilingualism in Wales. Ethnic and Racial Studies 7, 321-341. McKirnan, D. J., and Hamayan, E. V. (1984). Speech norms and attitudes towards outgroup members: a test of a model in a bicultural context. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 3, 21-38. Milroy, L. (1982). Language and group identity. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 3, 207-216. Oakes, P., and Turner, J. C. (1980). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour: does minimal intergroup discrimination make social identity more positive? European Journal of Social Psychology 10, 295-301. Ryan, E: B., Giles, H., and Sebastian, R. J. (Ϊ982). An integrative perspective for the study of attitudes towards language variation. In Attitudes Towards Language Variation: Social and Applied Contexts, E. B. Ryan and H. Giles (eds.), 1-19. London: Arnold. — , Hewstone, M., and Giles, H. (1984). Language and intergroup attitudes. In Attitudinal Judgement, J. R. Eiser (ed.), 135-160. New York: Springer. Saint-Blancat, C. (1985). The effect of minority group vitality upon its sociopsychological behaviour and strategies. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 6, 31-44. Smolicz, J. J. (1983). Meaning and values in cross-cultural contexts. Ethnic and Racial Studies 6, 33-49. Street, J. L., Jr., and Cappella, J. N. (eds.) (1985). Sequence and Pattern in Communicative Behaviour. London: Arnold. Sweeting, H. (1982). Aspects of psycholinguistic distinctiveness: a Scottish example. Unpublished B.A. thesis, University of Edinburgh. Tajfel, H. (1974). Social identity and intergroup behaviour. Social Science Information 13, 65-93. - (ed.) (1978). Differentiation Between Social Groups. London: Academic Press. — (ed.) (1982). Social Identity and Intergroup Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. - , and Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, W. C. Austin and S. Worchel (eds.), 33-53. Monterey: Brooks/Cole. Taylor, D. M., Doria, J., and Tyler, J. K. (1983). Group performance and cohesiveness: an attribution analysis. Journal of Social Psychology 119, 187-198. - , and McKirnan, D. J. (1984). Afive-stagetheory of intergroup behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology 23, 291-300. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Ethnolinguistic identity theory 99 - , and Royer, L. (1980). Group processes affecting anticipated language choice in intergroup relations. In Language: Social Psychological Perspectives, H. Giles, W. P. Robinson, and P. M. Smith (eds.), 185-192. Oxford: Pergamon. — , and Simard, L. (1975). Social interaction in a bilingual setting. Canadian Psychological Review 16, 240-254. Turner, J. C., Hogg, Μ. Α., Turner, P. J., and Smith, P. M. (1984). Failure and defeat as determinants of group cohesiveness. British Journal of Social Psychology 23, 97-111. Williams, C. (1979). An ecological and behavioural analysis of ethnolinguistic change in Wales. In Language and Ethnic Relations, H. Giles and B. Saint-Jacques (eds.), 2758. Oxford: Pergamon. Williams, G. (ed.) (1978). Social and Cultural Change in Contemporary Wales. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Williams, J. A. (1980). Women and group identification: some implications for their behaviour in the work force. Paper presented at the International Workshop on Social Psychology, Canterbury. - (1984). Gender and intergroup behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology 23, 311-316. Wong-Reiger, D., and Taylor, D. M. (1981). Multiple group membership and self-identity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 12, 61-79. Young, L., Giles, H., and Pierson, H. (1986). Sociopolitical change and perceived vitality. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 10, 459-469. Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM Brought to you by | Heinrich Heine Universität Düsseldorf Authenticated | 93.180.53.211 Download Date | 12/5/13 11:24 AM