. J. AGING AND HUMAN DEVELOPMENT, Vol. 64(4) 299-330, 2007 WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND THE “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE: THE IMPLICATIONS OF CHILDLESSNESS, FERTILITY TIMING, AND MARITAL STATUS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING IN LATE MIDLIFE TANYA KOROPECKYJ-COX University of Florida, Gainesville AMY MEHRABAN PIENTA University of Michigan TYSON H. BROWN University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill ABSTRACT We explore women’s psychological well-being in late midlife in relation to childlessness and timing of entry into motherhood. Using two U.S. surveys, Health and Retirement Study (HRS) (1992) and National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) (Sweet, Bumpass, & Call, 1988), we assess the well-being of childless women in their 50s compared to mothers with early, delayed, or normatively timed first births. We focus on the cohorts born between 1928 and 1941, who experienced strong normative pressures during the baby boom with regard to marriage and child-bearing. We find few differences among childless women but lower well-being among early mothers, related to singlehood and poorer socioeconomic status. Unmarried mothers are significantly disadvantaged regardless of maternal timing, controlling for socioeconomic status. Current maternal demands are independently related to well-being and help to explain observed differences in family satisfaction. Overall, childlessness and off-time child-bearing are related to midlife well-being through their link with more proximate factors, particularly current marital status, health, and socioeconomic status. 299 Ó 2007, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. 300 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN INTRODUCTION “The institution of marriage had a power and inevitability in the fifties that it has never had since. You simply didn’t ask yourself if you wanted marriage and children; the only relevant questions were when and how many? And the answers were, as soon as possible and as many as possible” (Harvey, 1993, p. 69). Women who were adolescents or young adults during the 1950s experienced the historically and demographically unique post-World War II, baby-boom era. These “mothers of the baby boom” are distinguished by early and near-universal marriage (median age of 19-20), earlier fertility (most first births occurred between 19 and 25), and higher fertility (Rindfuss, Morgan, & Swicegood, 1988). Only 8 to 10% of women born between 1928 and 1941 remained permanently childless, and voluntary childlessness was regarded as “nearly extinct” (Whelpton, Campbell, & Patterson, 1966, pp. 162-163). For women born in the late 1920s and 1930s, the distinctiveness of their birth cohorts reflects childhood experiences of the Great Depression, which steered many toward seeking economic stability and domestic home lives as adults (Elder, 1974). Their early hardships and modest expectations, combined with having been born into a small birth cohort, placed them in a position to benefit greatly in the postwar years (Easterlin, 1980). Those born in the late 1930s or early 1940s had little experience of the Great Depression, but were young children during the economic and social upheavals of World War II. For both the younger and older cohorts, the prosperity of the 1950s represented a tremendous boost in economic well-being that occurred as they were reaching adolescence or young adulthood, getting married, and starting families. This era offered unprecedented opportunities for home ownership, higher education, and new amenities and lifestyles (May, 1988; Modell, Furstenberg, & Hershberg, 1976). Beyond economics, the social and political cultures of the era contributed to the distinctive demographic profile. The ethos of the 1950s emphasized the nuclear family as a haven and as the locus for personal fulfillment, encouraging universal marriage, child-bearing, and homemaking roles for women (May, 1988, p. 11; see also Coontz, 1992, 2005). May (1988) has argued that the anxieties of the atomic age and Cold War further increased pressures to conform to societal norms. Men and women who did not take part in heterosexual marriage, child-bearing, and breadwinner-homemaker gender roles were labeled as deviant, “unnatural,” or dangerous (Coontz, 1992, 2005; May, 1988, 1995). The personal and social implications of marrying late or not at all, remaining childless, or having children early or late compared to their peers, therefore, may have been especially salient for women of this cohort. The current article explores the extent to which psychological well-being in late midlife among these women of the 1950s is linked with parental status and past fertility timing. We focus on women in their 50s, an age at which the WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 301 implications of earlier life history may be particularly significant in terms of employment and economic circumstances as well as relationships and personal self-evaluations. In light of earlier normative pressures and current concerns in midlife, we expect that women’s psychological well-being will be related to whether or when they became mothers. Specifically, we ask, Do women who became mothers “on time” have a psychological advantage in midlife compared to earlier and later mothers and women who have remained childless, and do these differences vary with current marital or partner status? To what extent are advantages explained by demographic and socioeconomic characteristics? Finally, to what extent do objective differences in the current and past demands on mothers explain any psychological differences between normative and “off-time” mothers? CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND Social Norms, Age Timetables, and Fertility Though the 50s have been described as the “prime of life” for women (Mitchell & Helson, 1990), psychological well-being in later midlife depends on the implications of earlier life paths (Barrett, 2000; Wheaton, 1990). Late midlife includes the physiological transition of menopause, signifying the end of childbearing or the permanence of childlessness, as well as social transitions around the launching of children, changes in employment, marital and relationship changes, and possible entry into grandparenthood. By their 50s, mothers who began child-bearing in their teens or early 20s have launched their young adult children. Their concerns in midlife may include the needs of younger children still at home, the transitions of young adults, and the needs of grandchildren. For women who became mothers in their late 20s to 40s, midlife may still be defined by parental responsibilities as well as relationships with adolescents and young adults. In addition, earlier experiences, including marital history (Wilmoth & Koso, 2002), child-bearing, and child-rearing (Moen, 2001; O’Rand & Henretta, 1982), may leave lasting and cumulative effects on women’s status in midlife, including their economic resources, employment, and psychological well-being. We draw on Neugarten’s (1968; Neugarten, Moore, & Lowe, 1965) conceptualization of “on-time” and “off-time” life transitions to explore whether timing of entry into motherhood is linked to psychological well-being in middle age. As originally described within role theory, the occurrence and timing of life course transitions (i.e., role entries and exits) are governed by social norms that include social sanctions when individuals do not conform to expectations (Clausen, 1972; Neugarten & Datan, 1973). Age timetables have been described more recently within the life course framework as the normatively defined 302 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN expectations regarding the sequence and timing of life transitions (Settersten & Hagestad, 1996a, 1996b). Transitions that occur “off-time” rather than “on-time,” as defined by social norms, more often result in adverse consequences (Brim & Ryff, 1980; Neugarten et al., 1965; Neugarten & Datan, 1973). Social changes in recent decades have brought greater life course variability and acceptance of different life paths, but concerns about the timing of life transitions have remained relevant, as is evident in current research on grandparents (e.g., Landry-Meyer & Newman, 2004), caregivers for persons with AIDS (Land, Hudson, & Steiffel, 2003; Wight, 2000), and the death of a parent (Cait, 2004). Remaining childless represents the absence of an expected, normative transition. In the 1950s, strong pro-natalist ideologies prescribed universal marriage and child-bearing, and both men and women were subjected to social pressures if they did not conform. Childless women in particular reported feeling stigmatized (Houseknecht, 1977; May, 1995; Veevers, 1980). Infertility was regarded with pity, whereas voluntary childlessness was met with scorn or suspicion (May, 1995). Further, the centrality of motherhood for women’s development, mental health, and fulfillment was stressed in both psychological and popular books of the time (e.g., Erikson, 1963; Farnham & Lundberg, 1947; see Epstein, 2003), and opinion surveys reflected widespread pro-natalist attitudes (Veroff, Douvan, & Kulka, 1981; see May, 1988, 1995). Though norms encouraging parenthood were strong, expectations regarding the timing of this transition were more flexible. Becoming a parent was contingent on “readiness,” including marriage and economic circumstances, and expectations varied in response to prevailing social and economic conditions (Rindfuss et al., 1988). As a result, ages of entry into motherhood have varied historically and within cohorts. At the same time, societal age norms became more potent in the postwar era with the decline of familial influence on life course transitions (Hareven & Masaoka, 1988; Modell et al., 1976). Several studies have documented specific norms for marriage and fertility timing as well as variations by cohort (Fallo-Mitchell & Ryff, 1982; Gee, 1990; Neugarten, Moore, & Lowe, 1965; Rindfuss & Bumpass, 1976). For example, a survey of middle-class Americans in the 1960s identified ages 19 to 24 as the best ages for women to marry, with child-bearing expected to begin soon thereafter (Neugarten et al., 1965). The concepts of normative and “off-time” transitions have been applied to women’s transitions to motherhood, with first births in the 20s generally defined as normative in the mid- to late-twentieth century (Rindfuss et al., 1988). “Off-time,” teen births have received the most attention, but less is known about normative and later births and their potential implications later in the life course (Rindfuss et al., 1988). Research on midlife well-being, on the other hand, has focused primarily on midlife transitions (e.g., Marks, Lambert, & Choi, 2002; Turner, Killian, & Cain, 2004) with less attention to longer term effects of earlier life paths. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 303 Psychological Well-Being of Women in Midlife Studies of the psychological strains and benefits of parenting have generally reported few differences in happiness, life satisfaction, loneliness, or depression related to parental status in older adulthood (Glenn & McLanahan, 1981; Koropeckyj-Cox, 1998a; Rempel, 1985), and few psychological advantages have been found for parents of all ages (Evenson & Simon, 2005). In midlife, empty-nest, married parents (aged 45-59) have reported greater satisfaction with their family lives but similar overall life satisfaction compared to the childless (Bell & Eisenberg, 1985). However, qualitative research has identified midlife as a potentially difficult time for childless women, drawing on retrospective accounts from older women (e.g., Alexander, et al., 1992; KoropeckyjCox, 1998b; Lang, 1991) and younger women’s concerns about the future (Ireland, 1993; Vissing, 2002). The long-term effects of fertility timing have received less attention. The most comprehensive study to date (Mirowsky & Ross, 2002) has reported a U-shaped relationship between age at first birth and depression for women, with particularly high levels among early mothers. Their greater distress is attributed to economic factors, family structure, and health. The observed increase in depression among later mothers (late 30s or later) is less definitive because of their small number (Mirowsky & Ross, 2002). Other studies have focused primarily on the longterm effects of early fertility, especially non-marital child-bearing (Furstenberg, Brooks-Gunn, & Morgan, 1987; Wu, 1996), which has been linked to a variety of social, economic, and psychological disadvantages. These include lower educational attainment, limited employment prospects, and lower incomes in adulthood, though these differences largely reflect predisposing factors for teen motherhood, including childhood poverty (Anderton, Tsuya, Bean, & Mineau, 1987; Geronimus & Korenman, 1993; Moore & Sugland, 1999). Psychological well-being in midlife reflects these material disadvantages and the associated risks of poorer health and singlehood (Mirowsky & Ross, 2002), which may also diminish the quality of parenting and mothers’ midlife self-evaluations and assessments of their mothering (Keyes & Ryff, 1999; Ryff, Lee, Essex, & Schmutte, 1994). Women who became mothers at older ages, on the other hand, may enter their 50s with on-going responsibilities and financial obligations to younger children. For example, Choi (2002) has found that mothers in late midlife with at least one child at home are less likely to see themselves as retired compared to childless women and empty-nest mothers, though they are equally likely to engage in paid work. Different perceptions of work and retirement status, as well as the potential demands of current maternal responsibilities, may reduce psychological well-being among later mothers in midlife. On the other hand, higher education levels and other social and economic advantages may help to buffer these stresses (Mirowsky & Ross, 2002). 304 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN The women born between the late 1920s and early 1940s are among the first women to notably increase their labor force attachment throughout the life course (Anderson, 1981). Expanding work opportunities, employment, and greater economic resources may have reduced the implications of maternal timing or childlessness in midlife. However, these structural changes have disproportionately benefited married, dual-earner couples at a time when the odds of divorce and single motherhood increased. In midlife, therefore, we expect to see greater disadvantage among women who are unmarried or have been single mothers in the past. Beyond economic considerations, psychological well-being in midlife is also shaped by social and internal processes over the life course. Research on midlife has linked distress or well-being with the relative importance of social roles, like marriage and parenthood, and combinations of multiple roles (Thoits, 1992). Greater distress among mothers is related to the demands of parenting and the need to balance multiple responsibilities, such as employment and caring for older parents. The symbolic importance of parenting roles may exacerbate the stresses of parents who are highly committed to their parenting identities (Simon, 1992). Attitudes about non-normative life paths and experiences of stigma—both in social interaction and in internalized self-appraisals—may also reduce well-being. Popular culture has reinforced negative images of off-time transitions and their consequences, particularly with regard to off-time motherhood and childlessness (Harvey, 1993; May, 1995; Neugarten, 1979). The normative emphasis on motherhood for women’s identity may challenge the self-concepts of childless women and off-time mothers, though the effects may be mitigated by supportive reference groups and positive attitudes (Carr, 2004; Houseknecht, 1977; Koropeckyj-Cox, 2002). Finally, perceptions of choice or mastery over the life course may also contribute to more positive outcomes, such as among women who are childless by choice (Connidis & McMullin, 1993; Jeffries & Konnert, 2002) or who deliberately delayed motherhood to pursue educational and career goals. Among mothers, a sense of mastery and more positive self-evaluations are linked with better psychological well-being (Keyes & Ryff, 1999; Ryff et al., 1994). Involuntary childlessness, on the other hand, is linked with greater distress (Connidis & McMullin, 1993; King, 2003; McQuillan, Griel, White, & Jacob, 2003) and regrets in midlife (Jeffries & Konnert, 2002). In sum, studies that have compared childless women with mothers have ignored the diversity of maternal timing among mothers. Although age patterns of fertility have been studied extensively, little research has examined the implications of maternal timing on psychological well-being over the life course. Further, a focus on economic and biological correlates of fertility timing has ignored the ways that historical, social, and cultural factors may have shaped the meanings and implications of fertility timing (see discussion in Mirowsky & Ross, 2002). The current analyses, therefore, begin to explore whether earlier social ideas about WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 305 “normative” vs. “non-normative” or “off-time” life transitions are related to women’s psychological well-being in midlife. RESEARCH HYPOTHESES We define “on-time” motherhood operationally as the ages at which about half of the women of the study cohorts gave birth to their first child (ages 19 to 24) based on the observed distributions in two surveys. These distributions concur with historical demographic data for these cohorts (Heuser, 1976). Drawing on the concept of “on-time” and “off-time” life transitions, we expect normative mothers to report greater psychological well-being in midlife than women with non-normative life paths, particularly those with early first births. Early mothers’ psychological resources in midlife are likely to be further reduced by socioeconomic disadvantages and the greater likelihood of marital instability over the life course. Normative timing, on other hand, is linked with generally higher education levels and a greater likelihood of being married, thus amplifying any normative advantages and leading to greater happiness and satisfaction. The psychological implications of remaining childless or delaying motherhood are less clear. Although likely to benefit psychologically and economically from higher levels of education and greater labor force participation, these women may find that they are out-of-step with regard to normative midlife transitions. The psychological consequences may be negative for delayed mothers who are still actively engaged in the demands of parenting in their 50s, while their peers with earlier fertility may be launching their young adult children, redirecting their energies into work or other pursuits, or becoming grandmothers. At the same time, delayed motherhood may offer psychological advantages through the personal satisfactions and integrative nature of active parenting compared to childless and empty-nest women (Burton, 1998). DATA AND METHODS Our analyses use data from two national surveys. The Health and Retirement Study, conducted in 1992, includes women aged 51 to 61 who were born between 1931 and 1941. The National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) provides a comparable sample from 1987-1988. Limiting our analyses to women aged 51-61, the NSFH sample includes the births cohorts of 1926 to 1938, providing substantial overlap with the HRS cohorts. We use the first waves of these surveys, both of which utilized representative sampling designs. We use sample weights provided with the data to account for the over-sampling of racial-ethnic minorities and other population sub-groups (e.g., cohabiting couples and other specific living arrangements in the NSFH). Our parallel, cross-sectional analyses are motivated by several reasons. First, because childless women are relatively rare in this cohort of women, examining 306 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN data from two nationally representative surveys strengthens our findings. Detailed information on fertility and other factors allows us to test hypotheses about the links between psychological well-being and parental status and timing; using both surveys allows for a conservative approach, seeking confirmation of findings across the two samples. Further, as neither survey is explicitly designed to study psychological status in detail, our parallel analyses allow for a wider range of psychological well-being measures than if we had included only one study. We acknowledge that using surveys from different years raises questions about comparability with regard to age, period, and cohort. Our primary aim is to compare well-being across subgroups of women, as defined by maternal status and timing. As psychological status, particularly depression, is known to vary with age (e.g., Kessler, Foster, Webster, & House, 1992; Mirowsky & Ross, 1992), we examine the same age groups in both surveys. Focusing on the women aged 51 to 61 (born 1928-38 in the NSFH vs. 1931-41 in the HRS) allows for substantial overlap of birth cohorts, with both covering the most distinctive cohort fertility patterns among women born in 1931 to 1938 (see Rindfuss et al., 1988). Other scholars have noted that the post-war baby boom patterns are shared across the range of women born between the late 1920s through the early 1940s, with distinct breaks distinguishing them from the cohorts born before and after. Finally, we note that the different sampling designs and measures argue against comparing the same cohorts at different ages. Health and Retirement Study The Health and Retirement Study (HRS) (see Juster & Suzman, 1995) is a national longitudinal survey of adults ages 51 to 61. Interviews began in 1992 with biannual follow-up interviews. Our analyses use cross-sectional data from women in the first wave. HRS is a valuable data source on marriage and fertility history, physical health, mental health, life satisfaction, and numerous socioeconomic indicators. Moreover, the historical significance of the HRS cohort has allowed us to examine psychological well-being among women who have likely faced harsher midlife implications of deviating from normative marital and fertility patterns. Our sample includes 4,984 women after excluding cases with missing data on key variables. National Survey of Families and Households: The National Survey of Families and Households was conducted in 1987-88 through face-to-face interviews and self-administered questionnaires completed by randomly chosen adult respondents from each household (see Sweet, Bumpass, & Call, 1988). The NSFH has provided invaluable data on marriage and cohabitation, fertility and family histories, attitudes about family life, and various indicators of adults’ and children’s well-being. We restrict our analyses to women aged 51 to 61 years to match the HRS sample ages. In order to focus on the contrast between biological parenthood and childlessness, we exclude from both surveys any biologically WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 307 childless women with adopted children. Excluding women with incomplete or missing data on the key variables of interest, the NSFH analyses are based on samples of 792, 744, and 719 women with data available for the dependent variables, loneliness, depression, and happiness, respectively. Dependent Variables We examine several measures of psychological well-being—family satisfaction, life satisfaction, reported happiness, depressive symptoms, and loneliness. Family Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction Family satisfaction and life satisfaction are measured in the HRS using a 5-point Likert scale with responses ranging from very satisfied to very dissatisfied. Family satisfaction and life satisfaction measures are derived from the survey questions, “Are you very satisfied, somewhat satisfied, evenly satisfied and dissatisfied, somewhat dissatisfied, or very dissatisfied with your family life (or life as a whole)?” Both response distributions are highly skewed, with “very satisfied” chosen by the majority of women. We therefore analyze a collapsed, dichotomous measure that compares the very satisfied with all other women. Self-Reported Happiness Self-reported happiness is measured in the NSFH in the self-administered questionnaire with a single, global assessment that asks, “Taking things all together, how would you say things are these days?” The responses range from 1 “very unhappy” to 7 “very happy,” with an average value of 5.34. We analyze both the original responses and a dichotomous indicator of being very happy (nearly 27% of women), though only the former analyses are reported in the tables. Depressive Symptoms Depressive symptoms are measured in both surveys using modified versions of the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, 1977). Both surveys ask about the frequency of feelings, such as loneliness and fearfulness, experienced by respondents over the past week. In the HRS, the scale includes 14 items with a four-category response set ranging from 4 for “most of the time” to 1 for “none of the time.” The 14 items are summed to create an index of depressive symptoms, ranging from 14 to 56, with an internalconsistency coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) of .87. In the NSFH, respondents are asked in the self-administered questionnaire how often in the last week they experienced each of 12 depressive symptoms, on a scale from 0 (none) to 7. The total depression score is the sum of the 12 items and ranges from 0 to 84. Higher scores in both surveys indicate greater reported frequency of depressive 308 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN symptoms. This shortened version of the CES-D is not a diagnostic tool of depression per se, but is a useful measure of distress in the general population. Loneliness Loneliness is measured in both surveys using a single item that appears as part of the CES-D, asking respondents how frequently in the past week they have felt lonely. Responses in the HRS range from all or almost all of the time to never or almost none of the time. These responses are collapsed into a dichotomous indicator of reporting loneliness all or most of the time compared to everyone else. In the NSFH, scores range from 0 to 7, with a high score indicating greater frequency of loneliness. The loneliness score is highly correlated with the total CES-D score (r = .56 in the HRS and .78 in the NSFH) and with the CES-D score excluding the loneliness item (r = .51 and .72, respectively). However, loneliness and CES-D scores show different patterns of covariation with the independent variables of interest and are therefore analyzed and discussed separately (see Koropeckyj-Cox, 1998a, 2002). Further, analyses using a dichotomous measure of high loneliness (the highest 20%, with scores greater than 2; results available from first author) confirm the findings from the interval measure of loneliness. Independent Variables Parental Status and Timing Parental status and timing is the main independent variable in the analyses. We define childlessness as the absence of either biological or adopted children. Mothers are divided into three groups based on the timing of their first birth in order to compare childless women with normative and off-time parents. The designations of early, normative, and late motherhood are based on the distributions of first-birth timing in the two surveys, which correspond with the historical distributions of maternal timing among women in the cohorts born between 1927 and 1941 (Heuser, 1976). The “normative” group includes about half of the mothers in the middle of the range of ages at maternity, with first births between the ages of 19 and 24. The early and delayed categories are defined as the lowest and highest quartiles of the distribution. Early mothers are those whose first birth occurred at age 18 or younger. Delayed motherhood is defined as having had a first birth at age 25 or later. Marital Status Marital status is coded as a set of dummy variables describing current marital or cohabitation status (never married, married/cohabiting, divorced/separated, widowed). As marital status is closely tied to fertility and is a well-documented correlate of psychological well-being, we examine the intersection of marital status and maternal status and timing with a series of mutually exclusive dummy WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 309 variables: childless never married, childless formerly married, childless married, early unmarried, early married, normative married, normative unmarried, late married, and late unmarried. In analyses of mothers only with the larger HRS sample, we also test detailed interactions between fertility timing and current marital status (results not shown). Other Variables Socio-demographic controls included in the models are measured the same way in both surveys. Age is measured in years, ranging from 51 to 61. Education is coded as a set of dummy variables for completed schooling (less than high school, high school, beyond high school). Self-rated health is a single-item, ranging from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent). Current labor force status indicates whether the respondent worked full-time (35 hours or more per week), part-time, or less than five hours per week at the time of the interview. Income represents the total household income at the time of the interview. We use the natural logarithm to reduce the skewed income distribution. Missing values for income are estimated using multiple imputation methods. Finally, in analyses of mothers only, we include several characteristics of the mothers’ histories and current responsibilities. These include the total number of children ever born, whether their youngest child is under the ages of 18 or 22, whether there are any children under 18 or grandchildren in the household, whether the first birth occurred outside of marriage, and whether the respondent had been a single mother. Analytical Methods We compare and test for bivariate differences among the parental status groups— childless, early mothers, normative mothers, and late mothers—computing Chi-square statistics for categorical variables and analyses of variance for means comparisons of scalar variables. We use multiple regression methods to test for differences among the parental status groups, controlling for marital status, and among the combined marital and parental status groups, controlling for age, race, health, work status, and household income. Finally, for mothers only, we run separate models testing each potential mediator between maternal timing and psychological well-being. Logistic regressions are used to analyze binary outcome variables: life satisfaction, family satisfaction, and loneliness (HRS). We use OLS regression methods to analyze the interval variables: depression (both surveys), happiness, and loneliness (NSFH). RESULTS Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of women in each maternal status group in both surveys. Maternal status and timing are associated with each outcome measure, with stronger differences in the negative outcomes. The majority of NSFH 1987-1988 HRS 1992 Early Normative Delayed Childless mothers mothers mothers Early Normative Delayed Childless mothers mothers mothers N 84 203 420 150 Positive outcome measures High Family Satisfaction (%) High Life Satisfaction (%) Happiness (mean; 0-7) — — 5.25 — — 5.15 — — 5.29 — — 5.70 — 1.03 — 11.6 17.4 — 1.48 — 19.5 31.5 — 1.27 — 15.9 22.0 — 0.65 — 10.5 13.2 45.7 5.2 11.2 37.9 63.7 19.2 15.8 1.3 73.4 13.5 12.4 0.7 74.6 16.2 8.1 1.1 Negative outcome measures Ever Lonely (%) Loneliness (mean; 0-7) CES-D (mean; 14-56) CES-D (mean; 0-84) High Depression (%) Control variables Marital Status (%) Married/Cohabiting Divorced/Separated Widowed Never Married ** ** *** *** *** 379 692 2,715 1,270 * ** 68.3 55.5 — 65.2 51.9 — 70.6 59.5 — 67.1 55.5 — *** 27.0 — 22.5 — — 34.6 — 24.3 — — 25.5 — 22.6 — — 24.8 — 22.1 — — 47.5 15.2 7.7 29.7 64.7 19.8 14.0 1.5 74.7 15.1 9.4 0.8 74.4 15.9 8.5 1.2 *** *** 310 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Table 1. Sample Characteristics of Women (Aged 51-61) by Maternal Status and Timing of First Birth: National Survey of Families and Households (1987-88) and the Health and Retirement Study (1992)a — ** Education (%) Less than high school High school More than high school *** Self-Rated Health (mean; 1-5 hi) Working (%) Little or no (0-5 hrs.) Part-time (6-34 hrs.) Full-time (35+ hrs.) Household Income (mean log) Characteristics of Mothersb Children ever born (mean) Age of youngest child (mean) Youngest under 18 (%) Youngest under 22 (%) Anyone in home under 18 (%) Grandchild in home (%) Non-marital first birth (%) Single mother of child < 18 (%) *** — 56.3 23.0 55.6 30.0 55.7 17.3 55.7 17.6 27.9 42.1 30.0 58.8 34.5 6.7 24.4 52.8 22.8 21.0 33.7 45.3 4.0 3.5 3.9 3.9 *** 42.5 7.0 50.5 9.3 47.7 15.5 36.8 9.1 47.8 14.7 37.5 9.8 42.3 19.6 38.1 10.4 *** *** ** *** ** * *** *** — — — — — — — — 4.6 27.4 13.1 24.8 9.9 14.4 22.6 53.2 3.8 26.1 12.1 27.4 10.4 12.8 6.2 26.3 2.7 22.5 21.9 46.3 20.9 5.2 3.2 25.1 55.6 22.5 55.6 35.2 55.8 16.6 56.1 17.8 15.8 35.1 49.1 55.1 31.6 13.2 20.4 50.0 29.6 14.4 37.4 48.2 3.6 3.0 3.5 3.7 *** 29.9 12.2 57.9 10.1 44.3 13.8 41.9 9.7 37.5 17.9 44.6 10.2 35.3 17.5 47.1 10.4 *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** — — — — — — — — 4.3 27.2 8.3 17.0 8.0 15.4 23.2 51.5 3.4 26.5 6.3 18.4 7.3 8.8 7.3 33.8 2.4 23.3 13.2 34.1 16.1 5.2 4.4 27.6 * *** *** *** *** / 311 aResults are based on weighted data. Maternal status groups are defined as follows: Childless = no biological or adopted children. Early = first birth at age 18 or younger; Normative = first birth between the ages of 19-24; Delayed = first birth at age 25 or older. bBetween-group differences are tested for mothers only. *Difference is statistically significant at p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE Age (mean) Non-White (%) 312 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN women in each group report high satisfaction, though the proportions with high life satisfaction are greatest for normative mothers (59.5%) and lowest for early mothers (51.9%). Early mothers report the most loneliness and depresion, whereas the other three groups are generally similar, with delayed mothers showing an advantage with regard to happiness and loneliness in the NSFH data but not in HRS. As expected, maternal status and timing are strongly associated with current marital status. Normative and delayed mothers are predominantly married (about 73-75%). Childless women are disproportionately never married (30% in HRS, 38% in NSFH); the difference between the two samples may reflect lower marriage rates among the slightly earlier birth cohorts in the NSFH or differences in sampling. Early mothers are most likely to be divorced or widowed and non-white, whereas normative mothers are disproportionately white (83%). Delayed mothers are the most educated, followed by childless women, normative mothers, and early mothers; more than half of early mothers have less than a high school education. Self-reported health is highest among childless women and delayed mothers, followed closely by normative mothers, with early mothers reporting the poorest health. Employment levels vary between the two surveys, potentially reflecting increases in mothers’ full-time employment among the somewhat younger cohorts in the HRS. In both surveys, more than half of the childless women work full-time. Relative income levels also vary between the two surveys, but delayed mothers have the highest incomes and early mothers have the lowest in both surveys. Next we compare characteristics related to motherhood itself, including overall fertility levels and current maternal responsibilities. Total fertility is linked predictably with timing—early mothers have more children on average than normative or delayed mothers. As a result, the average age of the youngest child is about the same for early and normative mothers though several years younger for delayed mothers. The delayed mothers are most likely to have a minor child, a child living in the home, or one under 22. Early mothers, on the other hand, are most likely to live with a grandchild (about 15% in both surveys). Finally, non-marital first births are disproportionately found among early mothers, and over half have been single mothers compared to about one-quarter to one-third of normative and delayed mothers. Regression models show no differences between childless women and normative mothers, controlling for demographic and socio-economic characteristics (Table 2). The disadvantages seen among early mothers in bivariate analyses disappear when marital and socio-economic statuses are controlled. The results for delayed mothers are mixed, with a significant advantage in happiness and lower loneliness and depression in the NSFH but lower life satisfaction in the HRS. Divorced and widowed women show strong, consistent disadvantages compared to married women. Because of the strong association between maternal and marital status, particularly the multicollinearity between childlessness and WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 313 remaining single, we next turn to the combined indicators of marital and maternal status and timing. Table 3 summarizes the results of regression models for the three positive outcomes (happiness, life satisfaction, and family satisfaction) and four negative measures (loneliness and depression in both surveys). To simplify the group comparisons and maintain adequate cell sizes, we have split the childless women into three marital status groups—never married, formerly married (widowed or divorced), and currently married or cohabiting. Mothers are split into the currently married or cohabiting and those who are unmarried (mostly divorced or widowed, with a small fraction of never-married mothers). In more detailed analyses, we find that the unmarried mothers are generally quite similar across specific marital status groups. Examining the intersection of parental and marital status, we find that unmarried mothers—whether early, normative, or delayed—are less likely to be satisfied with family and life, are more lonely (in both surveys), and are more depressed (in HRS but not NSFH) than normative, married mothers. Disadvantages among early and normative unmarried mothers are evident in both surveys, but differences among delayed, unmarried mothers are found only in the HRS. Never-married, childless women are also less likely to be satisfied with family and with life in general (for life satisfaction, b = –.693, or 50% lower odds). Single, childless women are more likely to be lonely and are more depressed in the HRS, but are not statistically different in the NSFH (and coefficients are in the opposite direction). In both surveys, the married childless women are no different from the married, normatively timed mothers, but the childless, formerly married women are disadvantaged on life satisfaction and ever feeling lonely in the HRS. In general, differences among the parental-marital status groups are driven by the impact of marital status. Health is strongly related to well-being in all of the models, and age, full-time employment, and being non-white are linked with more positive outcomes in most of the models. Table 4 summarizes the results for mothers only, controlling for marital status and the demographic and socioeconomic variables. Again, the bivariate disadvantages observed among early mothers are attenuated when marital status and other variables are controlled. Results for delayed mothers are mixed, with a relative advantage seen in the NSFH but lower satisfaction in HRS. In both surveys and on all but one outcome measure (depression in NSFH), we see the strong negative impact of being unmarried. More detailed analyses and tests of the interactions between maternal timing and marital status find a statistically significant interaction only in the models predicting family satisfaction (results available from the authors). Family satisfaction is significantly lower among unmarried mothers with early or delayed first births, but early mothers who are widowed show a smaller disadvantage compared to other widows. Finally, using the models in Table 4 as a base, we examine whether specific aspects of motherhood are related to maternal timing and its connection with Positive outcome Happiness (NSFH)b a Maternal status and timing Childless Early mothers Normative mothers (ref.) Delayed mothers Marital/Partner status Married/cohabiting (ref.) Divorced/separated Widowed Never married Control variables Age White Life Satisfaction (HRS)c b Negative outcomes Family Satisfaction (HRS)c c Loneliness (NSFH)b d Ever Lonely (HRS)c e Depression (NSFH)b f Depression (HRS)b g –.161 (.248) .085 (.145) .007 (.128) –.125) (.100) .155 (.136) –.177 (.102) –.160 (.310) .002 (.188) –.098 (.147) .061 (.108) –1.909 (2.680) .470 (1.598) –.141 (.337) .130 (.263) .301 (.157) –.241*** (.072) –.165* (.074) –.478* (.200) .096 (.083) –3.848* (1.677) .123 (.188) –.505*** (.150) –.635*** (.158) –.087 (.340) –.760*** (.086) –.707*** (.104) –.661*** (.188) –.647*** (.085) –.540*** (.104) –.744*** (.189) .510** (.192) 1.256*** (.201) .408 (.411) .871*** (.092) 1.396*** (.107) 1.014*** (.199) 1.063 (1.635) 3.730* (1.697) –2.607 (3.706) 1.175*** (.228) 1.924*** (.277) 1.351** (.502) .043* (.018) –.225 (.133) .031** (.010) –.210** (.081) .019 (.010) –.018 (.082) –.033 (.023) .422* (.172) –.031** (.011) –.051 (.087) –.106 (.195) 2.889* (1.463) –.144*** (.025) .523* (.212) 314 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Table 2. Psychosocial Well-Being of Women (Aged 51-61) by Maternal Status and Timing: National Survey of Families and Households (1987-88) and the Health and Retirement Study (1992)a Education Less than high school High school (ref.) More than high school Working Little or none (ref.) Part-time Full-time Household Income (log)d Intercept F R2 Wald chi-square 2 log likelihood N .139 (.084) .182* (.088) –.034 (.185) .147 (.090) 1.691 (1.567) .094 (.147) .420*** (.061) –.058 (.071) .459*** (.029) –.170* (.073) .218*** (.030) .113 (.187) –.482*** (.079) –.284*** (.084) –.345*** (.032) 1.459 (1.572) –6.381*** (.667) .065 (.184) .151 (.132) .016 (.029) –.092 (.090) –.181* (.072) .013 (.016) –.102 (.095) –.185* (.075) .016 (.017) –.297 (.229) –.460** (.169) –.026 (.039) –.322** (.106) –.213** (.081) .004 (.018) –3.215 (1.965) –2.685+ (1.421) –.195 (.345) 1.348 7.15*** .12 –2.629*** –.816 416.66*** 6864.55 4975 184.93*** 6045.98 4975 719 4.806*** 8.44*** .13 792 1.664* 497.99*** 5255.91 4975 .320 (.222) –.708*** (.186) –2.328*** (.075) –.635** (.236) –.526** (.187) –.115** (.043) 45.735*** 11.36*** .18 39.631*** 116.45*** .25 744 4975 / 315 aResults are based on weighted data. Maternal timing groups defined by age at first birth. Early = age 18 or younger; Normative = ages 19-24; Delayed = age 25 or older. bUnstandardized OLS regression coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses). cLogistic regression coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses). dRegressions use imputations for cases with missing data on total household income for both HRS and HSFH. *.01 < p <= .05. **.001 < p <= .01. ***p < .001. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE Self-Rated Health .135 (.144) Positive outcome Negative outcomes Life Family Happiness Satisfaction Satisfaction Loneliness (NSFH)b (HRS)c (NSFH)b (HRS)c a b d c Maternal and marital status Childless, never married Childless, formerly married Childless, currently married Early, unmarried mothers Early, married mothers Normative, unmarried mothers Normative, married mothers (ref.) Delayed, unmarried mothers Delayed, married mothers Control variables Age White Ever Lonely (HRS)c e Depression Depression (HRS)b (NSFH)b g f –.618** (.200) –.686** (.229) –.058 (.165) –.759*** (.160) –.193 (.120) –.768*** (.096) –.693*** (.200) –.375 (.234) .107 (.183) –.652*** (.158) –.320* (.126) –.731*** (.097) –.716 (.434) –.048 (.482) –.021 (.441) .948*** (.265) .160 (.259) 1.111*** (.220) .958*** (.213) 1.017*** (.238) –.114 (.211) 1.002*** (.162) .189 (.134) 1.132*** (.103) –5.914 (3.721) .084 (4.117) –3.375 (3.726) 2.676 (2.252) 1.957 (2.183) 3.724* (1.849) 1.168* (.541) 1.179 (.611) –.067 (.432) 1.873*** (.423) –.034 (.318) 1.418*** (.255) –.274 (.241) .317 (.198) –1.034*** (.126) –.232** (.083) –.759*** (.123) –.225* (.088) .082 (.307) –.173 (.258) 1.201*** (.131) .103 (.102) –1.464 (2.590) –3.268 (2.147) 1.502*** (.330) .131 (.218) .043* (.018) –.251 (.132) .032** (.010) –.209* (.081) .020* (.010) –.006 (.082) –.025 (.023) .466** (.173) –.026* (.011) –.048 (.086) –.065 (.194) 3.124* (1.467) –.138*** (.025) .544* (.213) .838* (.346) –.314 (.399) –.523 (.336) –.579** (.203) .144 (.195) –.607*** (.170) 316 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Table 3. Psychosocial Well-Being of Women (Aged 51-61) by Maternal Status and Timing and Marital Status: National Survey of Families and Households (1987-88) and the Health and Retirement Study (1992)a Education Less than high school High school (ref.) More than high school Working Little or none (ref.) Part-time Full-time Household Income (log)d Intercept F R2 Wald chi-square 2 log likelihood N .142 (.084) .190* (.088) –.022 (.187) .164 (.089) 1.731 (1.574) .088 (.147) .417*** (.060) –.062 (.071) .460*** (.029) –.174* (.073) .218*** (.029) .092 (.189) –.465*** (.080) –.297*** (.084) –.337*** (.032) 1.399 (1.579) –6.285*** (.667) .059 (.184) .163 (.131) .013 (.029) –.091 (.091) –.180* (.072) .013 (.016) –.099 (.095) –.181* (.075) .016 (.017) –.291 (.231) –.502** (.169) –.023 (.039) –.328** (.106) –.237** (.081) .005 (.018) –3.241 (1.973) –2.951* (1.423) –.139 (.345) –.635** (.236) –.543** (.187) –.114** (.043) 1.371* 41.867*** 9.90*** .18 39.305*** 101.50 .25 744 4975 1.392 6.92*** .14 719 –2.640*** 417.96*** 6864.55 4975 –.849 188.48*** 6041.73 4975 4.169** 6.93*** .13 792 485.58 5271.91 4975 .344 (.222) –.724*** (.186) –2.320*** (.075) / 317 aResults are based on weighted data. Maternal timing groups defined by age at first birth. Early = age 18 or younger; Normative = ages 19-24; Delayed = age 25 or older. bUnstandardized OLS regression coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses). cLogistic regression coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses). dRegressions use imputations for cases with missing data on total household income for HRS and sample mean for HSFH. *.01 < p <= .05. **.001 < p <= .01. ***p < .001. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE Self-Rated Health .126 (.144) Positive outcome Main Independent variables Maternal status/timing Early mothers Normative mothers (ref.) Delayed mothers Marital Status Married/cohabiting (ref.) Unmarried Control variables Age White Negative outcomes Happiness (NSFH)b a Life Satisfaction (HRS)c b Family Satisfaction (HRS)c c Loneliness (NSFH)b d Ever Lonely (HRS)c e Depression (NSFH)b f Depression (HRS)b g .071 (.145) –.125 (.100) –.180 (.103) .018 (.192) .056 (.108) .605 (1.633) .112 (.265) .329* (.157) –.239** (.072) –.170* (.075) –.509* (.203) .095 (.083) –3.971* (1.709) .130 (.189) –.637*** (.130) –.752*** (.073) –.673*** (.073) .818*** (.169) 1.042*** (.078) 2.377 (1.439) 1.578*** (.193) .038* (.019) –.262 (.141) .031** (.010) –.219* (.085) .015 (.011) –.113 (.087) –.024 (.025) .402* (.186) –.019 (.012) –.075 (.090) –.135 (.208) 2.891 (1.582) –.134*** (.027) .493* (.223) 318 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Table 4. Psychosocial Well-Being of Mothers Only (Aged 51-61) by Timing of First Birth: National Survey of Families and Households (1987-88) and the Health and Retirement Study (1992)a Education Less than high school High school (ref.) More than high school Working Little or none (ref.) Part-time Full-time Household Income (log)d Intercept F R2 Wald chi-square 2 log likelihood N .147+ (.087) .181* (.091) –.030 (.199) .147 (.092) 1.749 (1.687) .059 (.155) .376*** (.063) –.097 (.074) .449*** (.030) –.189* (.077) .224*** (.031) .078 (.201) –.437*** (.084) –.285** (.088) –.329*** (.033) 1.394 (1.697) –5.824*** (.712) –.029 (.190) .163 (.137) .021 (.031) –.083 (.093) –.175* (.075) .016 (.017) –.119 (.099) –.228** (.078) .017 (.017) –.258 (.241) –.443* (.179) –.042 (.042) –.305** (.109) –.247** (.084) .003 (.019) –3.575 (2.070) –2.996* (1.513) –.269 (.375) –.631* (.244) –.611** (.195) –.104* (.045) 1.799 7.93*** .12 –2.591*** –.464 (.630) 4.295** 8.00*** .11 .983 (.684) 46.036*** 11.15*** .16 38.963*** 138.78*** .25 672 4600 652 385.92*** 6336.91 4600 188.16*** 5756.87 4600 713 442.28*** 5366.34 4600 .289 (.229) –.748*** (.194) –2.318*** (.078) / 319 aResults are based on weighted data. Parental groups defined by age at first birth. Early = age 18 or younger; Normative = ages 19-24; Delayed = age 25 or older. bOLS regression coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses). cLogistic regression coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses). dRegressions use imputations for cases with missing data on total household income for HRS and sample mean for HSFH. *.01 < p <= .05. **.001 < p <= .01. ***p < .001. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE Self-Rated Health .125 (.152) 320 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN psychological well-being. Table 5 shows the coefficients for early and late mothers and being currently unmarried, and how they compare when each potential mediator is added alone to the basic model. Controlling for maternal timing, we find that living with at least one grandchild is related to significantly lower happiness (Panel A), lower family satisfaction (Panel B), and more depression (Panel D). The coefficients for early mothers are again not statistically significant and change only slightly when grandchild coresidence is included in the models. Having a child under 18 increases loneliness and depression in the NSFH (Panels C and D); adding this variable increases the salutary effect of delayed motherhood, suggesting that the advantages for delayed mothers are suppressed by the demands of having minor children. None of these variables is significant in predicting life satisfaction or loneliness in the HRS (not shown). Among the outcome measures, we find that the specific characteristics of mothers are most related to family satisfaction (Panel B). Having a non-marital first birth or having been a single mother with minor children reduces family satisfaction, and each of these variables helps to attenuate the marginally significant, negative effect of early motherhood. Having been a single mother with minor children also substantially reduces the negative effect of being currently unmarried (by 17%, or about one-sixth). For delayed mothers, on the other hand, lower family satisfaction is partially explained by their lower fertility and having younger children. DISCUSSION Our results confirm earlier findings that childlessness is not linked with psychological disadvantage in midlife, even among women who experienced the strong pro-natalist pressures of the 1950s. Among mothers, we find consistently poorer psychological status among those with early first births, related to their greater odds of being unmarried and their lower socioeconomic status. We find strong, consistent disadvantages among unmarried mothers, regardless of maternal timing, and some advantages for delayed mothers. We find similar patterns for happiness, loneliness, and depression in the NSFH and some similarities in loneliness and depression across the two surveys. We therefore focus first on these more standard indicators of psychological status, and then return to discuss the intriguing differences in satisfaction with family and with life more generally. Overall, childless women appear no different from mothers in both the bivariate and regression analyses, though results are mixed when childlessness and marital status are combined. Childless, never-married women are happier in the NSFH, but both never- and formerly married childless women in the HRS are less satisfied, more likely to be lonely, and more depressed compared to the normatively timed, married mothers. Married, childless women, however, are no different from married, normatively-timed mothers. These findings largely concur with earlier studies, though the significant differences in the larger HRS sample WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 321 suggest some psychological disadvantages among the childless that merit closer examination. Further, neither survey provides information on the reasons for childlessness and whether it was involuntary or chosen; the observed findings may therefore mask important differences among childless women in midlife. Early mothers’ lower psychological well-being in the bivariate analyses is largely explained by their greater likelihood of being single and economically disadvantaged. These results are consistent across the two surveys and concur with other studies (Mirowsky & Ross, 2002). Delayed mothers, on the other hand, report more positive well-being (in NSFH), even after controlling for their higher educations and incomes. This advantage becomes even stronger for loneliness and depression when we account for the presence of a potentially dependent child. The lack of adverse outcomes among late mothers, despite their greater current maternal demands, suggests that the “off-time” transition of delayed motherhood may have positive benefits for women, including greater economic and personal resources among midlife mothers (see Cooney, Pedersen, Indelicato, & Palkovitz, 1993). However, the HRS analyses failed to confirm these findings, indicating the need for caution in interpreting the results and for further research into the potential positive and negative effects of delayed motherhood on women’s well-being. The strongest and most consistent predictors of well-being are current marital status, self-reported physical health, and current employment. Unmarried women report more negative psychological status in both surveys, regardless of fertility timing, though the difference is more pronounced for early and normative mothers. The centrality of physical health in shaping psychological well-being is clear and underscores the importance of better understanding these cumulative and interwoven life course processes that influence women’s physical and mental health, including the role of earlier economic status and child-bearing. Living with a grandchild is independently related to lower psychological outcomes but does not explain differences among early or single mothers. Finally, racial-ethnic differences in psychological well-being are inconsistent, but also support the need to explore well-being over the life course in its larger social context. As noted earlier, satisfaction with family and with life in general reveals different patterns compared to other indicators of psychological well-being. Whereas loneliness and depression are measures of affective status—how a person has been feeling in the past week—the satisfaction items in HRS represent more global appraisals. These items do not provide a specific timeframe, and the questions ask for an overall evaluation of satisfaction, not the frequency of a specific feeling. Further, the question about satisfaction with family is the only outcome that is explicitly linked in its content and wording to the main predictors in our models. It is therefore not surprising but quite informative that the two satisfaction measures show the most distinctive patterns of covariates in Table 3 and the most connections with characteristics of motherhood in Table 5. Indeed, nearly all of the parental-marital status groups in Table 3 show lower family satisfaction Coefficient (s.e.) for Potential Mediator Coefficient (s.e.) for Early First Birth Coefficient (s.e.) for Delayed First Birth Coefficient (s.e.) for Currently Unmarried F R2 Panel A: Happiness (NSFH) Basic Modela Grandchild in the home — .071 (.145) .329* (.157) –.637*** (.130) 7.93*** .12 –.472* (.191) .062 (.145) .312 (.156) –.649*** (.129) 7.83*** .13 Coefficient (s.e.) for Delayed First Birth Coefficient (s.e.) for Currently Unmarried Wald chi-square –2 Log Likelihood Panel B: Family Satisfaction (HRS) Coefficient (s.e.) for Potential Mediator Coefficient (s.e.) for Early First Birth — Basic Modela –.180+ (.103) –.170* (.075) –.673*** (.073) 188.16*** 5756.87 (.021) –.198+ (.104) –.141+ (.078) –.673*** (.073) 189.74*** 5756.87 Youngest child under 22 –.137+ (.082) –.187+ (.103) –.147+ (.076) –.676*** (.073) 190.60*** 5756.87 Has been a single mother with child(ren) under 18 –.241** (.077) –.150 (.103) –.184* (.075) –.556*** (.082) 196.94*** 5756.87 First birth occurred outside of marriage –.198+ (.118) –.157 (.104) –.175* (.075) –.663*** (.074) 190.66*** 5756.87 Grandchild in the home –.335** (.115) –.174+ (.103) –.182* (.075) –.655*** (.074) 195.52*** 5756.87 Number of children ever born .028 322 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Table 5. Potential Mediators of the Relationship between Timing of First Birth and Psychosocial Well-Being of Mothers (Aged 51-61): National Survey of Families and Households (1987-88) and the Health and Retirement Study (1992)a Coefficient (s.e.) for Potential Mediator Coefficient (s.e.) for Early First Birth Coefficient (s.e.) for Delayed First Birth Coefficient (s.e.) for Currently Unmarried F R2 Panel C: Loneliness (NSFH) Basic Modela .484* (.214) .018 (.192) –.509* (.203) .818*** (.169) 8.00*** .11 .016 (.191) –.567** (.204) .774*** (.170) 7.80*** .12 Panel D: Depression (NSFH) Basic Modela — .605 (1.633) –3.971* (1.709) 2.377 (1.439) 11.15*** .16 Youngest child under 18 3.281+ (1.813) .665 (1.631) –4.315* (1.717) 2.106 (1.444) 10.53*** .16 Grandchild in the home 4.737* .702 (1.629) –4.187* (1.710) 2.299 (1.437) 10.55*** .16 (2.142) Panel E: Depression (HRS) Basic Modela .112 (.265) .130 (.189) 1.578*** (.193) 138.78*** .25 Youngest child under 22 .405+ — (.211) .134 (.265) .061 (.193) 1.585*** (.194) 127.59*** .25 Child under 18 at home .357 (.287) .122 (.265) .096 (.191) 1.584*** (.194) 127.37*** .25 aModels include controls for age, race, being unmarried, education, health, current work status, and logged household income. These basic models, including the coefficients and standard errors for control variables, are shown in Table 4. +.10 < p <= .05. *.05 < p <= .01. **p < .01. ***p <= .001. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE Youngest child under 18 — / 323 324 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN compared to the normative married mothers. The exceptions are the currently and formerly married childless women (who are no different from the normative married mothers), though both never- and formerly married childless women report lower satisfaction with life in general. Among mothers, being unmarried is highly predictive of lower satisfaction with both family and life in general. Single motherhood and having a non-marital first birth both reduce family satisfaction and help to explain the lower satisfaction among early mothers. Having been a single mother with minor children also substantially reduces the difference among the currently unmarried, though the predictor remains highly significant. For delayed mothers, lower fertility and having a child under 22 help to explain lower family satisfaction. Having a grandchild in the home is independently related to lower satisfaction, and, interestingly, controlling for grandchild coresidence further reduces the odds of high family satisfaction for delayed mothers. These results indicate that current demands and earlier characteristics of motherhood are important components of mothers’ appraisals of family satisfaction, but are less connected to the specific, time-limited, affective measures of psychological status (i.e., loneliness and depression). Overall, the relatively weak links between psychological well-being and maternal status and timing are noteworthy given the social pressures experienced by these cohorts, who were of child-bearing age during the post-war baby boom. On the other hand, the stronger connections with family and life satisfaction suggest that “non-normative” statuses may be salient components of women’s appraisals of themselves and their circumstances in midlife. Although current factors, such as marital status, health, and full-time employment, appear to influence conventional measures of psychological well-being most strongly, our results indicate that maternal timing and childless status may be important in retrospective self-assessments and need to be understood within the sociohistorical contexts experienced by different cohorts. That said, it is striking that the observed maternal status and timing differences are quite small despite the strong family ideologies and overt pressures experienced by men and women of these cohorts (Coontz, 2005; May, 1988, 1995). The very conformity within these cohorts (Stevens, 1990; Uhlenberg, 1974) suggests that “early” or “delayed” child-bearing within the narrow range of maternal ages may represent relatively small deviations from the modal behavior. Further, the quartiles based on the distribution of maternal timing may not reflect actual perceptions of “off-time” behavior (Marini, 1984). Indeed, Fallo-Mitchell and Ryff (1982) have noted that preferred ages for family transitions have generally been greater than actual ages, particularly among the early-marrying cohorts we study here. Finally, our analyses are guided by historical evidence that public images and discourse have emphasized universal (and often idealized), white middle-class standards against which non-conforming behaviors have been judged (May, 1988, 1995). However, recent research has increasingly noted variations in social norms about marriage and fertility related to social class and its intersection with WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 325 racial minority status (see for example, Edin & Kefalas, 2005; Geronimus, 2003). Focusing on modal behaviors in a population may mask or ignore important subgroup variations in social norms (Marini, 1984) and in the meanings and consequences of marital and fertility timing. At the same time, however, Geronimus (2003) has argued that macro-level beliefs and expectations have been very powerful in problematizing early motherhood despite sub-group norms that may define it as adaptive or desirable. While beyond the scope of the current analyses, we encourage future research that examines more local or specific sub-group variations in normative expectations, their interplay with larger societal norms, and their relative importance for well-being throughout the life course and in midlife. A major strength of the current research is its use of multiple data sets and multiple outcome measures. Parallel analyses allow us to confirm strong relationships (e.g., the impact of single motherhood) while also tempering the less consistent differences. The smaller NSFH sample size in particular provides for more conservative results compared to the much larger HRS survey. Emerging new data, including the expanded HRS sample and new waves of both surveys, will allow for further examination of these questions with more current data and successive age cohorts. Despite the strengths of the two surveys, questions remain about the quality of the outcome measures. We acknowledge that singleitem measures are problematic; in particular, the absence of positive well-being measures with stronger psychometric properties is a limitation in both surveys. Future surveys that combine detailed data on life course, family, and household characteristics with a wider range of psychological well-being measures will be crucial for understanding how social statuses and life course experiences influence psychological well-being. In conclusion, the current research provides some insights into the salience of earlier life transitions for women in midlife—specifically, whether and when they became mothers. Social changes in attitudes and behaviors have increased the diversity of life paths and have altered the meanings and implications of different life course experiences. At the same time, the proliferation of Internet sites, discussion groups, and popular books on relationships, marriage, and childbearing or remaining childless/childfree underscores their continued social and personal significance, especially in women’s lives. Our research has focused on an earlier set of cohorts, whose lives were shaped in many ways by more structured social circumstances (Dannefer, 2003). Further research is needed to explicate the relative impact of cohort experiences, social changes in norms and attitudes, and subjective evaluations on the well-being of successive cohorts entering midlife and old age. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS An earlier version of this article was presented at the Annual Meetings of the Gerontological Society of America, November 2003, in San Diego. The authors 326 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN thank Elaine Wethington, Deborah Carr, Connie Shehan, and John Henretta for helpful comments and suggestions. This research was conducted with funding support from the University of Florida Provost’s Opportunity Fund and the Department of Sociology, University of Florida. Tyson Brown’s participation has been supported through an NRSA fellowship from the National Institute on Aging and Traineeship at the Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, with prior support at the University of Florida from an AARP Andrus Foundation Scholarship, AGHE Emerging Scholars Award, and Graduate Minority Fellowship, University of Florida. The HRS (Health and Retirement Study) is sponsored by the National Institute on Aging (grant number NIA U01AG009740) and is conducted by the University of Michigan. The first wave of the NSFH (National Survey of Families and Households) was funded by a grant (HD21009) from the Center for Population Research of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The survey was designed and carried out at the Center for Demography and Ecology at the University of Wisconsin-Madison under the direction of Larry Bumpass and James Sweet. The field work was done by the Institute for Survey Research at Temple University. REFERENCES Alexander, B., Rubinstein, R. L., Goodman, M., & Luborsky, M. (1992). A path not taken: A cultural analysis of regrets and childlessness in the lives of older women. The Gerontologist, 32, 618-626. Anderson, K. (1981). Wartime women: Sex roles, family relations, and the status of women during World War II. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Anderton, D. L., Tsuya, N. O., Bean, L. L., & Mineau, G. (1987). Intergenerational transmission of relative fertility and life course patterns. Demography, 24, 467-480. Barrett, A. E. (2000). Marital trajectories and mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 451-464. Bell, J. E., & Eisenberg, N. (1985). Life satisfaction in midlife childless and empty-nest men and women. Lifestyles: A Journal of Changing Patterns, 7, 146-155. Brim, O. G., Jr., & Ryff, C. D. (1980). On the properties of life events. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 367-388). New York: Academic Press. Burton, R. P. D. (1998). Global integrative meaning as a mediating factor in the relationship between social roles and psychological distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 39, 201-215. Cait, C. A. (2004). Spiritual and religious transformations in women who were parentally bereaved as adolescents. Omega, 49, 163-181. Carr, D. (2004). “My daughter has a career; I just raised babies”: The psychological consequences of women’s intergenerational comparisons. Social Psychological Quarterly, 67, 132-154. Choi, N. G. (2002). Self-defined retirement status and engagement in paid work among older working-age women: Comparison between childless women and mothers. Sociological Inquiry, 72, 43-71. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 327 Clausen, J. A. (1972). The life course of individuals. In M. W. Riley, M. Johnson, & A. Foner (Eds.), Aging and society, Vol. 3: A sociology of age stratification (pp. 457-514). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Connidis, I. A., & McMullin, J. A. (1993). To have or have not: Parent status and the subjective well-being of older men and women. The Gerontologist, 33, 630-636. Cooney, T. M., Pedersen, F. A., Indelicato, S., & Palkovitz, R. (1993). Timing of fatherhood: Is on-time optimal? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 205-215. Coontz, S. (1992). The way we never were: American families and the nostalgia trap. New York: Basic Books. Coontz, S. (2005). Marriage, a history. New York: Viking. Dannefer, D. (2003). Toward a global geography of the life course: Challenges of late modernity for life course theory. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course. New York: Kluwer. Easterlin, R. A. (1980). Birth and fortune: The impact of numbers on personal welfare. New York: Basic Books. Edin, K., & Kefalas, M. (2005). Promises I can keep: Why poor women put motherhood before marriage. Berkeley: University of California Press. Elder, G. H., Jr., (1974). Children of the Great Depression: Social change in life experience. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: W. W. Norton. Epstein, R. H. (2003). Emotions, fertility, and the 1940s woman. Journal of Public Health Policy, 24, 195-211. Evenson, R. J., & Simon, R. W. (2005). Clarifying the relationship between parenthood and depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46, 341-358. Fallo-Mitchell, L., & Ryff, C. D. (1982). Preferred timing of female life events: Cohort differences. Research on Aging, 4, 249-267. Farnham, M., & Lundberg, F. (1947). The modern woman: The lost sex. New York: Harper. Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Morgan, S. P. (1987). Adolescent mothers in later life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gee, E. M. (1990). Preferred timing of women’s life events: A Canadian study. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 34, 279-294. Geronimus, A. T. (2003). Damned if you do: Culture, identity, privilege, and teenage childbearing in the United States. Social Science and Medicine, 57, 881-893. Geronimus, A. T., & Korenman, S. (1993). The socioeconomic costs of teenage childbearing: Evidence and interpretation. Demography, 30, 281-290. Glenn, N. D., & McLanahan, S. (1981). The effects of offspring on the psychological well-being of older adults. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 409-421. Hareven, T. K., & Masaoka, K. (1988). Turning points and transitions: Perceptions of the life course. Journal of Family History, 13, 271-289. Harvey, B. (1993). The fifties: A women’s oral history. New York: HarperCollins. Health and Retirement Study, public use dataset. (1992). Produced and distributed by the University of Michigan with funding from the National Institute on Aging (grant number NIA U01AG009740). Ann Arbor, MI. Heuser, R. L. (1976). Fertility tables for birth cohorts by color: United States, 1917-73. Rockville, MD: U.S. Dept. of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, National Center for Health Statistics. 328 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Houseknecht, S. (1977). Reference group support for voluntary childlessness: Evidence for conformity. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 285-292. Ireland, M. S. (1993). Reconceiving women: Separating motherhood from female identity. New York: Guilford Press. Jeffries, S., & Konnert, C. (2002). Regret and psychological well-being among voluntarily and involuntarily childless women and mothers. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 54, 89-106. Juster, F. T., & Suzman, R. (1995). An overview of the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Human Resources, 30, S7-S56. Kessler, R. C., Foster, C., Webster, P. S., & House, J. S. (1992). The relationship between age and depressive symptoms in two national surveys. Psychology and Aging, 7, 119-126. Keyes, C. L. M., & Ryff, C. D. (1999). Psychological well-being in midlife. In S. L. Willis & J. D. Reid (Eds.), Life in the middle: Psychological and social development in middle age. San Diego: Academic Press. King, R. B. (2003). Subfecundity and anxiety in a nationally representative sample. Social Science and Medicine, 56, 739-751. Koropeckyj-Cox, T. (1998a). Loneliness and depression in middle and old age: Are the childless more vulnerable? Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 53B, S302-S312. Koropeckyj-Cox, T. (1998b). The conditional benefits of parenthood (or childlessness) for subjective well-being in older adulthood. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania. Koropeckyj-Cox, T. (2002). Beyond parental status: Psychological well-being in middle and old age. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 957-971. Land, H., Hudson, S. M., & Steifel, B. (2003). Stress and depression among HIV-positive and HIV-negative gay and bisexual AIDS caregivers. AIDS and Behavior, 7, 41-53. Landry-Meyer, L., & Newman, B. M. (2004). An exploration of the grandparent caregiver role. Journal of Family Issues, 25, 1005-1025. Lang, S. S. (1991). Women without children: The reasons, the rewards, the regrets. Holbrook, MA: Adams Media Corporation. Marini, M. M. (1984). Age and sequencing norms in the transition to adulthood. Social Forces, 63, 229-244. Marks, N., Lambert, J., & Choi, H. (2002). Transitions to caregiving, gender, and psychological well-being: A prospective U.S. study. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 657-668. May, E. T. (1988). Homeward bound: American families in the Cold War era. New York: Basic Books. May, E. T. (1995). Barren in the promised land: Childless Americans and the pursuit of happiness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. McQuillan, J., Griel, A. L., White, L., & Jacob, M. C. (2003). Frustrated fertility: Infertility and psychological distress among women. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 1007-1018. Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2002). Depression, parenthood, and age at first birth. Social Science and Medicine, 54, 1281-1298. Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1992). Age and depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 187-205. WOMEN OF THE 1950s AND “NORMATIVE” LIFE COURSE / 329 Mitchell, V., & Helson, R. (1990). Women’s prime of life: Is it the 50s? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14, 451-470. Modell, J., Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., & Hershberg, T. (1976). Social change and transitions to adulthood in historical perspective. Journal of Family History, 1, 7-32. Moen, P. (2001). The gendered life course. In R. H. Binstock & L. K. George (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the life course (pp. 179-196). San Diego: Academic Press. Moore, K. A., & Sugland, B. W. (1999). Piecing together the puzzle of teenage childbearing. Policy and Practice, 57, 36-42. Neugarten, B. L. (1968). Middle age and aging: A reader in social psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Neugarten, B. L. (1979). Time, age, and the life cycle. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 887-894. Neugarten, B. L., & Datan, N. (1973). Sociological perspectives on the life cycle. In P. B. Baltes & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Life span and developmental psychology: Personality and socialization (pp. 53-79). New York: Academic Press. Neugarten, B. J., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. (1965). Age norms, age constraints, and adult socialization. American Journal of Sociology, 70, 710-717. O’Rand, A. M., & Henretta, J. C. (1982). Women at middle age: Developmental transitions. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 464, 57-64. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. Rempel, J. (1985). Childless elderly: What are they missing? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47, 343-348. Rindfuss, R. R., & Bumpass, L. L. (1976). How old is too old? Age and the sociology of fertility. Family Planning Perspectives, 8, 226-230. Rindfuss, R. R., Morgan, S. P., & Swicegood, C. G. (1988). First births in America. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Ryff, C. D., Lee, Y. H., Essex, M. J., & Schmutte, P. S. (1994). My children and me: Midlife evaluations of grown children and self. Psychology and Aging, 6, 195-205. Settersten, R. A., & Hagestad, G. O. (1996a). What’s the latest? Cultural age deadlines for family transitions. The Gerontologist, 36, 178-188. Settersten, R. A., & Hagestad, G. O. (1996b). What’s the latest? II. Cultural age deadlines for educational and work transitions. The Gerontologist, 36, 602-613. Simon, R. W. (1992). Parental role strains, salience of parental identity and gender differences in psychological distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 25-35. Stevens, D. A. (1990). New evidence on the timing of early life course transitions: The United States 1990 to 1980. Journal of Family History, 15, 163-178. Sweet, J., Bumpass, L., & Call, V. (1988). The design and content of the National Survey of Families and Households. NSFH Working Paper 1, University of Wisconsin, Madison. Thoits, P. A. (1992). Identity structures and psychological well-being: Gender and marital status comparisons. Social Psychological Quarterly, 55, 236-256. Turner, M. J., Killian, T. S., & Cain, R. (2004). Life course transitions and depressive symptoms among women in midlife. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 58, 241-265. Uhlenberg, P. (1974). Cohort variations in family life cycle experiences of U.S. females. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 36, 284-292. 330 / KOROPECKYJ-COX, PIENTA AND BROWN Veevers, J. E. (1980). Childless by choice. Toronto: Butterworths. Veroff, J., Douvan, E., & Kulka, R. A. (1981). The inner American: A self-portrait from 1957 to 1976. New York: Basic Books. Vissing, Y. (2002). Women without children: Nurturing lives. New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. Wheaton, B. (1990). Life transitions, role histories, and mental health. American Sociological Review, 55, 209-223. Whelpton, P. K., Campbell, A. A., & Patterson, J. E. (1966). Fertility and family planning in the United States. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Wight, R. G. (2000). Precursive depression among HIV infected AIDS caregivers over time. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 759-770. Wilmoth, J., & Koso, G. (2002). Does marital history matter? Marital status and wealth outcomes among preretirement adults. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 254-268. Wu, L. L. (1996). Effects of family instability, income, and income instability on risk of premarital birth. American Sociological Review, 61, 386-406. Direct reprint requests to: Tanya Koropeckyj-Cox Department of Sociology University of Florida P.O. Box 117330 3219 Turlington Hall Gainesville, FL 32611-7330 e-mail: tkcox@soc.ufl.edu