See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/354065550 English as a Foreign Language International Journal Article · July 2021 CITATIONS READS 0 4,559 1 author: Napak-On Sritrakarn Rajamangala University of Technology Isan Khon Khan Campus 9 PUBLICATIONS 27 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Napak-On Sritrakarn on 23 August 2021. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. English as a Foreign Language International Journal (EFLIJ) Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 Published by the Academics Education International Journals English as a Foreign Language International Journal A Division of AEIJ Part of TESOL One www.academics.education/eflij © English as a Foreign Language International Journal 2021 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of the Academics Education International Journals. No unauthorized photocopying All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying or otherwise, without the prior written permission of the Academics Education International Journals. editor@academics.education Publisher: Academics Education International Journals Chief Editor: Dr. John Adamson Guest and Production Editor: Dr. Jun (Scott) Chen Hsieh ISSN: 2799-0699 (Online) Table of Contents Foreword by Jun (Scott) Chen Hsieh.……………..…....………………..….…...... 1-2 1. Napak-on Sritrakarn ….………………...........………………..……................... 3-25 - The Effects of Anonymity and Non-Anonymity on Students’ Writing Reviews and Corrections 2. Omar Karlin & Sayaka Karlin ……..………………………..….…………….... 26-54 - Comparing the Effectiveness of L2 Extensive and Intensive Listening Approaches 3. Suthathip Thirakunkovit ………………………….………..………………...…. 55-83 - Production and Perception of Geminate Consonants in English Words by Thai Learners of English: Implications for English Teaching and Learning Book Reviews 1. English-medium Instruction and Translanguaging ………………………………. 84-86 BethAnne Palsrud, Tian, Zhongfeng and Jeanette Toth (Eds). Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 2020. ISBN 13: 978-1-78892-731-4 (pbk). Pp. 208. Reviewed by Marilyn Lewis, Honorary Research Fellow, The University of Auckland 2. Critical Thinking in ELT: A Working Model for the Classroom ………………… 87-89 Dummett and John Hughes. National Geographic Learning: Boston, USA, 2019. Pp. viii +158. Reviewed by Thiri Soe, Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University, Japan EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 1 July 2021 Foreword Welcome to the July issue of English as a Foreign Language International Journal in 2021. Including three research articles and two book reviews, this issue explores diverse topics essential to the field of teaching and learning English as a second/foreign language. Specifically, the research articles address anonymity and non-anonymity in writing, extensive and intensive listening approaches, and geminate consonants in English words. The July issue starts with “The Effects of Anonymity and Non-Anonymity on Students’ Writing Reviews and Corrections” by Napak-on Sritrakarn. In comparing the effects of employing the senior review activity in two modes of anonymous and non-anonymous reviews, Napak-on Sritrakarn observed students’ positive attitudes toward the mode of reviews and more constructive comments on junior students’ writing from senior students in the anonomous mode. Implications of a review activity in a writing classroom as well as suggestions for future studies are provided. Omar Karlin and Sayaka Karlin, in “Comparing the Effectiveness of L2 Extensive and Intensive Listening Approaches,” engaged three groups of Japanese university students in six listening assignments adhering to either principles of extensive or intensive listening. Based on the pretest and post-test of 100-question TOEIC listening tests, the researchers found the advantages of the extensive listening approach over the intensive listening approach. Implicatons and suggestions include codifying extenstive listening and intensive listening methodologies, ensuring variability in testing conditions, and scrutinizing the participants' adherence to the intervention. In the third article, entitled “Production and Perception of Geminate Consonants in English Words by Thai Learners of English: Implications for English Teaching and Learning,” Suthathip Thirakunkovit explored the production and perception of geminate consonants of Thai learners of English. By examining the tasks as reading aloud and dictation, Suthathip Thirakunkovit identified sifnificant differences in the production of lexical geminates between native and non-native English speakers. Low intermediate students seem to find identifying English geminates in regular speech challenging. This study paves the way for future research concerning geminate consonant production and perception in ESL/EFL contexts. 1 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 2 Marilyn Lewis and Thiri Soe, respectively reviewing “English-medium Instruction and Translanguaging” and “Critical Thinking in ELT: A Working Model for the Classroom”, succinctly convey the contents and value of the books with a brief summary of the book’s content and attention to its main focus, thus facilitating the readers’ understanding of EMI, translanguaging, and critical thinking. We hope you find the articles in this July 2021 issue to be informative, inspiring, and comprehensive. Bearing in mind the contribution to continuous improvement in English language instruction around the world, particulary in times of Covid-19 pandemic, we sincerely hope that this issue helps provide new insights into the formulation of future research and innovations for EFL/ESL practitioners in cross-border, interdisciplinary, and collaborative manners. We would like to express our sincere appreciation to the contributors and reviewers of articles and book reviews who have made this issue possible. Dr. Jun (Scott) Chen Hsieh Department of Foreign Languages and Literature Asia University, Taiwan Guest and Production Editor of the English as a Foreign Language International Journal (EFLIJ) July 2021 Issue 2 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 3 The Effects of Anonymity and Non-Anonymity on Students’ Writing Reviews and Corrections Napak-on Sritrakarn Rajamangala University of Technology Isan, Khon Kaen Campus, Thailand Bio data Napak-on Sritrakarn is an Associate Professor at the Department of English for International Communication, Faculty of Technical Education, Rajamangala University of Technology Isan, Khon Kaen Campus, Thailand. She received her Ph.D. from Macquarie University, Australia. Her research interests cover English Language Teaching (ELT), academic literacy, discourse analysis, and genre studies. napak-on.sr@rmuti.ac.th Abstract This study compared the effects of employing the senior review activity in two modes of anonymous and non-anonymous reviews. There were seven senior students who have made reviews on the essay writing drafts of two junior classes on the same topic. It was found that the two groups of junior students had positive attitudes toward the mode of reviews they participated in and that senior students had made more constructive comments on junior students’ writing when their names were not disclosed. The findings on senior students’ attitudes also informed that even though they had no problems with the review conditions of either being anonymous or non-anonymous, they preferred to know whose work they were reviewing. The paper discusses some implications for the application of a review activity in a writing classroom as well as directions for future studies. Keywords: writing reviews; anonymity; non-anonymity Correspondence address: Rajamangala University of Technology Isan, Khon Kaen Campus Address: 150 Srichan Road, Muang, Khon Kaen, 40000 Thailand. 3 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 4 Introduction English has long been used as a global language for communication worldwide, and the proficient use of it as a language tool is of imperative importance (Nur, 2020; Rajab et al., 2020; Vallente, 2020). Even though the requirements of its command are high, many Thai learners at different levels have difficulties in learning the language (Arjpru, 2017; Foley, 2005; Noomura, 2013; Sritrakarn, 2018; Sweeney, Kunyot, & Preedeekul, 2017; Viriya, 2018). Not only that students find it difficult to master the skills in English, but teachers also find teaching English challenging as well. Baiyaem (1997) reported that the obstacles faced by teachers include heavy teaching loads and insufficient language skills and teaching knowledge. For students, numerous studies claimed that the challenges for their learning are such as the lack of opportunity to use English in real life (Noom-ura, 2013; Viriya, 2018; Wiriyachitra, 2003), being passive learners (Arjpru, 2017; Viriya, 2018), and uninteresting lessons (Viriya, 2018). Especially for many Thais as well as other Asian learners, the culture issue could be one of the barriers for students to learn the English language (Chen, 2019; Raymond & Choon, 2017). Findings from previous studies show that many foreign language learners have anxiety in learning a language (Akbari & Sadeghi, 2013; Dordinejad & Ahmadabad, 2014). They are shy to communicate in English with their classmates (Arjpru, 2017; Vallente, 2020). Some even intend to make efforts to maintain friendly relationships with peers by trying to avoid confrontation in the interaction (Chen, 2019) while others may avoid asking questions for fear of being incorrect or feel embarrassed (Raymond & Choon, 2017). As a consequence, they engage less and display a passive role in class. These reflect serious problems as a result of cultural impacts. Given that exposure to the language through active participation in class is one of the key factors for successful English language learning, by avoiding to interact or participate, their chances for improvement could be limited. This may thereby result in unproductive learning outcomes. In order to overcome the abovementioned problems faced by both teachers and students in language learning and teaching, it is essential for the teacher to design a classroom learning activity that encourages students to interact actively using the language, take more control of their own learning, and work cooperatively to accomplish the task (Altun & Sabah, 2020). By focusing on the improvement of Thai students’ writing, the present study employed anonymous and non-anonymous review activities in an essay writing classroom. The following section discusses the literature related to the current study. 4 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 5 Literature Review Writing Writing is the act of putting down the graphic symbols that present a language in order to convey some meaning so that the reader can grasp the information which the writer has tried to impart (Sapkota, 2012). It is a key factor in students’ academic development and the primary means by which students transform from passive recipients of knowledge to active participants in their own education (Ruiling & Bol, 2007). Writing is a complex process that requires both language and many other skills to accomplish. For language skills, Watcharapunyawong and Usaha (2013) argued that writing requires both syntactic and semantic knowledge. For other skills, writing also requires a great deal of effort to continuously practice (ibid.). This also includes mental, psychological, rhetorical, and critical skills (Sapkota, 2012). As far as the nature of communication is concerned, writing and speaking skills are different in that while spoken language is context-dependent, the writer and reader share neither verbal nor aural contact in written language. In other words, the language is used independently from the context in writing, and the structure is organized in a monologic way with a diversity of lexical choices relevant to the topic contents (Piriyasilpa, 2009). For these reasons, many students find writing skill to be the most challenging in real practice and in the examination (Sapkota, 2012; Zakaria & Hashim, 2020), and this problem exists even among native English speaking learners (Lu & Bol, 2007). In order to support students to compose their English essays more successfully, a number of strategies have been employed in the language classrooms; and one of the common techniques is the peer review activity Peer review When students critique one another’s work with the intention of helping their peers revise and eventually improve their writing, the process is called peer review (Ruiling & Bol, 2007). Peer review is also defined by Waemusa (2017, p. 93) as “a learning process whereby students engage in communication for work evaluation and reflection.” According to Hosack (2003), peer review involves learners reading one another’s draft compositions and providing feedback that can be used by the writer when revising the drafts. Based on these definitions, the concepts of peer review can be summed up as a kind of cooperative learning activity in which students read the writing drafts of their peers and provide feedback with the goal to help revise and improve the drafts. Peer review can be both verbal and in written form. Even though the feedback made by students is suspected by Truscot (1996), other studies have argued that peer review has become commonplace in the writing environment (Ruiling & Bol, 2007) and the 5 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 6 activity is a central aspect of English as a Second Language (ESL) and English as a Foreign Language (EFL) writing programs across the world (Hyland & Hyland, 2006). Colpitts (2016) argues that allowing students to engage in peer editing of one another’s work is surely not as accurate as having the teacher do so, but it does reduce workloads for time-constrained educators. Previous studies have conducted peer review activity in the language classrooms and claimed for numerous benefits of the activity. First of all, the peer review process promotes critical thinking and the awareness of effective writing skills (Kunwongse, 2013). It allows students to analyze and provide constructive feedback on their peers’ writing (Ngoc lac & Gurung, 2015) before making a reflection on their own work (Hosak, 2003). Evidence of this has been shown in the findings from the study by Colpitts (2016), in which student participants considered that both giving and receiving feedback from other students helped with the act of “noticing” and the act of “discovery.” That is to say, by analyzing their peers’ work critically, students are encouraged to reconsider and adjust their own writing. For those who receive the feedback, they are more attuned to the needs and expectations of the readers. This whole process enables students to interact analytically and promotes cognitive processing, which results in the ‘durability of memory’ (Houston, 2001, p. 270) and constitutes part of the process of second language learning (Swain, 2005). Secondly, feedback made by peers tend to be pitched close to the learner’s own level of proficiency, so it is potentially more informative to the novice writer than feedback provided by the instructor (Hosak, 2003). The study by Sritrakarn (2018) also showed that students felt comfortable receiving feedback from other students while teacher comments were sometimes confusing and abstract. Finally, peer review activity promotes social interaction and collaborative learning (Lu & Bol, 2007). Given that language learning happens as a result of social interactions (Vygotsky, 1986), learners need to be engaged in the writing environments which enable them to collaborate and interact with peers or adults. By taking an active role in analyzing their peers’ work, and by providing feedback and receiving comments through the peer review activity, students are provided with the opportunity to communicate linguistically and eventually acquire the target language (Swain, 2005). Scholars have also pointed out some problems of peer review activity. Despite the problems in terms of time consumed (Kunwongse, 2013), the two main problems of peer review can be pointed out in terms of feedback quality and cultural issues. To begin, concerns have been made regarding the quality of peer feedback. Studies found that some students who provided feedback have limited knowledge of language and grammar (Wanchid, 2015) and felt they did not have enough English ability themselves to accurately give feedback to others 6 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 7 (Colpitts, 2016). Moreover, those who received the feedback stated that they still preferred the teacher’s feedback to the feedback received from other students (Sritrakarn, 2018). Even though some problems still exist in the application of peer review activity, numerous studies argue that if designed appropriately, the peer review activity can still bring benefits to students’ writing practices. Colpitts (2016), for example, argues that while the students lack the linguistic and grammatical abilities to always correct one another’s work with confidence, they still felt a sense of personal growth in terms of their own English writing ability occurring during the process of giving and receiving peer feedback. Therefore, it is essential for the study on peer review activity to be aware of those aforementioned problems and design the activity systematically to overcome the problems. Another problem that has been mentioned in a number of studies is related to cultural issues, especially if identifiable peer review activity is conducted. Evidence from studies conducted in Asian learning contexts show that cultural norms can restrain students’ level of collaborative learning (Chen 2019; Cote, 2007; Lu & Bol, 2007; Waemuza, 2017; Wanchid, 2015). Scholars found that some learners have difficulty providing feedback because they are reluctant to criticize their classmates’ work (Hosac, 2003; Waemusa, 2017). Raymond and Choon (2007) explain that this is because Asian students are “face conscious” in nature (p. 198). They believe that one should be modest and should not put someone else to shame (ibid.). As a result, they tend to maintain a harmonious relationship in the classroom and avoid challenging others by asking questions or expressing their different thoughts on the discussed issue. In addition to these cultural factors, other related factors that have the potential to influence students to be easily biased or not honest in giving feedback are such as friendship, gender, race, interpersonal relationships, or personal preferences (Lu & Bol, 2007). This implies that cultural issues as well as learners’ identities are vulnerable during the language learning process and can be at risk if students are exposed to direct criticism or forced to be critical of their peers (Silver & Coomber, 2010). The discussion also raises the question of whether or not the undisclosed identity would help to promote more interaction and successful language learning among students. By following the concepts of using ‘senior’ as a replacement for ‘peer’ (Sritrakarn, 2018), the present study aims to enhance the quality of feedback made in the review activity. Sritrakarn (2018) claimed that when working on the task with the assistance of experienced adults, students could refine knowledge and build up further from their existing knowledge and understanding of the subject matter. In this context of the investigation, the selected senior students have undertaken the English essay writing course. They, therefore, had the experience 7 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 8 in learning and composing the essay to review the writing drafts of junior students. To examine whether or not learner identities have an influence on the senior students’ reviews and the junior students’ corrections, anonymous and non-anonymous review activities were conducted. The following section discusses further the concepts of anonymity and its benefits. Anonymity Scholars define anonymity as the learning and teaching activity in which both reviewers and reviewees are kept unknown to one another (Lu & Bol, 2007), or learners’ real identities are kept unknown to others by using pseudonyms or not using names at all (Chen, 2009). Anonymity has been employed in different language classrooms of both native-speaking contexts (Chen, 2009; Lu & Bol, 2007) and non-native speaking contexts (Cote, 2014; Hosack, 2003; Jessup, Connolly & Tansik, 1990; Silver & Coomber, 2010). Based on the findings from these previous studies, the benefits of anonymity in language learning can be pointed out in three main aspects: deindividuation, interpersonal relationship, and more critical feedback. First of all, anonymity contributes to deindividuation, which was defined by Lu & Bol (2007) as the situations in which individuals in groups stop thinking of other members as individuals and feel that they cannot be singled out by others. Anonymity fosters the level of comfort in language learning. When students feel that they cannot be singled out by others, they stop thinking of other members as an individual, resulting in a reduction of normal inner restraints and enabling group members to engage in behavior that they would not ordinarily display (Jessup et al., 1990). In this way, they participate and contribute more to the learning process. Evidence of this has been shown in the study by Chen (2019), which investigated the impacts of anonymity on international students’ learning performances and found that deindividuation promotes less peer pressure and that international students who have diverse backgrounds suffer less from the social constraints and feel more comfortable to participate. Secondly, anonymity in peer review promotes interpersonal relationships among the participants. By nature, Asian students tend to be reluctant to criticize peers’ work (Hosack, 2003). They make an effort to maintain friendly relationships with peer students (Chen, 2019). So, they may try to avoid confrontations or conflicts with friends in face-to-face peer review activities. When staying low profile in the peer review activity, students make their comments less aggressive, allowing for a higher level of participation and production in peer interaction (ibid.). Finally, anonymous review enhances more critical feedback. Anonymous reviewers were more honest with their comments and provided more critical feedback than those who 8 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 9 conducted peer review identifiably (Lu & Bol, 2007). Cote (2014) adds that the feedback provided anonymously is based solely on the text itself, not its author, because the reviewers do not know whose essay they are reviewing. The activity, therefore, provides a comfort zone for sharing critical feedback because students do not feel worried about their comments on their peers (Waemosa, 2017), and the students thereby are more honest in their critique and provide feedback without bias regardless of attitude toward the experience. Even though there have been numerous studies employing peer feedback review in the Thai language classrooms (Sapkota, 2012; Sritrakarn, 2018; Wanchid, 2015), there has still been insufficient research comparing the use of anonymity and non-anonymity. Especially, the use of senior students instead of junior students has been very rare. To add more findings in the previous studies, the present study employed anonymous and non-anonymous review activities in an essay writing classroom. The study seeks to answer the research questions below. 1. What are the effects of anonymity and non-anonymity on the quality of junior students’ writing drafts? 2. How does anonymity or non-anonymity affect the reviews made by senior students? 3. What are senior and junior students’ attitudes toward the use of anonymity in peer review? Method Senior students In this study, junior students’ writing drafts were revised by the same group of senior students. Initially, eight senior students (one male and seven females) were selected to provide feedback on junior students’ essay writing. These students, majoring in English and were in their fourth year, have passed the Essay Writing Course and were undertaking the Argumentative Writing Course. By considering the potential problems regarding the non-systemic process of senior students’ selection (see Sritrakarn, 2018), the senior participants in the current study were selected using the purposive-sampling technique. In so doing, the students’ TOEIC score reports were considered, and those who have passed the TOEIC at the minimum score of 480 were recruited as the research participants. Later on, one senior student opted out. So, there were seven senior students (one male and six females) who have been selected as the research participants. 9 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 10 Junior students The two groups of junior students (Group A and Group B) majored in English. Students in Group A received feedback on their writing in anonymous mode while Group B students participated in the non-anonymous mode, receiving comments on their work by identifiable senior students. Twenty-three students (four males and nineteen females) in Group A enrolled in the Essay Writing Course in the first semester of 2019, and twenty-six students from Group B (two males and twenty-four females) enrolled in the second semester of the same year. The two groups of students have passed the Paragraph Writing Course, and their average level of English proficiency was pre-intermediate. During the research activity, some of the junior students from both groups did not participate constantly. This resulted in the number of junior participants in this study dropping to sixteen for anonymity (four males and twelve females) and seventeen for non-anonymity (two males and fifteen females). The data collection process Groups A (Anonymous Activity) Prior to the data collection, both junior and senior student groups were trained to get familiar with the Microsoft word ‘review’ system. The knowledge of essay structure was revised to the senior students, and they were trained about the feedback types and how to provide feedback using the ‘review’ system on Microsoft word program. The junior students were also trained on how to track the changes or comments given by senior students, as well as how to accept or reject them using the ‘review’ system. During the training sessions, the teacher ensured that both groups of students had plenty of practice to get used to the ‘review’ tracking system on the Microsoft word program. To collect the data, the teacher has made an announcement of the essay writing topic (Should same-sex marriage be legalized?) to the junior group and stated the deadline to submit the first drafts. After all junior students submitted their first drafts to their class email, the teacher checked emails and edited the names of the students by changing them to pseudonyms. The teacher then emailed the drafts to the class email of the senior group. The senior students would, later on, check emails and select the drafts they were assigned to review and provide feedback. After the reviewed drafts were returned back to the senior class’s email, the teacher changed the draft names back to junior students’ original names and emailed the drafts back to their class email. Junior students were told to return their edited drafts back on the deadline before the teacher changed the edited drafts back to the pseudonym and forwarded them to senior students for the second round of review. After junior students received the second 10 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 11 comments, they edited the drafts before submitting the final drafts to the teacher for marking. The data collection process can be summarized in Figure 1 below. Figure 1. The review process (anonymous review) Group B (Non-Anonymous Activity) Junior students in Group B were also trained about the feedback types and negotiated about the learning goals of the activity. This group of junior students was not trained about the ‘review’ system of the Microsoft word program because they participated in the non-anonymous mode. However, the process of review, as well as deadlines for submission of each draft, were identified, and students were advised that they could consult the senior students directly if there were some questions or any further explanations needed regarding the reviews. The review process can be summarized as follows. Figure 2. The review process (non-anonymous review) As shown in Figure 2, the junior students had the opportunity to further ask questions regarding the comments or seek further advice from senior students during the review process of their first and second drafts before submitting their drafts to the teacher. There was less involvement from the teacher than the anonymous review during the editing process. However, students were advised that they could always consult with the teacher if they were not certain about the comments. 11 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 12 Research instruments In order to investigate the effects of anonymity and non-anonymity on students’ reviews and writing corrections, the data were collected from three sources: students’ writing drafts, Likertscale questionnaires, and group interviews. The junior students’ writing drafts on the topic: “Should same-sex marriage be legalized?” were collected. The drafts composed by those who participated in the entire activity were used for data analysis. After the review activity, the questionnaires (see Table 5 and Table 6) were distributed to both junior and senior student groups to investigate their attitudes toward the activity. The designed questionnaires were sent to three experts in the field for comments and rating for content validity to validate the quality of the questionnaires prior to the distribution. Then the index of item-objective congruence (IOC) was calculated. The reviewers were asked to rate whether the question items were congruent (+1), incongruent (-1), or questionable (0). The items that had scored lower than 0.5 were revised, and the items that had scores higher than or equal to 0.5 were reserved. After changes had been made by the reviewers, they were piloted with students who were at a similar level to the research participants. Then, the reliability values of the two sets of questionnaires were calculated by using Cronbach’s alpha. In this study, the two sets of questionnaires had reliability values of 7.5 (for senior students) and 7.7 (for junior students), which were acceptable and could be used for data collection. To ensure the validity of an interview protocol, an expert in the field was consulted for comments and suggestions of how to make the question items precise and concise. Then changes were made based on the comments. Finally, the senior students and the two groups of junior students were interviewed to investigate their attitudes toward the activity and to follow up as a clarification of some certain issues received from the findings (see Appendix A: Interview Protocol). These questions were flexible to students’ responses in that further questions were sometimes added and asked to clarify the initial response made by junior students to each question. Data analysis The collected data were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively to find the answers to the research questions set above. To answer research question 1, the first and final drafts composed by junior students were compared in terms of essay structure and changes made in their responses to the reviews received from senior students. For research question 2, senior students’ reviews were investigated by classifying the types of feedback. Further analysis of comments 12 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 13 made by senior students was done to see whether similar or different types of comments were provided when senior students’ names were disclosed and hidden. Results Research question 1: What are the effects of anonymity and non-anonymity on the quality of junior students’ writing drafts? Taking a broad view, there was not much difference in the essay structures of the first and final drafts composed by the two groups of junior students. In other words, all of the students were aware of the essay structure and composed their drafts consisting of an introduction, body (with arguments), and conclusion. Further analysis of the first and final drafts of junior students’ writing was made to identify the efficiency of their correction. In so doing, changes in the final drafts as a result of senior students’ reviews in the first drafts were analyzed, and the findings are demonstrated as follows. Table 1 Successful responses to senior students’ comments Total reviews No. 383 % 100 Anonymous reviews Successful Unsuccessful responses responses No. % No. % 337 87.98 46 12.02 Total reviews No. 586 % 100 Non-anonymous reviews Successful Unsuccessful responses responses No. % No. % 532 90.78 54 9.22 As shown in Table 1, while non-anonymity resulted in more corrections in junior students’ final drafts, both types of review led to more successful corrections (87.98% for anonymity and 90.78% for non-anonymity). The two types of review also led to unsuccessful corrections, and these were due to different reasons. The main reasons were tallied and calculated in percentages, as shown in Table 2 below. Table 2 Reasons for unsuccessful responses Reasons Ineffective feedback Original language already correct Junior students’ rejection Junior students’ misunderstanding Adjustment of reviews Total Anonymous review 17 (36.95%) 23 (50.00%) 6 (13.04%) 46 Non-anonymous review 27 (50.00%) 2 (3.70%) 18 (33.33%) 5 (9.25%) 2 (3.70%) 54 Table 2 shows that the unsuccessful corrections of junior students were influenced by different reasons. When examined closely, however; most of the unsuccessful corrections were caused by senior students (86.95% in anonymous review and 53.70% in non-anonymous review), by 13 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 14 either providing ineffective feedback or making changes of the statements which were already correct. While there were only three reasons which affected the unsuccessful corrections made by junior students in the anonymous review activity, the reasons for unsuccessful corrections in non-anonymity varied. This raises the question of whether or not the mode of review has the effects on the review quality made by senior students. Further analysis would therefore be made to examine the types and quality of reviews made in the two circumstances. Research question 2: How does anonymity or non-anonymity affect the reviews made by senior students? To identify the effects of anonymity and non-anonymity on senior students’ reviews, the researcher has initially analyzed the types of feedback made on junior students’ writing drafts. Following Sritrakarn (2018), the reviews which affect direct responses or changes in junior students’ final drafts were categorized into four types: direct feedback (when junior students are provided with correct form), indirect feedback (when junior students are reminded that errors exist but correction is not provided), metalinguistic feedback (when some kind of metalinguistic clue as to the nature of the error is provided) and comments (other types of feedback that do not fit in the previous three categories made by senior students and received responses). The findings are presented in Table 3 below. Table 3 Feedback types Frequencies Anonymous review (383 reviews) Non-anonymous review (586 reviews) Feedback types Feedback types Direct Indirect Metalinguistic Comment Direct Indirect Metalinguistic Comment 295 (77.02%) 1 (0.26%) 13 (3.39%) 74 (19.32%) 11 (1.87%) 11 (1.87%) 17 (17.17%) 547 (93.34%) Table 3 shows that most of the reviews made by senior students, both anonymously or nonanonymously, are direct feedback. Especially for the non-anonymous mode, almost all of the reviews were direct feedback (93.34%). In addition to the high frequencies of direct feedback, there were significant numbers of comments made in the reviews. These comments shown in the table (19.32% in anonymity and 17.17% in non-anonymity) are those which received responses from junior students. There have been, however, more comments made by senior students that did not receive explicit responses nor affect changes in the writing. These comments may have an influence on the improvements of junior students’ writing in some way. 14 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 15 Further analysis will therefore be made to investigate all the comments made by the senior students. To do this, the researcher followed the analysis framework of Sritrakarn (2018), and divided the types of comments into compliments, questions to the writers regarding the contents, criticism, and suggestions of how the essay could be better constructed. The findings from the analysis are shown in Table 4 below. Table 4 Types of comments (anonymous) Compliments or support Anonymity Total anonymous reviews = 99 Nonanonymous reviews = 50 Nonanonymity 11 (11.11%) 10 (20%) Comment types Questions to the writer Criticism Suggestions of how the essay regarding the contents could be better constructed Anonymity NonAnonymity NonAnonymity Non-anonymity anonymity anonymity 2 (4.00%) 7 (7.07%) 12 (24.00%) 81 (81.81%) 26 (52.00%) Table 4 shows that senior students made more comments when they were anonymous, and most of their comments were to provide suggestions of how the junior students could improve their drafts (81.81%). There must be infinitive verb after modal verb. (Narisa) To avoid (using) the same word, this one you can cut ‘by’; and make prejudice to be adverb ‘prejudicially’ (Chanchai) The examples above show that senior students took more engagement in the reviewing process and provided more explicit comments on junior students when being anonymous. Moreover, the use of modalities (“must” or “can”) shows their confidence in delivering comments on junior students’ writing. When their names were disclosed, the number of their comments dropped to almost half of those made in the anonymous activity. Moreover, they made comments by providing compliments, asking questions, and criticizing more often when being non-anonymous. Good job!! ☺ (Compliment- Papawadee) And then what is next? (Asking question- Tad-dao) This sentence is too long. (Criticism- Chanchai) 15 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 16 Research question 3: What are senior students’ and junior students’ attitudes toward anonymous and non-anonymous review activity? The third area investigated in this study was the attitude of the participants toward the activity. Questionnaires were distributed to senior students as well as the two groups of junior students, followed by group interviews. The senior students’ attitudes are presented in Table 5 below. Table 5 Senior students’ attitudes Statements 1. It is useful to read and review the junior students’ work. 2. Reviewing junior students’ work helps me improve my own writing. 3. Reviewing junior students’ work helps me give some ideas for my own writing. 4. I am a better reviewer if I know whose work I am reading. 5. I am a better reviewer if I don’t know whose work I am reading. 6. It is difficult to find things to say about the junior students’ writing if my name is disclosed. 7. It is difficult to find things to say about the junior students’ writing even though it is anonymous. 8. In my non-anonymous review, I often tell the junior students about the strengths of their writing. 9. In my anonymous review, I often tell the junior students about the strengths of their writing. 10. In my non-anonymous review, I often tell the junior students about the weaknesses or problems of their writing. 11. In my anonymous review, I often tell the junior students about the weaknesses or problems of their writing. 12. In my non-anonymous review, I often tell the junior students if I don’t understand something they have written. 13. In my anonymous review, I tell the junior students if I don’t understand something they have written. Mean 4.71 4.43 4.71 3.71 3.29 3.00 2.71 4.86 4.29 4.43 S. D 0.49 0.53 0.49 1.25 1.50 1.41 1.50 0.38 0.76 0.79 4.43 0.79 5.00 0.00 4.29 0.76 As shown in Table 5 above, senior students had positive attitudes toward the activity. By indicating the low mean scores in number 6 (It is difficult to find things to say about the junior students’ writing if my name is disclosed.) and number 7 (It is difficult to find things to say about the junior students’ writing even though it is anonymous.), it indicates that senior students had no problems making reviews of junior students’ writing in either anonymous or nonanonymous mode. Even though they had no problems doing reviews with their names disclosed or hidden, the results, however; show that senior students preferred to know whose work they were reviewing as evidenced in their responses to number 4 (I am a better reviewer if I know whose work I am reading. m = 3.71) and number 5 (I am a better reviewer if I don’t know whose work I am reading. m = 3.29). Junior students from both anonymous and non-anonymous groups had positive attitudes toward the activity and found the senior students’ comments useful. Table 6 shows that the anonymous group indicated a low mean score (m = 3.04) when being asked if they wanted to know the name of the reviewer after receiving comments (no. 9). The non-anonymous group, however, showed a high level of agreement on this point (m = 4.14). This shows that, interestingly, each group preferred their way of receiving reviews. 16 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 17 Table 6 Junior students’ attitudes toward anonymous and non-anonymous activity Statements 1. I enjoy receiving the senior students’ comments on my writing. 2. I find the senior students’ comments helpful when I revise my writing. 3. In their comments, the senior students sometimes point out problems with my writing that I didn’t notice. 4. It is not useful if the senior students say only good things about my writing. 5. I sometimes disagree with the senior students’ comments about my writing. 6. The senior students’ comments help to make my ideas clearer for the reader. 7. The senior students’ comments help me improve the organization of my writing. 8. The senior students’ comments help me improve the language of my writing. 9. When I receive comments on my writing, I prefer to know who has written them. Anonymous Mean S. D 3.96 0.36 4.08 0.65 Non-anonymous Mean S. D 4.36 0.49 4.36 0.66 4.04 0.69 4.41 0.80 3.83 1.05 4.09 0.81 3.25 1.03 2.77 1.07 4.00 0.66 4.00 0.76 4.08 0.58 4.23 0.75 4.04 0.75 4.29 0.81 3.04 1.40 4.14 0.99 Discussion The findings from this study have provided some implications for language learning and teaching in Thailand and other EFL learning contexts, in particular for the application of the peer review activity. These implications will be pointed out and discussed below. Anonymity has led to more constructive responses from junior students As shown in Table 1 above, most of the unsuccessful corrections in both anonymous and nonanonymous activities were influenced by senior students, either providing ineffective feedback or making changes to the statements which were already correct. When examined further into the responses of junior students to senior students’ reviews in their second drafts, some in an anonymous group were made to react, question back, or challenge the senior students, and these actions were found in the drafts of junior students whose levels of proficiency were advanced or moderate. To exemplify this, Student ‘N’ in Figure 3 below was suggested by a senior student in an anonymous activity to combine two sentences. She challenged the senior student back by asking for the reasons to do so. 17 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 18 Figure 3. Sample of a junior student’s rejection and challenge to a senior student Moreover, the advanced junior learners also rejected the comments or feedback made by senior students more often than other students, and many of those rejections have led to successful corrections. The findings in this area confirm the claims made in previous studies regarding the quality of feedback made by student reviewers (Sritrakarn, 2018). The findings have also reflected some implications for the activity. Firstly, the teacher has to ensure that the chosen senior students have the capability to make reviews on junior students’ writing. Even though the average score of the selected senior students was 480, the findings show that the level of proficiency should be higher to reduce the chance to provide ineffective reviews. Secondly, the use of review activity may be suitable to certain groups of junior students whose level of proficiency was either advanced or moderate. For those who were poor students, the writing drafts should be submitted to the teacher to provide feedback or comments as well as to avoid confusion. Anonymous review activity influences the senior students to provide more constructive feedback From the findings, even though the two kinds of review have led to high frequencies of direct feedback made by senior students, when examined closely, senior students provided a greater number of comments to help the junior student better construct their essays when they were anonymous. When they could be identified, however, they shifted the strategies to provide compliments or support, to question the writers, and to criticize the junior students’ writing more often. This shows that anonymous review activity tended to provide senior students with ‘a comfort zone’ (Waemusa, 2017) in that when they provided feedback anonymously, they may become less worried about offending others (Hosack, 2003). So, they were courageous to take risks to provide direct suggestions when their names were hidden. When they could be identified, however, they avoided the chance to provide wrong comments by asking questions 18 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 19 or making criticism. In doing so, they allow junior students to take their own risks for correction. Evidence of this could be found in the interview with senior students. Out of seven, two senior students said that they preferred being anonymous, and one of the reasons was that they did not want to lose their face if making wrong comments. Being a senior student, I have to be careful not to make wrong comments and lose my face. They could laugh at me if my name was disclosed. So, I tried to say less and avoided making direct comments. (Tad-dao) This finding supports the claim made by scholars in previous studies that Asian learners hold their value of saving face (Raymond & Choon, 2017), so they tend to be compromising and may not want to give comments sincerely for fear that it might cause (them or) their peers to lose face (Kulwongse, 2013; Wanchid, 2015). This reflects the beliefs and attitudes of senior students toward the activity, which could affect the opportunity of junior students in improving their writing. Clear explanations of the goals for learning and exchanging collaboratively should therefore be made at an early stage. Training should be organized and activity goals should be negotiated explicitly with senior students. As shown in Table 6, even though most of the students had no problems providing feedback either in anonymous or non-anonymous status, they have shown a high level of attitudes (m = 3.71) when being asked about their interest in knowing the names of the writers when making comments (I am a better reviewer if I know whose work I am reading.). This reflects the uncontrolled curiosity of senior students about the owners of the work. Evidence of this was also demonstrated during the data collection stage when one anonymous-senior student posted the writing drafts of junior students on social media (Facebook) and made complaints about their writing problems. As the number of students in the context of the investigation was about ninety-six from year 1 to 4, it was still a small community. The primary goal for posting the writing drafts was, therefore, to trigger certain students who were also friends on social media and to communicate the feedback directly to the virtual community. By doing this, it reflected that some senior students did not seriously perform their roles of being anonymous, and this could lead to some misunderstanding or offense between the two groups of students. The findings also imply that training and negotiation of the activity goals should be made at the beginning prior to the data collection. In particular, the concept of ‘deindividuation’ (situations in which individuals in groups stop thinking of other members as individuals and 19 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 20 feel that they cannot be singled out by others- Chen, 2019) should be presented to ensure that students did not take the review activity personally. Furthermore, during the group interview, some senior students stated that they sometimes wanted to communicate directly to the owner of the work as they needed the clarification of some parts or wanted to explain some issue. Real communication, however, did not seem to happen as junior students submitted their drafts twice, and the activity was ended. Due to anonymity, both groups of students lack the opportunity to discuss the work or explain their points. The finding from this study supports the claim made by previous studies on the limitations of anonymous reviews regarding the lack of ongoing communication. Waemusa (2019), for example, stated that anonymous peer review has a limitation in terms of ongoing communication in that both reviewers and reviewees could not continue communication like that in the face-to-face method. Further study may create a special channel, for example, a Facebook group, where students use their pseudonyms to communicate. Alternatively, the teacher may create a common email address that can be used by both senior and junior students using their pseudonyms and let the students submit the drafts or return the comments without teacher intervention. By allowing students to take control of their own reviewing process, students may be able to manage the schedule and could seek further explanations from senior students. This could thereby promote more ongoing interactions among the two groups of learners. Conclusion This study investigated the effects of senior students’ reviews in anonymous and nonanonymous modes on the review quality and junior students’ corrections of their writing drafts. The findings from this current study have contributed to previous studies and provided useful implications on the implementation of review activity in the writing classroom. By demonstrating how the nature of anonymity helps to promote more constructive responses from junior students, the findings have also reflected the constructive nature of reviews made by senior students in the same circumstance. Moreover, the attitudes of students (both senior and junior groups) toward the activity also reflect their preferences in terms of learner identity disclosure, which provides implications and raises awareness of the teacher when applying the anonymous and non-anonymous review activity in other writing classrooms. Some of the findings in this study, however, could be limited to the groups of learners investigated in this context only. Different findings could be informed when applying the same activity in other learning contexts. To confirm the findings from this present study, further 20 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 21 studies could, therefore, be conducted to investigate the effects of anonymity and nonanonymity in different learning contexts. Moreover, as indicated in Table 6 that each group of junior students preferred their individual way of receiving comments. It is important to note here that the two groups of students participated in the consistent mode of review until the end of the semester. These junior students did not have the chance to swap the modes of review to experience receiving reviews in an alternative way. Their attitudes toward the activity could, therefore, be based on a one-sided experience. To enable the more objective findings, junior students should be exposed to both circumstances, followed by the questionnaire distribution to compare their attitudes toward the two modes of activity. 21 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 22 References Akbari, M. & Sadeghi, R.M. (2013). Foreign language learning anxiety: The case of Iranian Kundish-Persian Bilinguals. International Research Journal of Applied and Basic Sciences, 4(0), 1-9. Altun, M. & Sabah, R. (2020). The effect of cooperative learning strategies in the enhancement of EFL learners’ speaking skills. Asian EFL Journal Research Articles, 27(2.3), 144-171. Arjpru, T. (2017). The development of task-based speaking and communication strategies instructional model to enhance speaking competence of 1st year EIC students. Rangsit Journal of Educational Studies, 4(2), 72-89. Baiyaem, S. (1997). Learner training: Changing roles for a changing world, educational innovation for sustainable development. 3rd UNESCO-ACEID international conference, Bangkok, Thailand. Chen, C. (2019). Using anonymity inonline interactive EFL learning: International students’ perceptionsand practices. International Journal of Education and Development Using Informationand Communication Technology, 15(1), 204-218. Colpitts, B. (2016). Japanese students’ perceptions of peer corrective feedback in an EFL classroom. Humanities Series, No. 49, 345-358. Cote, A. R. (2014). Peer feedbackin anonymous peer review in an EFL writing class in Spain. Gist Educationand Learning Research Journal, 9, 67-87. Dordinejad, G.F. & Ahmadabad, M. R. (2014). Examination of the relationship between foreign language classroom anxiety and English achievement among male and female Iranian high school students. International Journal of Language learning and Applied Linguistics World, 6(4), 446-460. Foley, J. (2005). English…in Thailand. RELC Journal, 36(2), 233-234. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033688205055578 Hosack, I. (2003). The effects ofanonymous feedback on Japanese university students’ attitudes towards peerreview. Ritsumeikanhougakubessatsu, 1, 297-322. Houston, J. P. (2001). Fundamentals of learning and memory (4th ed.). Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers. Hyland, K. & Hyland, F. (2006).Feedback on second language students’ writing. Lang. Teach., 39, 77-95. Jessup, M.L., Connolly, T. &Tansik, A.D. (1990). Toward a theory of automated group work: The deindividuatingeffects of anonymity. Small Group Research, 21(3), 333-348. 22 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 23 Kulwongse, S. (2013). Thammasat Review 277Peer Feedback, Benefits and Drawbacks. Thammasat Review, 16(3), 227-288. Lu, R. & Bol, L. (2007). Acomparison of anonymous versus identification e-peer review on college studentwriting performance and the Extent of critical feedback. Journal of Interactive Online Learning, 6(2), 100-115. Ngoc Lac, H.H. & Gurung, S. (2015). Using online peer feedback in writing class: An action research study. Proceeding of the 2nd international VietTESOL conference: Transforming English language education in the era of globalization, 285-290. Noom-ura, S. (2013). English teaching problems in Thailand and Thai teachers’ professional development needs. English Language Teaching, 6(11), 139-147. Nur, S. (2020). Students’ perception toward the use of deductive and inductive approaches in teaching English grammar. TESOL International Journal, 15(1), 6-19. Piriyasilpa, Y. (2009). Thematic development in EFL student online discussion postings: A case study. The New English Teacher, 3(1/2), 17-33. Rajab, M.F., Alzeebaree, Y., & Zebari, HAI (2020). Effectiveness of English Language in a Globalized World: EFL Teachers of Duhok University. Asian EFL Journal Research Articles, 27(2.3), 38-47. Raymond, C.Y.L. & Choon, TT (2017). Understanding Asian students’ learning styles, cultural influences and learning strategies. Journal of Education & Social Policy, 7(1), 194-210. https://www.jespnet.com/journals/Vol_4_No_1_March_2017/23 Rayupsri, K. & Kongpetch, S. (2014). Implementation of the process-genre approach in an English in a foreign language classroom in Thailand: A case study. RJES, 1(2), 32-53. Robillos, J.R. & Phantharakphong, P. (2020). Enhancing EFL Learners’ argumentative abilities in written composition and critical thinking dispositions through argument mapping within metacognitive approach. Asian EFL Journal Research Articles, 27(3), 181-208. Sapkota, A. (2012). Developing students’ writing skill through peer and teacher correction: An action research. Journal of NELTA, 17(1-2), 70-82. Silver, R. & Coomber, M. (2010). How anonymity affects feedback in the peer review process. KOTESOL Proceedings: Advancing ELT in the global context, 299-308. Sritrakarn, N. (2018). A comparison of teachers’ and senior students’ feedback: Students’ attitudes and their writing improvement. The Journal of Asia TEFL, 15(2), 329-348. 23 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 24 Swain, M. (2005.) The output hypothesis: theory and research. In E. Heinkel (Ed.), Handbook of researchin second language teaching and learning (pp. 471-483). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sweeney, L. A.; Kunyot, T., & Preedeekul, A. (2017). Using task-based learning approach and drama to improve communicative competence for students at Pibulsongkram Rajabhat University. Journal of Faculty of Education Pibulsongkram Rajabhat University, 4(2), 112-123. Truscott, J. (1996). The caseagainst grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Language Learning, 46, 327-369. Vallente, P.J. (2020). Sources of embarrassment or empowerment? Oral feedback strategies in English language teaching classrooms. TESOL International Journal, 15(1), 31-52. Viriya, C. (2018). Using task-based learning with students of academic English. Arab World English Journal (AWEJ), 9(4), 337-346. Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge, MIT Press. Wanchid, R. (2015). Different sequences of feedback types: Effectiveness, attitudes, and preferences. PASAA, 50, 31-64. Wiriyachitra, A. (2003). Thai teachers’ role in teaching: Motivating students and guiding their learning. ThaiTESOL Focus, 16(2), 25-27. Zakaria, K.Y.N. & Hashim, H. (2020). Game-based assessment in academic writing course for pre-service teachers. TESOL International Journal, 15(1), 65-73. 24 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 25 Appendix Interview Protocol I. Interview questions for senior students 1. Did you find the anonymous and non-anonymous review activities useful? 2. Which of the two review activities did you prefer? 3. If you were not clear about the writing, what did you do? 4. Did you find any obstacles or problems during the two review activities? 5. Would you like to make any comments or suggestions for the implementation of anonymous and non-anonymous review activities in the future? II. Interview questions for junior students (anonymous group) 1. Did you find the anonymous review activity useful? 2. How did the activity help you improve your writing? 3. If you were not clear about the comments, what did you do? 4. Did you find any obstacles or problems due to the anonymous nature of the review? 5. Would you like to make any comments or suggestions for the implementation of an anonymous review activity in the future? III. Interview questions for junior students (non-anonymous group) 1. Did you find the non-anonymous review activity useful? 2. How did the activity help you improve your writing? 3. If you were not clear about the comments, what did you do? 4. Did you find any obstacles or problems due to the non-anonymous nature of the review? 5. Would you like to make any comments or suggestions for the implementation of a nonanonymous review activity in the future? 25 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 26 Comparing the Effectiveness of L2 Extensive and Intensive Listening Approaches Omar Karlin & Sayaka Karlin Toyo University & Japan Women's College of Physical Education Bio data Omar Karlin holds an Ed.D. from Temple University. His doctoral dissertation examined the validity of personality questionnaires in second language research, as well as the intersection of personality and second language development. His current research interests include the assessment of second language listening and improving listening ability. o_karlin@yahoo.com Sayaka Karlin holds a M.Sc. in Education from Temple University, as well as a M.Sc. in Economics from Manchester University. Her current research interests include the effect of authentic materials on learner motivation, task-based learning, and English for Specific Purposes. sayakakarlin@yahoo.co.jp Abstract In the fall semester of 2019, 237 Japanese university students were placed into three groups, 97 in an extensive listening group, 104 in an intensive listening group, and 42 in a control group. Participants were given a 100-question TOEIC listening test to assess L2 listening proficiency, and then placed into their respective group. During the 14-week semester, students completed six listening assignments adhering to either principles of extensive or intensive listening (as well as a control group which received no L2 listening homework). At the end of the semester, students were given another 100-question TOEIC listening test. Results indicated that the mean difference between the pre- and posttest scores was significantly larger for the EL group than the IL group t(193) = 2.14, p <.05. Among the suggestions for future research are a codification of EL and IL methodologies, a linkage of testing instruments to account for variability in testing conditions, and greater scrutiny of the participants' adherence to the intervention. Keywords: extensive listening, intensive listening, L2 listening, audiobook, TOEIC, Correspondence address: Department of Sociology Toyo University 5-28-20 Hakusan, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 〒112-8606 independent-sample t-test Introduction 26 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 27 When one thinks of the four L2 language skills of speaking, listening, writing, and reading, listening often seems to be regarded as the least important (Vandergrift & Goh, 2012), and usually the weakest skill for language learners (Vandergrift, 2013). To illustrate, in Japan, English components of university entrance exams only began to include a listening assessment as recently as 2006 (Saito, 2019), and high school English classes still focus predominately on grammar-translation methodologies with minimal focus on listening skill development (Asquith, 2014). Further, many classes in Japanese junior and senior high schools that are taught by non-native speakers do not use English as the language of instruction, denying students listening practice opportunities (Saito, 2019). Finally, textbooks are, by their nature, rife with reading practice opportunities, but not all come with an audio component for listening practice. Using the Japanese context, if one tallies the lack of institutional support of listening skill development stemming from grammar-translation focused testing, the language of instruction of the majority of teachers, and the inherent limitations of many textbooks, it is clear that listening is prioritized below that of speaking, writing, and reading. Yet in reality, listening may be the most important of the four skills (Flowerdew & Miller, 2010; Vandergrift, 2013; Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). When one speaks or writes in an L2, the speaker or writer has agency and can determine the complexity and difficulty of the language that they generate. However, when someone is engaged in an interaction with an interlocuter, they have very little control over the complexity and difficulty of the language that they receive from their interlocuter (Vandergrift, 2013). Ultimately, their continued participation in the interaction relies less on the effectiveness of their speaking than on the effectiveness of their listening, in that if they can only produce simple sentences, the interaction can probably continue but if they can only understand simple sentences, it is likely that the interaction will not be understood (especially if the interlocuter is unwilling or unaware that they need to simplify their output). Reading, to a lesser extent, also involves agency in that someone must resolve themselves to reading a text, whereas listening is often experienced whether one wants to engage in a listening task or not. For example, one might choose to not listen to train announcements, the conversations around them, or television commercials, yet these sounds are still experienced nonetheless. While these sounds can be ignored, short of wearing earplugs, they are still heard. In this regard, the exposure to listening is the most penetrative of the four skills. Considering the importance of listening competence in interactions, and the pervasiveness of listening opportunities, whether sought or not, listening could be viewed as the most important of the four skills. Yet, listening is rarely taught in language classrooms (Schmidt, 2016). Instead, what teachers may perceive to be the teaching of listening, such as asking students comprehension questions to listening 27 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 28 passages, is actually not the teaching of listening but, rather, the assessing of listening (Brown, 2011; Schmidt, 2016; Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). This paper will explore possibilities on how to more effectively teach listening in (and out of) language classrooms. Literature Review Among the topics to be reviewed in this section are differing methods of listening instruction (such as metacognitive, top-down, and bottom-up instruction), and how to define and operationalize some of the concepts (such as extensive and intensive listening) associated with these types of listening instruction. In the latter half of this section, relevant research that showcases these concepts will be highlighted, and a possible gap in the research, which this study hopes to address, will be explained. The teaching of listening in language classrooms is evolving towards more effective methods of instruction than in the past (Flowerdew & Miller, 2010). While it has been suggested that many listening teachers are just assessing listening, rather than teaching listening (Brown, 2011; Schmidt, 2016; Vandergrift & Goh, 2012), Brown (2011) suggests that some teachers have adopted teaching methodologies for listening instruction similar to those of reading instruction, with a pre-listening, listening, and post listening lesson format. Others have suggested that the key to improved L2 listening instruction lies in greater use of metacognitive strategy instruction. Because of the effectiveness of metacognitive strategy instruction in general L2 learning, the possibility exists that metacognitive strategy instruction is similarly effective in L2 listening classes (Goh, 2008; Tanewong, 2019; Vandergrift, 2013; Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). Finally, in addition to comprehension-oriented (top-down) activities often used for listening assessment, in which understanding the meaning of the listening is prioritized, Brown (2011) indicates that there has been increasing momentum in the field towards bottomup and blended approaches to listening instruction. Bottom-up processing focuses on units of sound, and the combination of these sounds into a comprehensible message (Vandergrift, 2013). With bottom-up processing, listeners rely on their phonological, lexical, and syntactic knowledge to construct meaning (Flowerdew & Miller, 2010; Rost, 2016), however bottom-up processing on its own might not be enough for comprehension as listeners cannot keep up with the sound stream (which is the figurative stream of sounds one would hear in an utterance; Vandergrift, 2013; Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). Keeping up with the sound stream can be especially difficult for learners with lower L2 proficiency as they have to manage three tasks simultaneously; identify the boundaries between 28 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 29 words, identify the L2 vocabulary in the listening passage, and ensure that they are not using L1 recognition habits when trying to decode an L2 listening passage (Vandergrift, 2013). The blended approach advocated by Jack Richards (2005) is a blending of what he terms listening as comprehension (or meaning-focused learning) and listening as acquisition (or formfocused learning). In Richards' article, he gives examples of activities that can be used in listening as comprehension situations, such as sequencing tasks, picture identification tasks, and summaries, as well as examples of activities that can be used in listening as acquisition situations, such as identifying differences between spoken and written text, cloze exercises, and dictation (Richards, 2005). Similarly, others have also endorsed a blended top-down and bottom-up focus for listening instruction (Brown, 2011; Flowerdew & Miller, 2010; Graham, 2006; Rost, 2016; Schmidt, 2016; Vandergrift, 2013; Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). In Japan, educational approaches have tended to favor traditional, teacher-fronted methodologies such as grammar-translation teaching (Mitchell, 2017; Saito, 2019), which give students little chance for communicative or cultural learning. In addition to historical traditions, a reason for this adherence to grammar-translation teaching is the influence of Japan's National Center Test, a standardized test used by hundreds of Japanese post-secondary institutions to assess student ability in a variety of school subjects, including foreign language. In the past, the foreign language component of the National Center Test has gravitated towards grammartranslation types of questions (for example, multiple choice grammar questions). In 2006, the test was modified to include a listening component in an effort to generate a positive washback effect for more communicative language teaching in Japanese high schools (Saito, 2019), yet despite this aim, there has been little progress in changing the teaching culture of foreign languages in Japanese high schools (Asquith, 2014; Mitchell, 2017; Saito, 2019). The Japanese government is eager to change the dominant teaching methodologies of high school to a more communicative approach, in part, because of the increased prevalence of English-medium instruction (hereafter referred to as EMI) at Japanese universities. Recently, EMI has increased dramatically at Japanese universities for a variety of domestic (falling birth-rate and greater competition for international students) and international (to produce more globally-mobile graduates) reasons (Shimauchi, 2018). Despite the greater need for L2 listening fluency at Japanese universities because of an increased emphasis on EMI, L2 listening at Japanese universities has tended towards assessment through the asking of comprehension questions, rather than instruction through alternative pedagogical approaches. In a study by Siegal (2014) involving ten EFL instructors in Japan, in which specific instances of listening pedagogy were coded and recorded, approximately 70% of L2 listening instruction instances were based on 29 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 30 comprehension questions while only 15% of instances followed a bottom-up approach to L2 listening instruction (with the remaining 15% of instances split amongst other pedagogical approaches to L2 listening, such as teaching metacognitive strategies, teacher modelling, and predictions). In sum, listening instruction in Japan has tended towards traditional approaches (such as grammar-translation and comprehension assessment) over emerging forms of instruction (such as communicative language teaching or bottom-up instruction). This paper will focus on two methods of L2 listening instruction that are closely aligned with top-down and bottom-up orientations, namely extensive and intensive listening. Specifically, this paper will examine how homework utilizing each of these listening concepts can be used to develop L2 listening skills. Extensive and intensive listening Within the top-down and bottom-up approaches to listening instruction are types of listening practice, extensive and intensive listening, that incorporate the principles of top-down meaningfocused learning and bottom-up form-focused learning. Extensive listening is a relatively recent practice in language teaching, and while it lacks a formalized framework or sizeable canon of existing research, some have used the principles of extensive reading (ER) as a guide for implementation (Chang, Millett, & Renandya, 2018; Day & Bamford, 1998; Mayora, 2017). While there are variations on how to define extensive reading, one of its chief principles is “an approach to language teaching in which learners read a lot of easy material in the new language” (Bamford & Day, 2004, p. 1). Defining what is easy is central to ER, and the Extensive Reading Foundation (2011), defines extensive reading as requiring 98% comprehension of a text. In their meta-analysis, Jeon and Day (2016) also note that another key feature of ER is that readers self-select their materials. In addition to the easiness and the self-selection of the material, Day and Bamford (2002) stress that there are three other necessary conditions for ER; readers be free to read as much as they can or want, that reading be done individually, and that teachers guide readers. Additionally, Jeon and Day (2016) have suggested that the most pronounced benefits of ER can be seen in adults as opposed to children, EFL situations as opposed to ESL situations, web-based materials as opposed to paper-based materials, and when established as part of a curriculum as opposed to being outside of the curriculum. Collectively, the ideal conditions for ER can be summarized as being easy, offering freedom of choice, being done individually yet within a curriculum, utilizing webbased resources, and involving adult learners in an EFL context. These conditions can be used to construct a framework for extensive listening. 30 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 31 Among the conditions outlined in the previously mentioned ER guidelines (Day & Bamford, 2002) and meta-analysis (Jeon & Day, 2016), an EL (extensive listening) framework can be constructed around several widely-agreed upon principles. Much like ER, EL is usually based upon students being exposed to a large amount of easily comprehensible input that they can enjoy listening to, in the form of movies, TV shows, podcasts, audiobooks, and radio (Chang, Millett, & Renandya, 2018). However, the Extensive Reading Foundation notes a key difference in that reading comprehension and listening comprehension usually do not equate in terms of difficulty level, with listening comprehension often below that of reading comprehension (The Extensive Reading Foundation, 2011, hereafter ERF). The ERF suggests that when using graded readers and their accompanying audiobook forms, text that is to be read is usually one or two levels higher than text that is to be listened to (2011), with the disparity between reading rates and listening rates most pronounced amongst lower-level students (Hirai, 1999). The reason for this disparity between reading and listening rates may be due to listening’s continuous, variable, and transitory nature. Van Zeeland (2014) describes continuous as words blending into each other without a clear start and finish point; variable as the same words pronounced in different ways; and transitory as words being ephemeral and requiring quicker processing. Van Zeeland's (2014) study indicated word listening inference rates of 35.6% versus reading inference rates in existing research closer to 50%, which is commensurate with the disparity suggested by the ERF. Continuing with a possible framework for EL, Mayora (2017) defines EL along five broad principles, namely quantity, comprehensibility, learner-centeredness, meaning orientation, and accountability. With Mayora's construct of EL in mind, researchers must be careful of constructing strawman definitions of IL (intensive listening), which are based upon unfavourable distinctions with EL. For instance, it is commonly agreed upon that ER (and by extension, EL) is characterized by reading (or listening to) large amounts of easily comprehensible material, with a focus on meaning. As a result, Mayora's first (quantity), second (comprehensibility), and fourth (meaning-orientation) principles of EL can be safely adopted, with IL offering a contrasted option of less, more difficult to comprehend material, with a focus on form. However, the strawman danger lies in the third (learner-centeredness) and fifth (accountability) principles of Mayora's EL construct. In Mayora's article, it is noted that locus-of-control is a powerful motivator of students, and that EL can offer students this locus-of-control as they self-select listening materials. However, with the abundance of digital materials available online, can't IL also offer students locus-of-control? Conceptually-speaking, if IL is viewed as how intensively students interact with listening texts, this can be accomplished through more difficult texts and 31 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 32 accompanying assignments, and is not contingent upon the distribution of texts (such as an open library versus teacher-assigned texts). Simply put, if the third EL principle of learnercenteredness allows EL to be conducted out of the classroom and offer students locus-ofcontrol, IL can as well, as long as students are interacting with the text in an intensive fashion. The conception of intensive studying should not be inherently connected to in-class studying administered by the teacher, and should be afforded the same opportunity to utilize the vast expanse of online digital resources that extensive listening does. Further, the fifth principle of accountability can also be equitably applied to both EL and IL. In fact, there is some disagreement as to how intrusive EL tasks should be, with the worry that demanding tasks could diminish the enjoyment (and by association, the frequency) with which students engage in EL (which is suggested in Day and Bamford's guide for extensive reading; 1998). Yet, there is widespread consensus that IL must be accompanied with form-focused activities. It is important to consider these issues of discrimination when constructing an EL vs IL dichotomy, and for the purposes of this paper, any areas in which one methodology is defined favourably at the expense of the other, has been minimized. In sum, an EL framework can be largely constructed around the extension of ER's principles, in that practitioners be exposed to large amounts of easily comprehensible input, with a focus on meaning. Yet it is important to remember that listening comprehension is usually below reading comprehension, and that suggestions of learner-centeredness and supplementary tasks should not be exclusively the domain of EL. In contrast to EL, IL involves close listening for precise units of sound and words (Rost, 2016) at a higher difficulty threshold, with the ERF advocating 90% rather than 98% comprehension for intensive reading activities. Another key difference between EL and IL is in the supplementary activities. With ER (and EL), Day and Bamford (1998) suggest that students be primarily focused on the input of as much text as possible, and not be burdened with exercises and comprehension questions. With IL, supplementary activities, such as dictation, are necessary and vital for permanent language acquisition (Rost, 2016). It is important to note that some of the previously mentioned ideal conditions for ER (and by extension, EL) from Jeon and Day's meta-analysis, such as offering learners freedom of choice, being done individually yet within a curriculum, utilizing web-based resources, and involving adult learners in an EFL situation, can also be applied to IL, and are not necessarily the exclusive purview of EL. With that said, the chief distinctions between EL and IL should be in the areas where one methodological approach is not biased over the other, such as the difficulty of the text, the frequency of listening, the closeness of listening, and supplementary activities. The distinction between EL and IL outlined by Chang (2012) follows similar guidelines, suggesting 32 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 33 that EL is abundant, easy-to-comprehend, and unburdened with supplementary tasks, while IL involves a closer listening of more difficult texts, with phonological-based supplementary activities. Additionally, the ease of comprehension for EL should result in a large amount of material listened to without the need for repetition, while the more difficult nature of IL and the phonological-based supplementary activities necessitates repetition of the text. While 98% comprehension is suggested for EL, and 80-90% for IL, the repetition of IL texts will likely result in increased comprehension with subsequent listening (Cervantes & Gainer, 1992; Iimura, 2007; Sakai, 2012). Chang (2012) distinguished between EL and IL by drawing upon the suggestions of Field (2008), Renandya (2011), and Waring (n.d.), which is shown in Figure 1. Extensive listening • Intensive listening Listening to (or being involved in) • Listening for specific information • Listening for the exact words of a phrase massive amounts of text • Text which learners can understand reasonably smoothly or expression • High levels of comprehension • Listening for details • Listening without being constrained by • Listening to mimic a text pre-set questions or tasks • Listening at or below one's comfortable fluent listening ability Figure 1. Differences between EL and IL (Chang, 2012) EL and IL research While ER is associated with a significant body of research, EL is relatively under-researched (Chang, 2012; Chang & Millett, 2016). Of the limited research involving EL and IL, many studies tend to focus on either EL or IL, with very few examples of direct comparison between the differing methodologies. One study focused on EL (Chang & Millett, 2016) suggested that when EL is supplemented with ER, significant listening gains can be observed in just 15 weeks. In the study, 76 Taiwanese university students read and listened to one graded reader per week, over a 15-week semester. Those that completed more follow-up assignments, which constituted answering 200 comprehension questions per graded reader, scored significantly higher on the listening component of a TOEIC practice test, as well as on teacher-constructed assessments, 33 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 34 leading the authors to advocate for a combined ER and EL approach with many supplemental activities. Worth noting is that treatment groups were determined based on the number of assignments that students completed over the semester, so students who completed the most assignments were assumed to have received the most treatment, thereby resulting in the highest listening scores. While completing more comprehension assignments may have led this group to higher listening scores, other factors such as greater self-efficacy, confidence, and motivation could have also played a role. Another study that advocated for a combined ER and EL approach (Chang, Millett, & Renandya, 2018) compared the listening gains of 69 Taiwanese college students split into three treatment groups; an ER only group, an EL only group, and a combined ER and EL group. The results of this study indicated that a combined ER and EL treatment yielded the greatest increase in listening comprehension scores on a teacher-created listening comprehension test. However, it should be noted that the combined ER and EL group received twice as much listening practice time as the other two groups, which may have led to their higher listening comprehension scores. Nevertheless, both studies suggest that EL is best when done simultaneously with ER. In one study focused on IL (Siegal & Siegal, 2015), 44 Japanese university students were placed into two groups, a treatment group receiving bottom-up instruction with six unique bottom-up activities, and a control group that received regular English instruction with no emphasis on bottom-up instruction. Results indicated that the treatment group outperformed the control group on a dictation test and on the listening component of the CASEC English proficiency test, suggesting that some degree of bottom-up instruction can be beneficial to students. The CASEC English proficiency test has four parts, two vocabulary and two listening, and is taken online. Usually lasting about 40-50 minutes, the website platform uses item response theory to continuously adjust the difficulty of the questions in order to match the testtaker's ability level, thereby creating a more efficient testing instrument. The two listening components of the CASEC test focus on general (17 multiple-choice questions) and specific (11 dictation questions) comprehension of a listening passage (CASEC, n.d.). To date, there have been very few studies that have directly compared the effectiveness of EL against IL in developing L2 listening ability. The first noteworthy study in this area was conducted by Chang (2012), and involved 55 Taiwanese adult students. In the study, the EL group (n = 31) listened to 15 audiobooks, while the IL group (n = 24) listened to three audiobooks over the course of a 26-week period. Both groups received several interventions from the teacher during class time, such as teacher explanations, vocabulary instruction, comprehension checks, and, solely for the IL group, dictation tests. Participants also had 34 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 35 repeated listening opportunities, as well as the opportunity to read their corresponding graded reader in tandem with listening to the audiobook. Results indicated significant gains by the EL group on a post-intervention listening test (a modified TOEIC listening test) and by the IL group on a vocabulary test (VLT and AWL), however there are several caveats with this study that make it difficult to generalize these results with other EL and IL comparison studies. Specifically, the graded readers were assigned to both groups, minimizing the autonomy of choice that is often a hallmark of ER programs (and by extension, EL). Moreover, the three audiobooks used by the IL group were also studied by the EL group (in addition to 12 other texts that were mostly easier). Typically, one of the conditions of EL is that texts be easy, yet if the IL group was using books that EL group was also using, there would be less differentiation in this aspect of the intervention. Lastly, with regard to the texts, there is conflicting research on the pace with which learners should advance in an ER program, with some suggesting that students read five books per level before moving up to the next level (Nation & Wamg, 1999) while others suggest that even 10 books per level may not be enough for lower-level students (Webb & Chang, 2015). In the Chang (2012) study, it is acknowledged that the learners' “English competence was low” (p. 32), which would presumably mean in an ER program they would read at least five, but likely 10, graded readers before moving up, yet the EL intervention's progression of audiobook texts on the Oxford Bookworms series scale was as follows: levels 2, 1, 1, 1, 5, 1, and 2 in the first half of the research period, and levels 2, 2, 3, 4, 2, 3, 3, and 4 in the second half of the research period. It must be noted that books 1, 11, and 14 were not from the Oxford Bookworms series but their respective difficulty was equated to the Oxford Bookworm levels of 2, 4, and 3 (these books appeared to be from the Cambridge, Scholastic, and MacMillan series, respectively). The relatively quick advancement of students to more difficult texts would not seem to suggest a true EL intervention, and may, at least in terms of the text difficulty level, be similar to that of an IL treatment (which is underscored by the EL group using the same texts as the IL group). In addition to this, ER (and EL) is predicated on participants choosing their own texts and reading at their own pace, with little teacher intervention, however the texts in this study were chosen for the EL group, listening was done during class at a fixed pace for everyone, and teacher instruction was frequent. Again, this intervention would seem to be more congruous with traditional intensive methodologies, with little student autonomy and a sameness of experience for all. It should be noted that this paper will advocate that EL and IL follow a similar protocol in the area of learner-centeredness, with both the EL and IL groups enjoying autonomy and a uniqueness of experience. Recalling Mayora's (2017) five principles of EL of quantity, comprehensibility, learner-centeredness, 35 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 36 meaning orientation, and accountability, the boundaries between the EL and IL interventions in the Chang (2012) study are not easily discernable in the areas of quantity (in terms of inclass listening time), comprehensibility, and learner-centeredness, so while this study offers some valuable insights into the direct comparison of EL and IL, there are caveats that must be acknowledged that mitigate the generalizability of the findings. Similarly, another study with several caveats that directly compared EL and IL methodologies in developing L2 listening ability was conducted by Karlin and Karlin (2019). This study involved 80 first-year Japanese university students, who were split into EL and IL groups, with each receiving listening homework that adhered to either EL or IL conventions. Results indicated that the IL group significantly outperformed the EL group on a listening exam, which included seven 5-minute listening passages, with six comprehension questions per listening passage (three focused on general comprehension, and three on specific details from the listening passage). Among the chief caveats with this study is a research design which utilized an unbalanced intervention between the groups. While the IL group was prescribed an intervention that required several hours of listening practice every two weeks in order to complete a dictation assignment, the EL group was prescribed an intervention that required a much smaller amount of time to complete. The reason for this discrepancy was that the participants in this study were a sample of convenience, from two different listening classes, conducted by two different teachers, with two different views on assigning homework. While the teacher coordinating the EL group assigned homework that most would characterize as exceedingly easy, the opposite was true of the teacher coordinating the IL group, indicating a bias in the interventions upon which this study was based. Future research will need to ensure that the EL and IL interventions are sufficiently differentiated (unlike the Chang study), and that EL and IL interventions are unbiased and roughly equal in the burden placed upon students (unlike in the Karlin and Karlin study). While not measuring L2 listening ability, Ahmadpour and Asadollafam's (2018) study involved 60 Iranian EFL learners and compared extensive and intensive listening approaches in developing verb tense usage proficiency. The authors concluded that both EL and IL helped develop proficiency in verb tense usage, however this finding comes with a few caveats. First, the control group, which received no special treatment, improved in almost equal measure as the intervention groups. Second, the authors concluded that there was no significant difference between the mean change of the EL and IL groups' test scores, thus no difference in the effectiveness of EL and IL at developing verb tense usage proficiency. However, the mean difference in favor of the IL group almost reached significance with a p value of .059 suggesting 36 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 37 that if the research period was slightly longer or had slightly more participants (which was already quite low), significance could have been achieved. Further, when the mean difference of the two treatment groups were compared with the control group, the IL group scored significantly higher than the control group, while the EL group did not, which can be viewed as an indirect measure of a significant difference between the EL and IL groups. Finally, when making direct comparisons between the two interventions, it is essential that both interventions exert an equal burden upon participants, in order to avoid bias. It seems, according to the authors, that the EL group's intervention was conducted both at home with homework and inclass with teacher instruction, yet the IL group's intervention was only conducted in-class, with no mention of a homework assignment to equate the study-time with the EL group, seemingly putting the IL group at a disadvantage. Despite this, the IL group significantly outperformed the control group while the EL group did not. These findings are consistent with Chang's (2012) study that indicated an IL group outperformed an EL group in the area of vocabulary acquisition. In sum, while limited research favours EL in terms of L2 listening ability development, either explicitly or implicitly when considering research design bias (Chang, 2012; Karlin & Karlin, 2019), other areas of language development, such as vocabulary acquisition and correct verb tense usage, seem to favor IL (Chang, 2012; Ahmadpour & Asadollafam, 2018). While not a direct comparison between EL and IL, Chang (2010) compared EL with a methodology she terms as formal instruction, which is modelled after traditional teacherfronted foreign language teaching. Involving 92 Chinese college students, Chang measured the L2 listening proficiency and anxiety of groups receiving an EL or formal instruction intervention over a 1-year research period. Results indicated that both groups improved in measures of L2 listening proficiency, namely a narrative listening test and a conversational listening test, and the EL group increased significantly in listening anxiety. It should be noted that the EL group improved to a greater degree than the formal instruction group in measures of L2 listening proficiency, with mean gains of 7.87 and 5.79 on the narrative and conversational listening tests, respectively. The gains for the formal instruction group were 2.66 and 4.21 on the narrative and conversational listening tests, respectively. It should also be noted that there appears to be a mistake in the text of the article, suggesting the opposite findings, indicated in the study's Table 1, with the formal instruction group out-performing the EL group on the conversational listening test, with mean scores of 5.79 and 4.21, respectively. Chang concluded that the surprising increase in anxiety for the EL group may have been due to the novelty of EL learning activities, in which traditional educational tools of tests and grades are 37 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 38 minimized, which might have caused apprehension in students who may have felt as if they were doing something against educational norms. While the formal instruction group was not explicitly identified as utilizing an IL methodology, on the surface, there appeared to be similarities with traditional interpretations of intensive learning in that there was a focus on teaching vocabulary and grammar, regular testing, and a shared text and learning pace amongst all students. Despite this seeming affirmation of EL, this study has some of the same caveats identified in the Chang (2012) and Karlin and Karlin (2019) studies. In the case of the former, the specific nature of the EL intervention is not clearly defined in the article, and is described as the class using the same 25 audiobooks (it is unclear if these texts were utilized individually or collectively, in class or at home) with some teacher-led discussion and comprehension testing. In the case of the latter, there is an acknowledgment that a convenience sample was used involving different instructors with different degrees of strictness and leniency, which may have resulted in unintended bias within the sample. However, Chang does indicate that both groups received about 100 minutes of listening instruction per week, suggesting an effort was made to minimize bias resulting from time inequalities. Results were consistent with Chang's later study in 2012, which affirmed that EL was more effective at boosting L2 listening proficiency when compared to other methodologies (formal instruction in this case, and IL in the 2012 study). Research question To what extent does EL and IL homework improve the L2 listening comprehension of Japanese university students? Participants Initially, 398 students participated in this study (246 female, 152 male), drawn from 16 classes taught by the researchers. Of these students, 234 were first-year students, 149 were second-year students, nine were third-year students, and two were fourth-year students. Additionally, four Masters students participated in this study. The year of study, gender, and proficiency levels for participants is shown in Table 1. Table 1 Proficiency, gender, and age of initial participants, by class. Class Proficiency University n Male students Female students Year of study Avg. age 1 Intermediate One 28 18 10 2 20.07 38 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 39 2 intermediate one 29 7 22 2 19.72 3 beginner one 27 1 26 2 19.96 4 beginner one 25 13 12 1 18.88 5 beginner one 28 17 11 1 18.71 6 advanced three 23 11 12 varied 20.04 7 advanced three 12 4 8 varied 21.25 8 intermediate two 21 6 15 1 18.67 9 intermediate two 22 10 12 1 18.73 10 advanced three 24 9 15 1 18.96 11 advanced three 25 11 14 1 18.80 12 advanced one 25 5 20 2 20.32 13 beginner one 26 12 14 2 19.92 14 advanced one 25 8 17 1 19.52 15 intermediate one 28 11 17 1 18.75 16 beginner one 30 9 21 1 18.53 398 152 246 Varied 19.37 Totals The 16 classes used in this study were from three major Tokyo-area private universities, and as a result, there were curriculum differences amongst the classes, yet all followed a communicative teaching methodology. For university one, classes met once a week for 90 minutes, and used a four-skills textbook to learn and practice new grammar in communicative situations, such as role-plays and conversations. Students in the classes at university one completed the textbook's online homework component every two weeks, and had a speaking assessment at the end of the semester. For university two, classes met once a week for 200 minutes, and used a four-skills textbook to learn and practice the new grammar in communicative situations, such as role-plays and conversations. Students in the classes at university two did not have an online homework component, however, during the semester they completed five quizzes, two group presentations, and a final exam, all based on the textbook's content. For university three, classes met once a week for 90 minutes, and students were drawn from three types of classes, one that used a four-skills textbook to learn and practice the new grammar in communicative situations, one that used a reading textbook to practice reading skills and discuss questions related to each chapter's reading passages, and one that used a writing textbook to clarify and practice the components of an academic essay. The first class had online homework every two weeks and two written exams, based on the course textbook. The second class had weekly vocabulary quizzes, a small online homework component, and two exams during the semester. Finally, the third class had regular writing and revision 39 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 40 assignments in which they would practice the various parts of a standard academic essay. Despite the differences in curriculum, the classes were similar in that they were communicative in nature and the workload associated with the homework was roughly equivalent amongst all classes. Due to attrition, the final number of participants in the analysis was reduced to 237 from 398. The chief reason for the attrition of participants was not closely adhering to the study's research interventions (explained further in the analysis section). The final tally of participants is shown in Table 2. Table 2 Proficiency, gender, and age of final participants, by class. Class Proficiency University n Male students Female students Year of study Avg. age 1 Intermediate One 18 10 8 2 20.00 2 intermediate one 24 5 19 2 19.71 3 beginner one 16 0 16 2 19.81 4 beginner one 11 5 6 1 19.00 5 beginner one 13 9 4 1 19.08 6 advanced three 13 4 9 varied 19.54 7 advanced three 11 3 8 varied 21.00 8 intermediate two 8 1 7 1 18.25 9 intermediate two 14 8 6 1 18.79 10 advanced three 18 5 13 1 18.94 11 advanced three 21 8 13 1 18.81 12 advanced one 14 3 11 2 20.21 13 beginner one 18 8 10 2 20.00 14 advanced one 18 6 12 1 19.61 15 intermediate one 10 3 7 1 18.40 16 beginner one 10 1 9 1 18.30 237 79 158 Varied 19.41 Totals Instrument This study used an assessment based on the listening component of the TOEIC proficiency test. The TOEIC test's listening component is comprised of 100 questions spread across four parts; six questions for part one (understanding the descriptions of photographs), 25 questions for part two (choosing the correct response to a question), 39 questions for part three (understanding conversations), and 30 questions for part four (understanding announcements; Cid, Wei, Kim, & Hauck, 2017). It was believed that the standard TOEIC format might prove to be too difficult 40 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 41 for the lower-proficiency participants in this study, and the resulting demotivation from a difficult testing experience might affect their feelings towards the course. As suggested by Brown (2011), it is advantageous to adapt the difficulty of listening texts and tasks to the ability of students. As a result, three versions of the listening assessment were created for this study; one test for participants in beginner-level classes, one for those in intermediate-level classes, and one for advanced-level classes. In a study involving 3673 Japanese and Korean test-takers (Cid, Wei, Kim, & Hauck, 2017), it was determined that the difficulty level of the four sections of the TOEIC listening component followed an ascending order of difficulty, with part one representing the easiest section, followed by part two, part three, and part four, with p-values of 0.80, 0.67, 0.66, and 0.57, respectively, in one analysis of the TOEIC, and p-values of 0.82, 0.70, 0.62, and 0.62,respectively, in another analysis (p-value was defined as the proportion of test-takers who could correctly answer a question within the population). Accordingly, the beginner and intermediate-level versions of the listening assessment used in this study included more questions from parts one and two, and fewer questions from parts three and four. The advanced-level test was kept the same as the standard version of the TOEIC listening component. The breakdown in questions from each part of the listening assessment for beginner, intermediate, and advanced versions of the test is shown in Table 3. Table 3 Number of questions for each part of the listening assessment, by difficulty level difficulty level Part 1 questions Part 2 questions Part 3 questions Part 4 questions Beginner 40 30 21 9 Intermediate 18 35 33 14 Advanced 6 25 39 30 This study included both a pretest and a posttest to assess English listening proficiency. While both the pretest and posttest followed the TOEIC format outlined above, different questions were used for the pretest and the posttest, resulting in six different versions of the instrument. Procedure In the first class of the semester, the research project was explained in English to the participants. An explanation of the research project was also given to participants in written form, in both English and Japanese. Participants were then asked to sign a consent form acknowledging their willing participation in the study. After completing the consent form, participants took the pretest, which lasted for 40 minutes. 41 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 42 After completing the pretest, participants were randomly placed into one of three groups; two intervention groups (an EL and an IL group) and one control group. Once students were placed into groups, the homework expectations were explained to each group. The EL group was advised to choose three audiobooks from the class' online library of audiobooks, listen to their three chosen audiobooks, and to write a brief summary of each audiobook (one paragraph) in English. Collectively, the three summaries from this group were expected to fill an A4-sized worksheet. The EL group was instructed to only choose audiobooks for which they had approximately 98% listening comprehension. The IL group was advised to choose to one audiobook from the class' online library of audiobooks, listen to it, and write a one-page dictation from any portion of the audiobook on an A4-sized worksheet. The IL group was instructed to only choose audiobooks for which they had approximately 80-90% listening comprehension. The control group was told to not listen to any of the audiobooks, but instead to listen to three texts in their native language, including, movies, television shows, and podcasts, and write a brief summary of each (one paragraph) in English. Additionally, they were instructed to write a one-page dictation of one of their chosen listening texts on an A4-sized worksheet in their native language. The reason the control group was given both an English writing assignment (summaries) and a native language writing assignment (dictation) was to equalize the English writing component between the control and EL groups, and the time burden amongst all groups. If the control group had no English writing component, the mean difference in pre- and post TOEIC scores between the control and EL group could be partially reflective of the writing component that the EL group had to complete. Additionally, it was thought that the control group should spend roughly the same amount of time completing the homework as the other groups, otherwise the two intervention groups might view their heavier workload as unfair. This homework cycle was repeated every two weeks with new listening texts, with participants ultimately completing six assignments over the course of the 15-week semester. An effort was made to give assignments in two-week intervals; however, this was not always possible. For example, the third class for some courses had to be cancelled because of Typhoon Hagibis. In this case, there was a three-week gap from when the homework assignment was given and when it was collected. Similarly, near the end of the fall semester there was a twoweek break in classes so students could celebrate year-end and new-year holidays. For some courses, the homework assignment was collected in the week preceding this break, and the new assignment was collected immediately after the break; a three-week gap from when the assignment was given and when it was collected. 42 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 43 In the final class of the semester, participants completed the 40-minute posttest. Audiobook library The two intervention groups were asked to self-select their own listening texts during the semester. The listening texts were available online through the media-hosting website Mediafire. The listening text library consisted entirely of the audiobooks of graded readers from a variety of publishers, as shown in Table 4. Table 4 Graded reader audiobook series used in study Series Level headwords Titles ERF grading scale Popcorn ELT Starter 150 12 Beginner Popcorn ELT 1 200 18 Beginner Popcorn ELT 2 250 20 Beginner Oxford Starter 250 26 Beginner Pearson 1 300 25 Beginner Popcorn ELT 3 300 16 Beginner Scholastic Starter 300 8 Beginner Oxford 1 400 39 Elementary Pearson 2 600 48 Elementary Scholastic 1 600 13 Elementary Oxford 2 700 39 Elementary Oxford 3 1000 27 Intermediate Scholastic 2 1000 31 Intermediate Pearson 3 1200 49 Intermediate Oxford 4 1400 20 Intermediate Scholastic 3 1500 33 Intermediate Pearson 4 1700 30 Upper-intermediate Oxford 5 1800 12 Upper-intermediate Scholastic 4 2000 5 Upper-intermediate Pearson 5 2300 32 Upper-intermediate Oxford 6 2500 9 Advanced Pearson 6 3000 21 Advanced Total tiles 533 Audiobook files were in MP3 format, and participants were able to stream the audio files through their smartphone, or download them to their computer and listen with a media player. Analysis 43 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 44 L2 listening proficiency data was collected on paper response sheets from participants on the first and last days of class. Participant responses were entered into a text file, and processed through the Rasch measurement analysis software, Winsteps. This software allows for the creation of a variety of statistical data, such as raw scores, Rasch person measures, person fit, and item fit. Rasch reliability and separation data for persons and items is shown in Table 5. Table 5 Rasch reliability and separation for pre- and post-tests Person n Item reliability separation SD n reliability separation SD Pre-test 237 .93 3.60 .92 144 .96 4.68 1.14 Post-test 237 .93 3.70 .89 144 .96 4.73 .98 This study used the person raw scores to determine mean difference between the pre- and posttests. The person raw scores were entered into the software IBM SPSS Statistics ver. 22, and an independent-sample t-test was conducted on the pre- and post- mean difference for the two intervention groups. Findings & Discussion Results Initially, this study included 398 participants, however many participants did not adhere to the prescribed intervention, and as a result, were removed from the study. To elaborate, the 398 participants were categorized into four streams of based on their degree of intervention completion. Those participants that completed all homework assignments, without any late assignments were categorized as stream 1 and kept in the study. Those participants that did all homework assignments but submitted an assignment late once or twice were categorized as stream 2, and kept in the study. However, while one or two late assignments was viewed as a minor disruption to the prescribed intervention, three or more late assignments was viewed as a significant disruption to the intervention. For example, some participants did not submit homework for six weeks, but then did all of their assignments in one day and submitted multiple assignments at the same time. This would indicate that participants were not getting regular L2 listening practice, which is a primary condition for improving L2 listening proficiency. As a result, participants who submitted 3-5 late assignments or did not submit an assignment, were categorized as stream 3 and removed from the study. Finally, participants that were late with all of their assignments, or did not submit two or more assignments were categorized as stream 44 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 45 4, and removed from the study. The descriptive statistics for the remaining 237 participants are shown in Table 6. Table 6 Descriptive statistics for remaining participants Group Pretest n score Posttest SD score SD Mean difference SD EL 91 56.00 17.00 57.51 14.87 1.35 8.45 IL 104 54.56 16.48 53.07 15.08 -1.49 9.93 42 53.81 12.11 52.50 12.70 -1.14 10.25 237 54.98 15.96 54.67 14.72 -0.34 9.50 Control Total Note: the maximum score is 100. Recalling the intervention for each group, the EL group was instructed to listen to three texts for which they had 98% comprehension, while the IL group was instructed to listen to one text for which they had 80-90% comprehension. This difference in comprehension of selected listening texts between the EL and IL groups is reflected in the average headwords of the selected listening texts. As the EL group was selecting more comprehensible texts, the headwords of these texts was considerably lower than that of the listening texts selected by the IL group. The average headwords for each group on each task, is shown in Table 7. Table 7 Average headwords for each group, by task EL 91 Task 1 headwords 286.40 IL 104 636.06 622.60 658.65 652.88 690.87 687.91 42 0 0 0 0 0 0 237 389.08 384.59 405.85 404.86 427.00 430.06 Group Control Total n Task 2 headwords 290.08 Task 3 headwords 304.24 Task 4 headwords 308.25 Task 5 headwords 325.11 Task 6 headwords 334.79 An independent-sample t-test was conducted for the mean difference in TOEIC listening scores between the pre- and post-test, for the EL and IL groups. Levene's test for equality of variance indicated homogenous groups, with the EL group's subsequent mean difference significantly larger than the IL group; t(193) = 2.14, p <.05. Results from the independent-sample t-test are shown in Table 8. Table 8 Independent-sample t-test of mean difference between EL and IL groups 45 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 Raw score Levene's test for equality of variance F sig. 2.22 .14 46 T df 2.14 193 sig. .03 Mean difference SE difference 2.84 1.33 95% CI Lower .22 Upper 5.47 Discussion The results of this study were consistent with those of Chang's study (2012) in that EL proved to be better at developing L2 listening proficiency, albeit to a lesser degree than was observed in the Chang study. The results run contrary to the findings in Karlin and Karlin's study (2019), and presumably the chief reason is that study failed to equate the EL and IL interventions, with the IL group doing much more L2 listening practice, hence their superior L2 listening proficiency development. Regarding the difference in the magnitude of L2 listening proficiency gains between the Chang (2012) study and this study, the reason perhaps lies in the difference in class time spent doing L2 listening practice and teacher guidance between the two studies. The Chang study used a lot of class time with consistent teacher guidance, which presumably enhanced the learning of participants, whereas the Karlin and Karlin study used no class time, and had minimal teacher guidance beyond the initial explanation of the assignment. Presumably if the two studies had equal measures of class time and teacher guidance, and if the Karlin and Karlin study had a more balanced research design (with equal weight on the EL intervention), the L2 listening proficiency gains would have been more consistent between the two studies. The discrepancy in the results of these two studies stems from the inconsistent interpretations of EL and IL interventions within the research field, particularly with widely disparate implementations of EL research designs. Because ER relies on empowering students to choose their own materials, read at their own pace, and be unencumbered with tasks, future research should ensure that class time and teacher guidance for EL research be underpinned with similar principles. Considering these caveats, despite the Chang (2012), Karlin and Karlin (2019), and this study differing in their results, from strongly supportive of EL, mildly supportive of IL, and mildly supportive of EL, respectively, it appears that these differences stem from research design incongruities. Another noteworthy feature of the data was the importance of intervention adherence in the sample. As mentioned above, principles of ER (and, by extension, EL) are based on empowering students to study by themselves, however under such circumstances, the risk of students not effectively studying or doing their homework is high. Without teacher intervention to ensure students complete every assignment properly and on-time, it is likely that a great deal of students will not meet expectations, which is what happened in this study. Within just a 46 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 47 single semester, over 40% of the participants in this study had to be removed because of a failure to adhere to the prescribed intervention. Over a longer timeline, such as a full year, or even multiple years, the attrition rate could be so high that the credibility of the study could be threatened. Perhaps the way to prevent participants from undermining the intervention is to add greater incentives into the research design for participants to adhere to the intervention, such as grade penalties for failure to submit listening homework on time, and greater teacher scrutiny on the proper completion of homework, such as with regular teacher consultations with students or devoting a portion of class time to oversee the completion of L2 listening practice and accompanying tasks. Doing so, however, would seem to violate one of the principal tenets of EL in that it should be done independently and without much teacher involvement. As a result, future research will need to address this tension between teacher oversight and student empowerment, and negotiate a resolution. It should also be noted that while the gains observed by EL over IL were modest, the timeline of this study was shorter than that of other studies. The full length of this study was only 14 weeks, while that of the Chang (2012) study was 26 weeks, Chang and Millett (2016) was 15 weeks, and Siegal and Siegal (2015) was 15 weeks (but 30 classes). It is unclear how a longer timeline might have affected these results, but it is assumed that gains would have been amplified. The reasoning for this lies in the affective benefits of EL. As students do more EL practice, the hope is that the empowerment inherent in the activity and the variety of the listening texts engenders a listening habit in which students enjoy listening, resulting in a more accepting stance towards listening practice and more listening opportunities as they begin listening to audiobooks in their free time, for fun rather than out of a homework obligation. Thus, a two-semester timeline, which would be around 28-30 weeks, might yield EL gains closer to those observed in the Chang (2012) study. For Japanese universities, the implications of these findings would suggest that more EL resources be available to students. As it stands, many universities in Japan have ER resources such as graded reader libraries, however few have EL resources such as online audiobook libraries. With the proliferation of smartphones that are easily capable of playing mp3 audio files, as well as the widespread availability of internet access through 4G and 5G data networks and free campus wifi, not fully utilizing audiobooks in English-language curriculums seems like a wasted opportunity. Never before has it been this easy for students to access such a diverse array of L2 listening content, not just with the audiobooks that are paired with graded readers, but also video streaming services (such as Netflix, Hulu, and Disney+), music streaming services (such as Spotify, Apple Music, and Pandora), podcasts, online lectures (such as through 47 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 48 the TED website), and online news broadcasts (such as through the BBC). Curriculums are going to have to adapt from traditional models of in-class textbook-based instruction to a model that better incorporates these resources. The audiobooks associated with graded readers, such as the ones used in this study, are particularly effective because they are modified to suit a variety of different proficiency levels, allowing students with intermediate proficiency or below, to find engaging listening materials, whereas in the past, the majority of listening materials were limited to advanced students. Japan seems particularly well-suited to the adoption of EL in that most students rely on public transportation to attend school. Public transportation gives students dedicated time twice a day, where there is little else that can be done besides sitting (or more likely, standing) during a commute, representing an ideal time to listen to five minutes of an audiobook and to establish a daily listening habit. In 2020, the importance of developing a self-study listening habit was even more acute, as most universities in Japan relied fully on online lessons because of COVID-19. Having dedicated online L2 listening resources can prove to be a boon to education when students must adopt more independent learning behaviours because of external factors. Further, if the Japanese government's goal is to transition to more EMI at universities, having online resources that offer students long-form L2 listening practice may help with the transition for some students from grammar-translation learning in high-school to EMI learning at universities. Limitations There were several limitations with this study that need to be considered when evaluating the findings, some of which are correctable in future research and some of which are inherently difficult to resolve. Among the issues that are difficult to resolve is determining interventions for EL and IL that are fairly balanced, in order to get an equitable comparison between EL and IL. For instance, it is suggested in the existing research that EL be easier so that students will be more likely to practice EL, when compared with IL. However, because of a lack of research and codified definitions regarding EL and IL, researchers must use their judgment when establishing an equitable division of work between EL and IL. In this study, EL participants were instructed to listen to three times as many audiobooks as IL participants (3 audiobooks vs 1 audiobook) every two weeks. The decision to give the EL group three times as many audiobooks per assignment was based on roughly how long it would take to listen to three easier audiobooks versus one more difficult audiobook (for example, an hour of listening). Yet, because students were free to choose their own audiobooks, many students in the EL group especially, seemed to choose audiobooks that were too easy and fully-within their level of 48 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 49 comprehension, as evidenced by the wide disparity in headwords between the two groups (shown in Table 7). The IL group ultimately chose audiobooks with twice as many headwords, far more than the 8-18% increased difficulty that was explained to them in the initial explanation of the assignment (i.e. 98% comprehension for EL vs. 80-90% comprehension for IL). Nevertheless, future researchers will need to do their best to equate the EL and IL interventions so that they represent roughly the same work burden for students, even though several variables will be quite different, such as the difficulty of the audiobooks, the length of the audiobooks, the frequency of listening practice, and the type of follow-up tasks (if any). Bias exists in all of these variables and researchers will need to earnestly evaluate all intervention choices in order to minimize biases in the research design, and unfortunately, will never fully eliminate bias or achieve universal consensus within the research field. Among the correctable limitations in this study, one would be giving students regular feedback on the level of audiobook that is appropriate for their proficiency level. It was observed in the study that occasionally students who were of very high proficiency, almost native-level, were choosing the easiest audiobooks because they wanted to reduce their homework burden. Even though these students did all of the homework, and submitted the assignments on time, they found a way to circumvent the goals of the intervention, and future research would be advised to limit audiobook options for these students or to suggest that they choose more appropriate audiobooks if it appears they are choosing audiobooks well-below their L2 listening level.. At the same time, it is important to preserve some freedom of choice, since freedom of choice is a central ER principle and student empowerment has been shown to have numerous benefits in the area of developing independent and motivated learners (Dörnyei & Csizér, 1998). Again, the lack of research for EL and IL will require a great deal of negotiation on behalf of the researchers in regard to how much teacher intervention can be implemented while still maintaining adequate learner autonomy (Tholin, 2008). Another limitation related to this study's interventions involves the handwritten completion and in-person submission of homework assignments. With 237 participants completing six assignments each, the number of total assignments completed in the study was in excess of 1400. For the researcher, it is crucial that each assignment receive some degree of scrutiny. For example, the IL group was required to complete a dictation of about one A4 page in length. Yet, some students proved adept at finding ways around this requirement by increasing the line spacing or font size in their assignment, resulting in vastly disparate assignments between students who studiously completed their dictation and students who were trying to evade their homework. A way to combat this might be to introduce a minimum word 49 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 50 count for dictations (for example, 650 words), and in order to police this requirement, it would be necessary for students to submit their assignments as a Word file (which displays the number of words in each document). The 1400 homework assignments in this study were hand-written, making it impossible to monitor word counts. Further, with 237 participants, the total number of selected texts was almost 3000. It would be virtually impossible to effectively monitor 3000 assignments for plagiarism without the use of plagiarism software. In order to process assignments through plagiarism software, assignments need to be in a digital form (such as a Microsoft Word or pdf file). Obviously students engaged in plagiarism are not following the prescribed intervention, and would need to be removed from the sample. Lastly, if there are problems with the homework or the level of the text(s) chosen by the participant, the researcher needs to be able to give feedback, and with classes only once a week, it would not be advantageous for the researcher to have to make notations in a log-book on who to give feedback to, when to give it, and what feedback to give (this would likely result in forgotten details and missed consultations). A more direct and immediate line of communication, such as through email, would reduce the complexity of this feedback process and the burden on the researcher. As a result, for future research, participants should submit assignments via email, which would allow the researcher to check each assignment and provide immediate feedback (without the cumbersome process of recording details in a log for a later consultation). Suffice it to say, monitoring the quality of completed assignments (such as word count for dictations), catching plagiarism, and providing guidance would all be enhanced through the digital completion and submission of assignments. Another limitation with this study was the seeming variability in test performance between the pre- and post-tests, which resulted in a relatively small improvement for the EL group. The pretest, conducted at the beginning of the fall semester, was given to students who were wellrested and not burdened with other homework assignments. Conversely, the posttest, conducted in the last class of the fall semester, was given to students who were, in many cases, fatigued because of the long semester, not sleeping well because of the time demands put on them by the final assignments from other classes, and in some cases, hung-over because Japan's comingof-age celebration commemorating the start of adulthood occurred the day (and night) before the posttest (for some classes). While it was assumed that the pre- and post-tests were roughly equal in difficulty level, the testing conditions for the posttest were more onerous, possibly resulting in diminished scores. To overcome this, future research would be well-served to link the pre- and post-tests with several shared items, and thus, when analysing the data through Winsteps, the shared items can serve as a way to calculate the relative difficulty of all items in 50 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 51 the pre- and posttest. Thus, the resulting weighted test data would likely be much more accurate, particularly the Rasch person measure scores produced by Winsteps. It has been shown that Rasch person measure scores can be significantly more accurate than raw scores in assessing person ability (Weaver, Jones, & Bulach, 2008), and linking these two tests with shared items would improve testing accuracy. The result would be mean differences in testing scores that would be less susceptible to variations in testing conditions, such as student fatigue. Conclusions Hopefully, this paper has helped to clarify some of the benefits of EL and IL in developing L2 listening proficiency. While there are only a few studies that have directly compared EL and IL in terms of developing L2 listening proficiency, it would seem that the results of this study are consistent with existing research, especially when considering the contextual factors surrounding each study. Perhaps of even greater value than the results of this study, is the raising of the issues of codified frameworks for EL and IL, the need for consistency in research design in EL and IL studies, and the elimination of bias when comparing these variables. While the results of this and previous studies in EL and IL are relatively consistent when considering contextual factors, the need to account for contextual factors and differences in perceptions of EL and IL muddies the waters when attempting to come to a consensus on the findings. Until research addresses these contextual factors, consensus may prove to be elusive. Nevertheless, this study represents a step in that direction. 51 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 52 References Ahmadpour, S., & Asadollafam, H. (2018). A study of the effect of extensive and intensive listening on the tense use among EFL learners in Iran. The Journal of Applied Linguistics and Applied Literature: Dynamics and Advances, 6(2), 141-161. Asquith, S. (2014). Integrating a functional approach with Japanese junior high school teaching practices. In P. Clements, A. Krause and H. Brown (Eds.), JALT2014: Conversations across borders. Ibaraki: JALT. Bamford, J., & Day, R.R. (Eds.). (2004). Extensive reading activities for teaching language. New York: Cambridge University Press. Brown, S. (2011). Listening myths: Applying Second Language Research to Classroom Teaching. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. CASEC global website. (n.d.). Retrieved February 04, 2021, from https://global.casec.com/ Cervantes, R., & Gainer, G. (1992). The effects of syntactic simplification and repetition on listening comprehension. TESOL Quarterly, 26(4), 767-770. doi:10.2307/3586886 Chang, A. (2010). Second-language listening anxiety before and after a 1-yr intervention in extensive listening compared with standard foreign language instruction. Perceptual and Motor Skill, 110(2), 355-365. doi:10.2466/PMS.110.2.355-365 Chang, A. (2012). Gains to L2 learners from extensive listening: Listening development, vocabulary acquisition and perceptions of the intervention. Hong Kong Journal of Applied Linguistics, 14(1), 35-47. Chang, A., & Millett, S. (2016). Developing L2 listening fluency through extended listeningfocused activities in an extensive-listening programme. RELC Journal, 47(3), 349362. doi:10.1177/003368821663117 Chang, A., Millett, S., & Renandya, W. A. (2018). Developing listening fluency through supported extensive listening practice. RELC Journal, 49(3), 1-17. doi:10.1177/0033688217751468 Cid, J., Wei, Y., Kim, S., & Hauck, C. (2017). Statistical analyses for the updated TOEIC Listening and Reading test (Research Memorandum No. RM-17-05). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Day, R., & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading in the second language classroom. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Day, R., & Bamford, J. (2002). Top ten principles in teaching extensive reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 14, 136–141. 52 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 53 Dörnyei, Z., & Csizér, K. (1998). Ten commandments for motivating language learners: Results of an empirical study. Language Teaching Research, 2, 203-229. Field, J. (2008). Listening in the language classroom. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511575945 Flowerdew, J., & Miler, L. (2010). Listening in a second language. In A. D. Wolvin (Ed.), Listening and Human Communication in the 21st Century. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing. doi:10.1002/9781444314908 Goh, C. (2008). Metacognitive instruction for second language listening development: Theory, practice and research implications. RELC Journal, 39(2), 188-213. doi:10.1177/0033688208092184 Graham, S. (2006). Listening comprehension: The learner's perspective. System, 34, 165-182. Hirai, A. (1999). The relationship between listening and reading rates of Japanese EFL learners. Modern Language Journal, 83(3), 367-384. doi:10.1111/0026-7902.00028 Iimura, H. (2007). The listening process. Effects of question types and repetition. Language Education & Technology, 44, 75-85. doi: 10.24539/let.44.0_75 Jeon, E-Y., & Day, R. (2016). The effectiveness of ER on reading proficiency: A metaanalysis. Reading in a Foreign Language, 28(2), 246-265. Karlin, O., & Karlin, S. (2019). L2 listening homework: Intensive vs. extensive. Korea TESOL Journal, 14(2), 97-115. Mayora, C. A. (2017). Extensive listening in a Colombian university: Process, product, and perceptions. HOW, 24(1), 101-121. doi: 10.19183/how.24.1.311 Mitchell, C. (2017). Language pressures in Japanese high schools. JALT Shiken, 21(1), 1-11. Nation, P., & Wang, M-T. (1999). Graded readers and vocabulary. Reading in a Foreign Language, 12(2), 355-380. Renandya, W. A. (2011). Extensive listening in the second language classroom. In H. P. Widodo & A. Cirocki (Eds.). Innovation and creativity in ELT methodology (pp. 2841). New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers. Richards, J. (2005). Second thoughts on teaching listening. RELC Journal, 36(1), 85-92. doi:10.1177/0033688205053484 Rost, M. (2016). Teaching and Researching Listening, 3rd ed. New York: Routledge. Saito, Y. (2019). Impacts of introducing four-skill English tests into university entrance examinations. The Language Teacher, 43(2), 9-14. doi:10.37546/JALTTLT43.2-2 Sakai, H. (2012). Effect of repetition of exposure and proficiency level in L2 listening tests. TESOL Quarterly, 43(2), 360-372. doi:10.1002/j.1545-7249.2009.tb00179.x 53 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 54 Schmidt, A. (2016). Listening journals for extensive and intensive listening practice. English Teaching Forum, 54(2), 2-11. Siegal, J. (2014). Exploring L2 listening instruction: Examinations of practice. ELT Journal, 68(1), 22-30. doi: 10.1093/elt/cct058 Siegal, J., & Siegal, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of L2 listening instruction: Making a case for bottom-up activities. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 5(4), 637-662. doi:10.14746/ssllt.2015.5.4.6 Shimauchi, S. (2018). English-medium instruction in the internationalization of higher education in Japan: Rationale and issues. Educational Studies in Japan: International Yearbook, 12(1), 77-90. doi: 10.7571/esjkyoiku.12.77 Tanewong, S. (2019). Metacognitive pedagogical sequence for less-proficient Thai EFL listeners: A comparative investigation. RELC Journal, 50(1), 86-103. doi:10.1177/0033688218754942 The Extensive Reading Foundation. (2011). The Extensive Reading Foundation's guide to extensive reading. Retrieved August 6, 2019, from erfoundation.org/guide/ERF_Guide.pdf Tholin, J. (2008). Learner autonomy, self-directed learning and assessment: Lessons from Swedish experience. Retrieved from http://bada.hb.se/bitstream/2320/3572/1/Tholin%20190508%20learner%20autonomy. pdf Vandergrift, L. (2013). Teaching listening. In C. A. Chapelle (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing. doi:10.1002/9781405198431.wbeal1169 Vandergrift, L., & Goh, C. (2012). Teaching and Learning Second Language Listening. New York, N.Y.: Routledge Press. Van Zeeland, H. (2014). Lexical inferencing in first and second language listening. Modern Language Journal, 98(4), 1006-1021. doi:10.1 111/modl. 12152 Waring, R. (n.d.). Starting extensive listening. Retrieved from http://www.robwaring.org/er/ER_info/starting_extensive_listening.htm Weaver, C., Jones, A., & Bulach, J. (2008). Comparing placement decisions based on raw test scores and Rasch ability scores. The Language Teacher, 32(6), 3-8. Webb, S., & Chang, A. (2015). How does prior word knowledge affect vocabulary learning progress in an extensive reading program? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(4), 651-675. doi: 10.1017/S0272263114000606 54 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 55 Production and Perception of Geminate Consonants in English Words by Thai Learners of English: Implications for English Teaching and Learning Suthathip Thirakunkovit Mahidol University, Thailand Bio data Suthathip Thirakunkovit is Assistant Professor in the Department of Applied Linguistics at Mahidol University, Thailand. Her current research interests cover language test development, test validation, assessment literacy, corpus linguistics, language teaching and learning, and second language writing. Her recent work can be found in the Asian EFL Journal, English for Specific Purposes, and the Journal of Asia TEFL. suthathip.thi@mahidol.edu Abstract The present study aims to explore the production and perception of geminate consonants of Thai learners of English. This study seeks to answer three research questions. First, are there any significant differences in the production of different types of English geminate consonants among five groups of participants (four groups of non-native speakers of English and one group of native speakers of English)? Second, does the level of English proficiency of the English learners affect the production and perception of English geminate consonants? Third, do English orthographic forms affect the pronunciation of Thai learners of English? To answer these three questions, the researcher compared the performance of 90 students from three different levels of language proficiency (low intermediate, high intermediate, and advanced) and of 15 Thai university instructors with the performance of eight native speakers of English. The researcher examined the performance of the participants by using two tasks: reading aloud and dictation. Even though the statistical results showed a significant difference only in the production of lexical geminates between native and non-native English speakers, it should be noted that the students from the lower levels of language proficiency seemed to have greater difficulty in identifying English geminates in regular speech. Ultimately, the researcher would like to argue that teaching pronunciation is not just about teaching students how to produce the Correspondence address: 999 Faculty of Liberal Arts, Mahidol University Phutthamonthon Sai 4 Rd. Salaya Sub-district, Phutthamonthon District, Nakhon Pathom 73170, Thailand 55 correct sounds. It is also about helping them to hear the sounds correctly, so that they can understand what they hear. EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 56 Keywords: English geminates, English teaching, EFL learners, English pronunciation Introduction When learning the pronunciation of a new language, it is possible that the sound patterns of foreign words are adapted to conform to those existing in the native phonological system (Thirakunkovit, 2020; Ying, 2016). The result of the researcher’s pilot study previously published in Thai in the Journal of Language and Culture, an academic journal in Thailand, has provided a piece of strong evidence to support this claim. In this pilot study, the majority of her participants who were Thai undergraduate students pronounced the English word happy /ˈhæpi/ as [ˈhæppi], for example. Therefore, she concluded that this mispronunciation came from the influence of the Thai syllable structure which requires two separate sounds for a coda consonant and an initial consonant of another syllable (Thirakunkovit, 2020). Yet at the same time, some people may think that the mispronunciation could come from a [pp] input of the English orthographic representation that could be seen as two distinctive geminate consonants [pp] by many second language learners. Therefore, the researcher decided to conduct this follow-up study in order to validate the results of her previous study and explore in depth whether or not English orthography has an effect on the production of English geminate consonants of Thai learners of English, and whether or not the mispronunciation of English geminate consonants has an effect on the perception of English geminate consonants. In the researcher’s previous study, she found a number of flaws with her word list and data collection procedures. Moreover, some questions related to second language acquisition and language teaching have not been addressed. Therefore, she decided to extend the scope of previous study and gather more data in order to answer her research questions in a more specific way. In this current study, the researcher has collected more data by including two more groups of participants in order to explore the effects of language proficiency on overall learner performance. The English word and sentences on the previous list have also been adjusted, so that the researcher can find more concrete evidence to answer her research questions. Moreover, in order to shed light on the perception of English geminate consonants of Thai learners, she has added a dictation task to determine whether or not Thai learners of English from four different levels of language proficiency have difficulty in identifying English geminates in natural speech. At the same time, the researcher has investigated how the discrepancy between orthographic representations and acoustic realizations influences the way Thai learners perceive English geminate consonants. 56 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 57 This language feature is chosen for investigation because it has rarely been mentioned in language classrooms. Although some may argue that mistakes in the pronunciation might not cause serious misunderstanding, learning to correctly say words or phrases that contain geminate consonants can result in more fluid and fluent sounding speech (Thirakunkovit, 2020). Moreover, flipping through more than a dozen commercial English textbooks currently available in the market, the researcher found only a few textbooks that mentioned the pronunciation of English geminates in their content (Thirakunkovit, 2020). The researcher also noticed that when she talked to her colleagues who are English teachers in college, there was only one teacher who said he tried to teach the concept of English geminates to his students; the rest have never done it at all! Therefore, the researcher feels that it is important for language teachers to guide students to this language feature. If students are aware of the characteristics of English geminate consonants, they will understand what native speakers say more easily and accurately, and at the same time they can sound more natural, which is an important goal in teaching speaking skills and successful communication (Thirakunkovit, 2020). The Effects of Orthographic Representation on the Pronunciation of L2 Learners A number of studies on the acquisition of second language pronunciation have focused on the effects of L2 orthographic representations. Many of these studies were led by Dr. Bene Bassetti from the University of Warwick. In 2015, Bassetti and Atkinson (2015) conducted an experiment to investigate whether the written representation of English words (L2) affects the pronunciation of high-school learners of English whose L1 was Italian, which is a language that makes a clear distinction between non-geminate and geminate consonants. In the experiment, Bassetti and Atkinson examined the effects of vowel and consonant spelling on their duration. To be more specific, they explored whether or not the Italian learners produced the same target vowel and consonant as longer when they are spelled with two vowel and consonant letters than with a singleton letter, i.e., whether the learners pronounced a longer [t] in kitty than in city. Their analyses revealed that the Italian EFL learners pronounced vowels and consonants longer in words spelled with double letters rather than with single letters. Another similar study was published by Bassetti, Sokolović-Perović, Mairano, and Cerni (2018). This study also investigated orthographic effects on the pronunciation of Italian learners of English. In this study, the participants were asked to produce 33 English orthographic minimal or near-minimal pairs, for example, finish and Finnish. The results also confirmed the conclusion from the previous study. Italian learners produced English word pairs in a distinctive way by a long or 57 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 58 short sound (both consonant and vowel) when the target sound was spelled with a single letter in one word and a double in the other. Therefore, it can be seen that the alternation between single and double consonants in these speakers’ production is clear evidence of the effects of orthographic representation on the pronunciation of L2 learners. Based on these studies, it can be argued that there are potential effects of L1 orthography on L2 learners’ pronunciation. That is, the errors in L2 pronunciation can be traced back to L1 orthography-phonology correspondence. Therefore, potential effects of L1 orthographic forms on the pronunciation of L2 should be considered in order to improve the teaching of L2 pronunciation. Therefore, in the current study, the researcher would like to hypothesize that English orthography might be a factor that affects the production and perception of English consonant geminates produced by Thai learners. Background Information about Geminate Consonants Geminate Consonants in Different Languages According to Crystal (2008), gemination is “a term used in phonetics and phonology for a sequence of identical adjacent SEGMENTS of a sound in a single morpheme, e.g. Italian /nɔtte/ (‘night’)” (p. 206). In phonetics and phonology, gemination in a number of languages is a process in which an articulation of a sound has been prolonged for a certain period of time compared to that of its single instance (Crystal, 2008). Geminate consonants in some languages can be understood as a double consonant. For example, in Japanese, geminate consonants are seen as a long sequence of two consonants that acts as the same segment (Davis, 1999). Gemination can occur with either consonants or vowels. In this study, however, only the gemination process occurring in consonants will be studied. Gemination of consonants occurs in a number of languages. Some are distinctive, while others are not. In some European languages such as Finnish as well as some Asian languages such as Japanese, geminations of consonants are distinctive. In other words, consonant length is necessary to distinguish words. In the Finnish language, gemination can occur with both consonants and vowels. For example, taka /taka/ means back, while takka /takka/ means fireplace, and taakka /taakka/ means burden. In the Japanese language, there is a strong contrast in consonant duration between a singleton and a geminate or a double consonant. For example, そっと /sotto/ means quietly, while そと /soto/ means outside. As shown in Figure 1, the geminate /t/ as in /sotto/ is not perceived as two sounds from two separate syllables. Shibatani (1990) described this language phenomenon as a homorganic consonant sequence which 58 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 59 consists of a coda in the first syllable and onset of the second syllable (as cited in Hardison & Saigo, 2010). Figure 1. The Phonetic Structure for the Japanese Geminates and Singletons (σ = syllable, μ = mora) Different from the aforementioned languages, gemination in English is not a distinctive feature. English gemination is usually indicated in writing by double consonants. Even though there are two letters, they are pronounced together as one sound (one singleton). In other words, there is only one onset with one release. Some examples of geminate consonants in English include /ˈhæpi/ happy, /ˈbɒsi/ bossy, /ˈrʌnər/ runner, and /ˈhæmə/ hammer. For English gemination, it is particularly important to note that it is still controversial whether the single /p/ in happy and the single /m/ in hammer belong to the coda of the first syllable of the word or the onset of the second syllable (McCully, 2009). In other words, should we syllabify the words as ha.py and ha.mer or hap.y and ham.er, respectively? Even though many L2 learners might be tempted to syllabify the words as hap.py and ham.mer, they are, of course, impossible and considered ill forms for native speakers of English. McCully (2009) has tried to solve this issue by proposing the term “ambisyllabicity” to refer to a process when an intervocalic consonant belongs to both syllables simultaneously. Based on the concept proposed, the structure for the word hammer looks like the following: 59 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 60 Figure 2. The Sound Structure of the Word hammer This language feature can pose a problem to non-native speakers of English because they may intuitively pronounce these double consonants with the length that is twice as much as one singleton [ˈhæppi] due to the orthographic representation happy. Even though it is true that the production of these geminates with double consonant length may not affect the meaning in most instances, they may sound odd and confuse English-speaking listeners momentarily (Thirakunkovit, 2020). In the worst-case scenario, a communication breakdown or misunderstanding can occur if the learners cannot perceive geminate consonants in normal English speech. Therefore, it is important for language teachers to teach this language feature explicitly in order to increase the level of production and perception accuracy of learners from all levels (Thompson & Gaddes, 2005). Classification of Geminate Consonants in English In English, there are three kinds of geminates: (1) lexical, (2) assimilated, and (3) concatenated (Lahiri & Hankamer, 1988). Lexical geminates occur in the lexicon. In other words, they are part of the phonemic inventor (e.g. /ˈhɒli/ holly and /ˈgræmər/ grammar). Similarly, assimilated geminates occur within the same words, but through morphological processes i.e., affixation. For example, in the word unknown the affixing of un with its final [n] and known with its initial [n], the consonant /n/ becomes /nn/ (Ball & Rahilly, 2014). On the other hand, concatenated geminates occur between words when two adjacent consonants are identical such as the phrase bad day. In this case when the two /d/s are adjacent, it is possible that the speakers will merge them together with a long hold phase in a natural conversation speech. In other words, the first /d/ becomes unreleased and the second /d/ has merged with the first /d/. When two identical plosives have merged, a sequence of homorganic plosives is said to have occurred (Knight, 2012). 60 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 61 Gemination in Thai Thai is a language which contrasts only short and long vowels (e.g. /la/ means to omit vs. /la:/ means a donkey). In other words, there is no gemination for Thai consonants. Even though the lengths of the same consonants might vary in natural speech, this is simply due to the manner of articulation of a particular consonant (e.g. a plosive vs. a lateral), the voicing of a particular consonant (e.g. /b/ vs. /p/), and the tone and stress of a syllable. In any case, they do not affect the meaning. As there is no geminate consonant in the Thai language, Thai learners might not be aware of this linguistic feature and produce it differently from native speakers of English. Research Aims The present study specifically investigates the duration of geminate consonant production in native Thai learners of English. Specifically, the researcher would like to investigate timing differences in the production of different types of English geminate consonants between native and non-native speakers of English. Moreover, this study explores whether the level of English proficiency of Thai learners affects the production and perception of English geminate consonants. These objectives have addressed the following research questions: 1. Are there any significant differences in the production of different types of English geminate consonants among five groups of participants (four groups of non-native speakers of English and one group of native speakers of English)? 2. Does the level of English proficiency of Thai learners affect the production and perception of English geminate consonants? 3. Do English orthographic forms affect the pronunciation of Thai learners of English? Research Methodology Participants This study includes three major groups of participants – 90 Thai EFL students, 15 Thai instructors of English, and eight native English speakers. The total number of participants was 113. All potential participants were approached via email to ensure voluntary participation. The first group of participants included 90 undergraduate students from three sub-groups: English majors, non-English majors of low intermediate proficiency, and non-English majors of high intermediate proficiency. The researcher first contacted course coordinators of several English courses and asked whether she could come to their classes to recruit their students. The courses were chosen from the requirement of minimum English scores from the national admission test, which could be further classified as low intermediate, high intermediate, and 61 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 62 advanced. For students who expressed their interest in participating in the study, they were given a direct link to the consent form. After consenting, they were asked to complete a short demographics survey concerning their educational background and provide contact information. The researcher tried her best to have an equal number of participants in each group. The second group included 15 university instructors teaching in the English or Applied Linguistics Programs at the Faculty of Liberal Arts. All of them were native speakers of Thai. The average length of their teaching career was ten years. About half of them graduated from a university in the United States of America. The third group included eight native speakers of English currently teaching English at the university level. Four of them were American and the other four were British. The data elicited from the eight native English speakers were used as a benchmark for performance. The reason why the number of native speakers of English in this study was quite small is because only eight native English-speaking instructors were available at the time of data collection. Research Tool The present study examines the performance of the participants in two tasks: reading aloud and dictation. Reading Aloud A list for the reading aloud task includes 24 English single words, eight English noun phrases, and five English sentences. The prepared list is shown in Appendix A. The list was prepared solely by the researcher based on previous studies of English geminations. Since no studies have previously been conducted to explore the pattern of geminate consonants of English words produced by Thai learners, as mentioned earlier, the researcher previously did a pilot study on a group of approximately 60 Thai participants who were undergraduate students in order to ensure the validity of the instructions and the research tool. After the pilot study, the instructions and the words on the list had been slightly adjusted for the current study. Twenty three word stimuli contained two identical geminate consonants in their orthographic representation. Twelve words were lexical geminates, and 11 words were assimilated geminates. Seven words contained /p, t, k, b, d/ stop geminate consonants. Six words contained /m, n/ nasal geminate consonants. Four words contained /s, f/ fricative geminate consonants. Six words contained /l, r/ liquid geminate consonants. The last word copy does not contain any geminate consonants. All of them were words with relatively high frequency in both spoken and written language. For the stimuli at the phrasal level, three phrases 62 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 63 contained /p, d/ stop geminates, three phrases contained /m, n/ nasal geminates, one phrase contained /s/ fricative geminates, and one phrase contained /l/ liquid geminates. Finally, stimuli at the sentential level consisted of five sentences that contained all three kinds of geminate consonants -- lexical, assimilated, and concatenated -- with a heavier focus on word-boundary levels in the analysis. For the group of Thai participants, a list of 6 two-syllable Thai words was also added into the current study for a comparison between Thai and English pronunciation. The first syllable of every word ended in a consonant sound and the following sound began with the same consonant sound. The reason why a list of Thai words has been added into the current study is because the researcher would like to explore whether the native language of the participants might influence the consonant length of English words. In Thai, when the final consonant of the first syllable and the initial consonant of the second syllable are the same, they are always pronounced as two separate sounds (CVC.CVC). Dictation After the first task, all participants in the current study were required to do a dictation task. This task consisted of eight sentences. All of them were relatively short, ranging from three to eight words. The first sentence I love you was a warm-up item that was used to make sure that the participants were ready for the dictation task and could hear the sound clearly. The rest of the sentences had at least one word that contained geminate consonants. The dictation task contained all three kinds of geminates -- lexical geminates e.g. puppy and summer; assimilated geminates e.g. illegal and immigrant; and concatenated geminates e.g. one night and lab boy. Every target word and phrase is presented in a meaningful sentence and can be interpreted within the sentence. 63 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 64 Table 1 Items Tested in Each Sentence Sentence (Number of Words) Items Tested Type of Geminates warm-up item — one night Concatenated loves signing Concatenated 4. The lab boy has an irregular work schedule. (8) lab boy irregular Concatenated Assimilated 5. My dad gave me a puppy last summer. (8) puppy summer Lexical Lexical 6. It takes time to love someone. (6) It takes Concatenated John needs needs support support Concatenated Concatenated Lexical illegal immigrant Assimilated Assimilated 1. I love you. (3) 2. I ran into him one night. (6) 3. Bill loves singing. (3) 7. John needs support. (3) 8. He is an illegal immigrant. (5) Research Procedures This study took place at a large public university in Thailand. All participants were required to complete two task types: reading aloud and dictation. The two tasks given to every participant followed the same order. The researcher hired two research assistants to help ensure that data collection followed the same procedures. In the first task, all participants were given a printed list of material (See Appendix A). The participants were given approximately 10 to 15 minutes of silent reading to make sure that they could do the reading aloud task correctly and confidently. Then, during the recording session, participants were asked to produce each word, phrase, and sentence twice. The words, phrases, and sentences on the list came in a different randomized sequence in order to prevent order effect. For each recording, the participants were instructed to produce the speech at a natural speaking rate. The participants were asked to put each word and phrase in the carrier sentence, “Please say the word/phrase __________ again.” and were asked to read it two times. Between the two readings, there was a three-second pause. Only the second production of each pair was used in the analysis. The researcher demonstrated what the desirable speech rate was for the reading aloud task, which is approximately 130 words per minute for a normal speaking 64 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 65 rate. She also asked the participants to imitate the speech rate. During the recording session, one of the research assistants was asked to monitor the consistency of the participants’ speech rate, pitch, and tempo. The responses of all participants were recorded in a soundproof recording studio. The utterances were recorded onto audio files using a professional stereo microphone. In the second task, the participants were asked to listen to eight English sentences produced by two native speakers of English: a male and female. In this task, all participants were asked to write down what was being said as accurately as possible. Each sentence was said twice. After the participants heard a sentence, they were given 20 seconds to write it down. The first four items were spoken by a male voice, and the last four items were spoken by a female voice. The speech obtained from the native speakers was tested for auditory intelligibility by the researcher and a native speaker of English who was not a participant in this study. The purpose of the second task is to test whether they could understand the native speakers’ pronunciation that contained geminate consonants and then write down the sentences they heard. Even though the participants were supposed to write down exactly what they heard, neither punctuation nor capitalization was scored. Both tasks were completed on the same day with a five-minute break in between. Data Analyses The data were analyzed with two computer programs. For acoustic analyses, Praat, a publicly available software for the analysis of speech, was used to identify the acoustic duration of geminate consonants. The duration of geminate consonants was measured from the offset of a preceding vowel to the onset of a vowel that immediately followed the geminate consonants (Thirakunkovit, 2020). If the speaker paused between words, the total duration between the preceding and following vowels was calculated. For fricative consonants, the total duration was measured from the start of the formant to the end when the formant suddenly disappeared or became remarkably weaker. For each word, the mean duration of the target sound was obtained from the measures of the second production. Twenty percent of the data were double-measured by a second person who was a phonetician to ensure the accuracy of Praat analysis. All disagreements were resolved during discussion. Next, SPSS software was used to calculate the means and standard deviations of the duration of geminate consonants produced by the participants, and a one-way ANOVA was run to determine whether there were any differences in significant timing for the mean durations of 65 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 66 different types of English geminate consonants between native and non-native speakers of English. For the data from the dictation task, the responses were analyzed for errors and the causes of those errors attributed to the misperception of English pronunciation. A one-way ANOVA was run to investigate whether there were any significant differences among the five groups of participants. The researcher’s marking scheme consisted of marking a word as either correct (one full mark) or incorrect (no mark) on the basis of whether the participants could correctly identify words or phrases that contained geminate consonants. In other words, the participants did not receive any points if they were unable to identify those words and phrases. The researcher ignored minor spelling errors. For example, if a student wrote puppi for puppy, no mark was deducted since it does not require a substantial amount of judgment. No half marks were awarded for this task. To put the current marking system into perspective, an example of marking results is given below. Correct answer: My dad gave me a puppy last summer. Participant 1: My dad gave me a puppi(1) last summer(1). Correct answer: The lab boy has an irregular work schedule. Participant 2: The last boy(0) has an regular(0) work schedule. With the system of scoring set at 0 or 1 for each word or phrase containing geminate consonants, Participant 1 would receive a score of 2 even though she made a spelling mistake. However, Participant 2 would receive a zero because it appears that Participant 2 was not able to identify the word and phrase that contained English geminates. With this marking system, the researcher believes that the score differential can reflect the ability of the learners in identifying geminate consonants. Results The Results from the Reading Aloud Task In the analysis, sounds that came from mispronunciation of the target words or phrases were eliminated from the study. The average duration of each word and phrase produced by each group of participants is shown in Appendix B. Figure 3 shows the results of mean duration across groups. The error bars indicate standard deviations of the dataset. 66 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 67 Figure 3. Mean Duration of Three Kinds of Geminates across Groups For all three kinds of geminates, only the mean duration of lexical geminates indicated significant differences among groups, F(4,108) = 3.17, p = .01. Post hoc analyses using Tukey HSD for significance indicated that the mean duration for native speakers of English was significantly different from those of low and high intermediate students (p < .01 and p = .03, respectively). Even though the means of advanced students and English instructors did not show significant differences from that of native speakers, the mean duration of these two groups was higher than that of native speakers. It should be noted that the findings of the ANOVA test should be interpreted with caution because the sample sizes of each group are unequal. If we examine the waveforms and spectrograms in Praat of some lexical geminates of the native and non-native speakers, some differences in the articulation can be found. One obvious difference can be seen from the plosive p in the word pippin. The non-native speaker produced it with a much longer duration of geminate and a complete closure in between (0.179 vs. 0.097). See Figure 4. One interpretation can be that this speaker saw the [pp] input as two separate consonants, thereby producing the sound relatively longer than it should be. However, for the native speaker of English, the duration is shorter, and some voicing can still be seen, as indicated by greater darkness between the two vowels. See Figure 5. 67 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 68 Figure 4. Waveform and Spectrogram Display of a Non-Native Speaker of English for pippin Figure 5. Waveform and Spectrogram Display for a Native Speaker of English for pippin Another obvious difference can be seen from the production of the nasal formant. From Figure 6, it seems that the Thai learner produced the word innate with a double n sound because the spectrogram shows a quick closure between the two syllables. On the other hand, the native speaker of English pronounced innate with one [n] (Figure 7). 68 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 69 Figure 6. Waveform and Spectrogram Display of a Non-Native Speaker of English for innate Figure 7. Waveform and Spectrogram Display for a Native Speaker of English for innate When investigating the mean durations of assimilated and concatenated geminates, even though the results of the one-way ANOVA did not indicate any significant differences between the native and non-native speakers of English, the non-native groups had a tendency to pronounce them with an average of 0.04 seconds longer than the native speakers of English. Moreover, the results from the English reading aloud task has revealed the effects of orthography on the pronunciation of consonant durations in the L2 English of L1 Thai speakers. The mean lengths of the [p] sound in the word floppy being pronounced longer than that of [p] 69 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 70 in the word copy in every group of Thai learners. However, this claim should be further investigated by including other minimal pairs in future studies. Apart from the investigation of gemination in English words, the current study also investigates the closure duration of Thai words that contain the same consonant between two syllables (e.g. /khan-naa/ คันนา, /paak-kaa/ ปากกา, /ŋoŋ-ŋuai/ งงงวย) and only one consonant in their orthography between two syllables (e.g. /kin-na-rii/ กินรี and /tham-ma/ ธรรมะ). From the results of mean durations, we can see that the means of all kinds of English geminate consonants, especially concatenated geminates and Thai words are actually very similar across proficiency groups (See Figure 8 and Appendix C). Therefore, we might be able to see some L1 influence on L2 pronunciation. Figure 8: Mean Durations of Thai words when compared with English Geminates The Results from the Dictation Task In order to explore whether the level of language proficiency has a significant effect on the perception of English geminates, responses from the dictation task were investigated. Table 2 shows the mean scores and standard deviations across five groups. A one-way ANOVA showed that there is a significant effect of the level of language proficiency on the participants’ performance, F(4,108) = 30.23, p < .00. Post hoc analyses using Tukey HSD post hoc criterion for significance indicated that every pairwise comparison of mean scores among the groups of lower proficiency (Low Intermediate and High Intermediate) were significantly different from the higher (Advanced, English Instructors, and Native Speakers of English)ones with p-values less than .00, except for the comparisons between the advanced and English instructor groups (p = .46), the advanced and native speaker groups (p = .45) and the English instructor and native speaker groups (p = .99) 70 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 71 Table 2 Results of the Dictation Task Students Native Speakers of English Low Intermediate High Intermediate Advanced English Instructors Means 7.5 9.5 11.17 12.73 13.0 SD 2.10 2.3 2.07 0.59 0 Table 3 shows the percentages of participants who could write down the words or phrases correctly. Even though the percentages of correct answers for each test item varied widely among the five groups, the overall accuracy rates increased as proficiency levels increased. The participants from the low intermediate level had difficulty in identifying English geminates, especially when the vocabulary or expressions were unfamiliar. Even though their responses in general matched the original at both the syntactic and semantic levels, they showed poor results in geminate perception with less than 40% accuracy rates. However, the participants from the high intermediate and advanced levels had better performance perceiving English geminates correctly, at approximately 60% and 80% accuracy, respectively, during the second hearing. For the English instructors, their accuracy rates approached 100% for most of the dictation items during the second hearing. For native speakers of English, their responses were 100% accurate during the second hearing. 71 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 72 Table 3 Percentages of Correct Responses of the Participants Students Items Tested High Intermediate Low Intermediate Advanced English Instructors Native Speakers of English 1st 2nd 1st 2nd 1st 2nd 1st 2nd 1st 2nd Warm up 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% one night 52.5% 57.5% 60.5% 73.5% 85% 95% 93% 100% 100% 100% Bill loves 82.5% 90% 81.5% 97% 83% 83% 93% 100% 100% 100% loves singing 35% 35% 63% 66% 73% 76% 86% 100% 100% 100% lab boy 0% 2.5% 2.5% 10.5% 7% 19.5% 40% 46% 87.5% 100% irregular 10% 20% 34% 55% 44% 54% 53% 66% 100% 100% puppy 45% 50% 63% 79% 85% 95% 100% 100% 100% 100% summer 80% 85% 87% 97% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% It takes 65% 90% 79% 90% 97.5% 100% 93% 100% 100% 100% John needs 35% 50% 47% 58% 73% 80% 93% 100% 100% 100% needs support 17.5% 25% 50% 53% 71% 80% 93% 100% 100% 100% support 87.5% 82.5% 81.5% 84% 90% 97.5% 100% 100% 100% 100% illegal 5% 7.5% 26% 24% 49% 56% 53% 66% 87.5% 100% 7.5% 15% 39.5% 37% 85% 88% 93% 100% 100% 100% immigrant In the second step of the analysis of the dictation task, the answers of the participants were analyzed according to the errors made. The following examples are the misperception commonly found in the answers: Original Misperception irregular regular illegal legal immigrant migrant lab boy last boy / rap boy one night night / tonight 72 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 73 In investigating the errors the participants made in taking dictation, it is quite clear that a number of participants could not accurately perceive the variation of in- geminates and geminates in word boundaries that are not commonly found in everyday speech. “He is an illegal immigrant.” and “The lab boy has an irregular work schedule.” are two items from the dictation section which clearly demonstrate this argument. These poor accuracy rates might result from the fact that the production of consonant geminates by native speakers of English is subtly different from the sound sequences in the Thai language, which are strung together with obvious boundaries between them. When looking at the errors made by the Thai participants, the non-salient nature of unstressed English syllables seems to be another factor that affects their perception (Suntornsawet, 2019), making them unaware of the geminate sounds. A number of participants misperceived unstressed geminate consonants as singleton consonants, i.e., illegal as legal and irregular as regular. As the Thai language does not have unstressed syllables/words like English does, it is especially difficult for Thai English learners to notice these geminate sounds. In order to improve the perception of English geminates, the researcher would like to suggest the use of explicit instruction and a dictation task with input enhancement to draw the attention of the learners to this language feature. Conclusions The results of the reading aloud and dictation tasks reported in this study seemed to show the influence of both English orthography and L1 interference on the production and perception of English geminate consonants of Thai learners. Moreover, the use of Thai reading aloud task has shown a possible influence of the native language because the closure duration of Thai words that contain the same consonant between two syllables are very similar to those of the mean durations of English words that contain geminate consonants. The spectrogram of the pronunciation of some English words such as innate (Figure 6) shows a quick closure between the two syllables, which resembles the pronunciation of Thai syllable structure (e.g. /paak-kaa/ CVVC.CVV). Finally, the researcher believes that these two assumptions can help explain why a number of Thai learners in this study could not accurately identify English geminate consonants in natural speech. Implications for English Teaching Research related to teaching pronunciation has started to gain more attention in recent years (Lightbown & Spada, 2013). One question is whether intelligibility or native-like production 73 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 74 should be a standard that L2 learners strive for. Even though a number of studies show that the presence of foreign accents does not necessarily result in reduced intelligibility or comprehension (Breitkreutz, Derwing, & Rossiter, 2001; Jenkins, 2006), some research evidence shows that some listeners negatively react to second language learners’ pronunciation (Breitkreutz, Derwing, & Rossiter, 2001; Munro & Derwing, 2011). On many occasions, we cannot deny the fact that accent can serve as a marker of group membership and may be used as a basis for discrimination. Therefore, the researcher believes that second language learners should be motivated to learn the correct pronunciation right at the start of their study. This has become a challenge. How can learners change their view of speaking in English if they view target-like pronunciation as an unrealistic or unnecessary goal at the moment? Should a combination of instruction that aims at a target-like level, exposure, and motivation be used in classrooms? And how can language teachers provide guidance and emphasize the importance of native-like pronunciation when preparing their students for interacting with native speakers of English or speakers who use English as a lingua franca? Although the teaching of English pronunciation might not be a top priority for some language teachers, pronunciation is an important aspect of learning English due to the growing needs of cross-cultural communication and social integration. Some language learners may find it useful to imitate pronunciation samples of native speakers’ speech. Therefore, classroom teachers should ask their students to spend time carefully listening to native speakers’ speech and then imitate what they have heard. However, in doing this, the researcher has no intention of sending a strong message that language teachers have to use only native-like pronunciation as the sole model. Instead, the researcher wants to point out that language teachers should at least use native-like pronunciation as general guidelines to help their students correct or adjust their pronunciation in order to increase intelligibility and improve their listening skills. In this case, the researcher would like to recommend the use of explicit instructions to elicit correct pronunciation. If the teachers can draw their students’ attention to this specific feature, the researcher believes that it can result in permanent changes in their productions. In the case of teaching English geminates, teachers should explicitly tell their students that they should avoid the temptation to lengthen a double consonant letter because, most of the time, it is simply pronounced as one short consonant. Even though there might be some exceptions to geminates in the morpheme and word boundary patterns such as immigrant and prime minister, the teachers should emphasize that those lengthened consonants rarely occur in casual speech. For example, the word prime minister is usually pronounced as [prʌɪ mɪnɪstə] rather than [prʌɪm mɪnɪstə], and the expression gimme (and informal version of give me), is actually pronounced 74 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 75 [gɪmi] with only one [m]. Moreover, consonants after the [i] in words such as illogical, immoral, irregular are pronounced as singletons. For pedagogical pronunciation approaches, there are several techniques that language teachers can incorporate in their classes. They can choose to integrate this language feature into their curriculum, or to devote 10 to 15 minutes of class time to this specific pronunciation instruction. The teachers can begin by introducing the concept of English geminates. This allows the students to realize that the occurrence of a double consonant letter in English spelling does not correspond with single consonant articulation. When the teachers want to integrate the theoretical concept of English geminates into pronunciation practice, their activities need to be based on real-life situations, so that the students can transfer their linguistic behavior beyond the classroom. For example, the teachers may start from contexts that are familiar to students, such as using the sentence “This is a good dress.” as an example of English geminates when teaching students how to give a compliment to someone’s dress, or they can use pop songs to help students hear geminate consonants better. In the 2013 song ‘Happy,’ Pharrell Williams sings: Because I'm happy =>/ˈhæpi/ Clap along if you feel like that's what you want to do =>/fiːlaɪk/, /wɑːntə/ From the song, the teachers may ask the students to identify how many consonants they have heard in happy, feel like, and want to. After that, the students can be encouraged to produce English germinate consonants through the teacher backchaining models for pronunciation. Once the concept of English geminates is firmly established, the teachers can introduce English geminates that occur in less familiar contexts or those in unstressed syllables such as illegal. Nevertheless, the teachers should be reminded that the native speakers’ pronunciation should be held as a reference point only. The goal of their pronunciation instruction is not to achieve a native-like accent, but to help their students become more comprehensible, especially when interacting with native speakers of English. Even though some may argue that the mispronunciation of English geminates, such as mispronouncing /ˈhæpi/ as [ˈhæppi], should not create misunderstandings in communication in most cases, the mistake could make them sound strange and definitely not English-like. Therefore, the researcher believes that explicit instructions on both production and perception should go hand in hand in order to enhance the learners’ acquisition of English geminates. Teachers should recognize pronunciation errors and expose students to the correct 75 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 76 pronunciation through extensive drills and exercises, so that students will have opportunities to improve their pronunciation and perception. Limitations and Directions for Future Research There are two major limitations of this study that could be addressed in future research. First, the number of native English speakers was very small, and they were sampled from only two English-speaking countries. Therefore, the results gained from this group might not be representative of the population. Second, there is still a lack of prior research on the production and perception of English consonant geminates by Thai learners of English. Therefore, some of the phonological issues related to Thai learners of English might not be fully addressed. Acknowledgments This study is fully supported by a research grant of Mahidol University, Thailand. 76 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 77 References Ball, M. J., & Rahilly, J. (2014). Phonetics: The Science of Speech. New York: Routledge. Bassetti, B., & Atkinson, N. (2015). Effects of orthographic forms on pronunciation in experienced instructed second language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(1), 6791. Bassetti, B., Sokolović-Perović, M., Mairano, P., & Cerni, T. (2018). Orthography-induced length contrasts in the second language phonological systems of L2 speakers of English: Evidence from minimal pairs. Language and Speech, 61(4), 577-597. Breitkreutz, J., Derwing, T. M., & Rossiter, M. J. (2001). Pronunciation teaching practices in Canada. TESL Canada Journal, 19(1), 51-61. Collins, B. S., & Mees, I. M. (2013). Practical Phonetics and Phonology: A Resource Book for Students. New York: Routledge. Crystal, D. (2008). A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics (6th ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. Davis, S. (1999). On the representation of initial geminates. Phonology, 16(1)93-104. Jenkins, J. (2006). The spread of EIL: A testing time for testers. ELT Journal, 60(1), 42-50. Hardison, D. M., & Saigo, M. M. (2010). Development of perception of second language Japanese geminates: Role of duration, sonority, and segmentation strategy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(1), 81-99. Hazan, V., Sennema, A., Iba, M., & Faulkner, A. (2005). Effect of audiovisual perceptual training on the perception and production of consonants by Japanese learners of English. Speech Communication, 47(3), 360-378. Knight, R. A. (2012). Phonetics: A Coursebook. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lahiri, A., & Hankamer, J. (1988). The timing of geminate consonants. Journal of Phonetics, 16, 327-338. Li, F. (2016). Contrastive study between pronunciation Chinese L1 and English L2 from the perspective of interference based on observations in genuine teaching contexts. English Language Teaching, 9(10), 90-100. Lightbown, P. M., & Spada, N. (2013). How Languages are Learned 4th edition - Oxford Handbooks for Language Teachers. Oxford: Oxford University Press. McCully, C. (2009). The Sound Structure of English: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Munro, M. J., & Derwing, T. M. (2011). The foundations of accent and intelligibility in pronunciation research. Language Teaching, 44(3), 316-327. 77 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 78 Pojprasat, S., & Thirakunkovit, S. (2018). An exploratory study of Thai natives’ gemination in English words. Veridian E-Journal, 11(1), 1103-1119. Suntornsawet, J. (2019). Problematic phonological features of foreign accented English pronunciation as threats to international intelligibility: Thai EIL pronunciation core. Journal of English as an International Language, 14(2), 72-93. Thirakunkovit, S. (2020). Production of English geminate consonants by Thai learners. Journal of Language and Culture, 39(1), 19-36. Thompson, T., & Gaddes, M. (2005). The importance of teaching pronunciation to adult learners. The Asian EFL Journal, 2(1), 1-11. Tsukada, K. (2009). Durational characteristics of English vowels produced by Japanese and Thai second language (L2) learners. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 29(2), 287-299. Ying, L. (2016). Degree of Foreign Accent in English Production by Japanese, Thai and Italian Adults and Children. The Asian EFL Journal, 95, 75-90. 78 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 79 Appendix A Test Materials for the Reading Aloud Task Task 1: Reading Aloud Instructions: Look at the following words and phrases. Read each word and phrase twice within the sentence “Please say the word/phrase __________ again.” at a normal speed. happy hobby support spelling floppy runner slipper misspell cancellation bossy pippin ladder effect irresponsible illegal yellow unknown ammonia curriculum differ innate immoral unnamed copy top pick one nurse nice sock bad day good dress fun name beautiful life dim morning Instructions: Look at the following words. Read each word twice within the sentence “กรุณาพูดคาว่า __________ อีกครัง้ ” at a normal speed. คันนา /khan-naa/ กินรี /kin-na-rii/ กินนอน /kin-nɔɔn/ ปากกา /paak-kaa/ ธรรมะ /tham-ma/ งงงวย /ŋoŋ-ŋuai/ Instructions: Look at the following sentences. Read each sentence twice at a normal speed. 1. He became the prime minister in 2000. 2. This address seems wrong. 3. It makes sense to me. 4. Jill loves to go jogging in the morning. 5. Johnny is a calm man. Task 2: Dictation 79 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 80 Instructions: Listen to the recording and write down what you hear. You will hear each item twice. You have 20 seconds to write down your answer for each hearing. 1. First hearing: …………………………………………………………………..….. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 2. First hearing: …………………………………………………………………….... Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 3. First hearing: …………………………………………………………………..….. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 4. First hearing: …………………………………………………………………...…. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 5. First hearing: …………………………………………………………………...…. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 6. First hearing: …………………………………………………………………...…. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 7. First hearing: ………………………………………………………………...……. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 8. First hearing: ……………………………………………………………...………. Second hearing: ……………………………………………………………………. 80 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 81 Appendix B Means and SD Durations of Items Tested Students Items Tested Low Intermediate High Intermediate Advanced English Instructors Native Speakers of English Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD happy 0.166 0.039 0.151 0.028 0.159 0.030 0.136 0.033 0.103 0.008 pippin 0.178 0.047 0.179 0.043 0.168 0.036 0.173 0.047 0.105 0.025 support 0.199 0.036 0.189 0.034 0.179 0.033 0.290 0.442 0.152 0.040 slipper 0.197 0.038 0.197 0.038 0.214 0.031 0.219 0.036 0.095 0.012 floppy 0.198 0.061 0.177 0.052 0.173 0.036 0.150 0.085 0.085 0.076 ladder 0.113 0.048 0.095 0.046 0.083 0.036 0.069 0.025 0.047 0.021 hobby 0.164 0.038 0.151 0.046 0.134 0.029 0.122 0.028 0.071 0.022 runner 0.144 0.031 0.116 0.027 0.117 0.025 0.111 0.024 0.050 0.006 innate 0.159 0.032 0.141 0.036 0.131 0.028 0.125 0.034 0.067 0.007 unknown 0.164 0.039 0.144 0.026 0.153 0.027 0.159 0.022 0.176 0.027 unnamed 0.184 0.034 0.161 0.025 0.158 0.027 0.163 0.029 0.171 0.025 immoral 0.147 0.028 0.144 0.026 0.141 0.029 0.155 0.038 0.116 0.026 ammonia 0.128 0.031 0.118 0.025 0.125 0.023 0.122 0.026 0.087 0.006 differ 0.176 0.027 0.207 0.231 0.161 0.024 0.179 0.043 0.129 0.011 copy 0.160 0.055 0.162 0.054 0.157 0.044 0.123 0.066 0.099 0.050 effect 0.182 0.033 0.159 0.032 0.151 0.027 0.159 0.015 0.140 0.024 misspell 0.130 0.039 0.135 0.042 0.137 0.045 0.153 0.062 0.153 0.019 bossy 0.166 0.030 0.158 0.029 0.154 0.028 0.164 0.029 0.151 0.010 irresponsible 0.087 0.032 0.070 0.012 0.080 0.032 0.074 0.029 0.077 0.027 spelling 0.085 0.033 0.087 0.025 0.087 0.026 0.088 0.031 0.054 0.000 yellow 0.115 0.101 0.091 0.030 0.095 0.028 0.087 0.021 0.057 0.009 illegal 0.098 0.043 0.102 0.035 0.108 0.031 0.114 0.023 0.088 0.008 cancellation 0.075 0.031 0.080 0.020 0.074 0.020 0.079 0.025 0.062 0.012 curriculum 0.053 0.019 0.061 0.019 0.064 0.024 0.061 0.017 0.039 0.004 top pick 0.225 0.069 0.237 0.080 0.213 0.078 0.261 0.103 0.229 0.045 bad day 0.157 0.030 0.162 0.061 0.156 0.038 0.172 0.031 0.177 0.029 81 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 82 Students Items Tested Low Intermediate High Intermediate Advanced English Instructors Native Speakers of English Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD good dress 0.172 0.041 0.179 0.066 0.158 0.045 0.189 0.066 0.177 0.019 one nurse 0.185 0.032 0.192 0.081 0.164 0.057 0.200 0.099 0.164 0.029 fun name 0.204 0.042 0.207 0.114 0.170 0.055 0.186 0.058 0.181 0.040 dim morning 0.170 0.064 0.181 0.068 0.165 0.045 0.225 0.094 0.199 0.023 nice sock 0.190 0.079 0.215 0.133 0.163 0.043 0.240 0.119 0.211 0.060 beautiful life 0.078 0.022 0.093 0.024 0.093 0.030 0.101 0.041 0.078 0.008 He became the prime minister in 2000. 0.133 0.105 0.086 0.022 0.088 0.026 0.153 0.208 0.096 0.013 Johnny is a calm man. 0.150 0.032 0.183 0.059 0.147 0.022 0.191 0.091 0.151 0.018 This address seems 0.312 wrong. 0.134 0.323 0.115 0.257 0.151 0.309 0.143 0.200 0.033 It makes sense to me. 0.234 0.085 0.234 0.085 0.189 0.072 0.222 0.075 0.173 0.024 Jill loves to go jogging in 0.180 the morning. 0.087 0.157 0.082 0.124 0.052 0.143 0.070 0.107 0.047 82 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 83 Appendix C Means and SD Durations of Thai Words Students Thai words Low Intermediate High Intermediate Mean Mean SD English Instructors Advanced SD Mean SD Mean SD คันนา /khan-naa/ 0.193 0.030 0.180 0.030 0.178 0.022 0.195 0.042 กินนอน /kin-nɔɔn/ 0.205 0.038 0.200 0.050 0.191 0.026 0.229 0.064 กินรี /kin-na-rii/ 0.106 0.032 0.101 0.030 0.093 0.025 0.102 0.022 ธรรมะ /tham-ma/ 0.200 0.032 0.182 0.045 0.177 0.022 0.200 0.032 ปากกา /paak-kaa/ 0.194 0.044 0.182 0.043 0.179 0.024 0.203 0.033 งงงวย /ŋoŋ-ŋuai/ 0.182 0.042 0.219 0.065 0.192 0.037 0.280 0.128 83 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 84 English-medium Instruction and Translanguaging BethAnne Palsrud, Tian, Zhongfeng and Jeanette Toth (Eds). Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 2020. ISBN 13: 978-1-78892-731-4 (pbk). Pp. 208. Reviewed by Marilyn Lewis, Honorary Research Fellow, The University of Auckland The status of the English language in the world and, more specifically in classrooms internationally, has been the focus of several recent articles and books. In a volume edited by Sifakis and Tsantila (2019), contributors from countries where English is not the first language, such as Germany, Greece, Portugal and Turkey, as well as writers from English language countries, reported on the place of English in their classrooms. Marr and English (2019) also investigated questions relating to TESOL teachers and the language they use with their students, building partly on their own experiences. Most recently, in this journal, Tamin (2021) discussed differences between policies and practices in Pakistan. Perhaps these sources came out too late to be referenced by the present editors, since none of them are listed here. This most recent book also draws on contributors’ experiences in many countries. It has 11 chapters plus a foreword, introduction, epilogue and conclusion. One of the collection’s refreshing aspects is its inclusion of teachers’ voices from countries whose voices we hear less often. (I use the words “less often” to be safe, but in my case the truer word would be “never”.) First, a couple of definitions. The term EMI is used for contexts where English is used exclusively as a medium of instruction. When it comes to the title’s term of ‘translanguaging’ the writer of the foreword speaks of “the multiple ways in which it is understood” (p. xvi). More than one article in the book mentions that the word was originally coined in Welsh in the 1980s to describe what happened in Welsh-English bilingual classrooms. The varied descriptions from chapter to chapter confirm the difficulty of coming up with one, simple definition for this word which, very briefly, involves the use of multiple languages. Since the book is not divided into sections nor arranged in any special chronology, the reader can feel free to pick chapters in any order. In my case, curiosity had me starting with countries I knew least about. In the Maldives, a Muslim country, children are exposed to Arabic and English as well as their local language. The widely published writer, Naashia Mohamed, starts by outlining how and why English came to have status in the Maldives to the point where it is now the medium of instruction for all subjects except the other two language classes. Given 84 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 85 the careful details about this case study of four friends aged between six and eleven telling stories, readers wanting to replicate it in their own country have a strong starting point. For a study of a different age group and country there is the chapter on Khazakhstani students at an English-medium university, One of the three authors, Sulusash Kerimkulova, speaks from her starting point of more than 40 years’ experience of teaching and research in that country. The chapter opens with some more definitions of translanguaging, which the three authors describe as “both an approach to teaching and learning, and the application of that learning” (p. 141). The ’trans’ part of the term must have special meaning in a country which, apart from Khazak and Russian, has 130 ethnic groups. One interesting feature of this study is that it reports students’ own views about which languages they chose to study and why. Next I turned to places that were familiar to me, starting with a Cambodian study, the country where I once taught at a university in the early 70s. At the unnamed Phnom Penh university in this 2009 study, bilingual students and staff in the M.Ed programme were surveyed for their translanguaging attitudes, practices and views. A sense of that country’s international contacts came in the list of countries from which the staff involved in the study had gained their PhDs: four from Japan and one each from the United States and Germany. The other seven studies are from South Africa (2), Kenya, Malawi, Hong Kong, Italy and Japan. The classes investigated in the book include primary, secondary and tertiary levels, the last being in the majority. They vary in their methods and, not surprisingly, in their results. It would be interesting to hear how many of the groups mentioned in the book are amongst its readers. Will it be the policy makers whose decisions determine which languages are used in their country’s education systems? Probably not, because such choices are usually made for political rather than educational reasons. Will it be researchers? Probably, because the wideranging topics and methods give them a good starting point. Hopefully, classroom teachers will also get hold of the book so they can try out some of the many interesting tasks described here. Also, if they happen to have students from any of the countries mentioned, then the contents will help them understand those people’s backgrounds. The book is recommended to all these groups. References Marr, T., & English, F. (2019). Rethinking TESOL in diverse global settings: The language and the teacher in a time of change. London: Bloomsbury. Sifakis, N. C., & Tsantila, N. (Eds.). (2019). English as a lingua franca for EFL contexts. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. 85 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 86 Tamin, T. (2021). Monolingual Policies and Plurilingual Practices in English Language Classrooms: Addressing Shared Guilt and Threats. Asia EFL Journal, 25(2), 2-28. Bio data Marilyn Lewis is an Honorary Research Fellow at Auckland University and may be e-mailed at mn.lewis@auckland.ac.nz 86 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 87 Critical Thinking in ELT: A Working Model for the Classroom Paul Dummett and John Hughes. National Geographic Learning: Boston, USA, 2019. Pp. viii +158. Reviewed by Thiri Soe, Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University, Japan Incorporating critical thinking in all different subject areas is highly recommended as an educational goal. In the field of English Language Teaching, Dummett and Hughes’s (2019) Critical Thinking in ELT: A Working Model for the Classroom is a comprehensive introductory book with guidelines and models for integrating critical thinking activities in language skills. The book delivers ELT teachers step by step explanations from the operational definitions and concepts of critical thinking to the application of critical thinking activities in ELT materials in its seven chapters. Chapter 1 explains readers why critical thinking has to be a central role in education. The increasing access of internet and information from digital media is the main reason that requires 21st century learners to evaluate and resonate the information before them. From the subjectspecific perspective, there is a strong bond between critical thinking and language acquisition. Deeper processing and production of language is achieved by means of critical thinking. It is also stressed in this chapter that two types of critical thinking skills, higher order thinking skill and lower order thinking skill are neither inferior nor superior to each other. Having the balance of both thinking skills is crucial in effective language learning. Chapter 2 states how critical thinking benefits learners in such factors as academia, business, early education, and everyday life and internet. Learners can get more engagement and learning autonomy by exercising thinking activities from ELT textbooks. Critical thinking activities can be added into various stages of lessons from the smallest unit of discourse like words and syllables to larger discourse texts like paragraphs and articles. Not only that, critical thinking activities can be applied to receptive skills as well as productive skills. Dummett and Hughes’ innovative lesson planning ideas for inclusion of critical thinking into different language levels and skills can be learnt in Chapter 3, 4, and 5. Their examples of critical thinking activities are built on the language tasks and skills that ELT textbooks cover: grammar, vocabulary, pronunciation, reading, listening, speaking, and writing. Inductive reasoning, comparative analysis, rephrasing and reformulation, translation, rearranging word order, identifying word order, and identifying idiomatic usage are example 87 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 88 activities applicable in grammar teaching. For teaching vocabulary, comparing words, considering the word scope, understanding grammatical function of words, considering emotional, social, and cultural aspects of words, and understanding literal and figurative uses of words are critical thinking encouraging activities. Analyzing pronunciation patterns to see how they convey meaning, analyzing one’s own pronunciation to remove areas of ambiguity, and conscious awareness of the pronunciation patterns of one’s interlocutor are example activities for teaching pronunciation critically. Another crucial idea the authors state is that critical thinking activities can be infused with both receptive and productive skills. Identifying authors’ aims, examining the reliability of arguments, separating facts from opinions, and looking for bias from texts are critical thinking encouraging activities in teaching receptive skills. Three factors constitute critical language productive skills: generating ideas, setting criteria for language output, and evaluating the output. In setting up the criteria, learners need to pinpoint two sub-goals: identifying the goal of the text, and the perspective of their text readers. Chapter 6 engages ELT teachers to the extensive need of critical thinking skills beyond classrooms, by suggesting activities learners should apply in confronting different types of literacy. Critical thinking is an essential armor for the learners to wear in exposing themselves to twenty-first century literacies such as visual literacy, information literacy, media literacy, and cultural literacy. The reason for an ELT teacher to focus on these literacies is that they appear as language input to the learners. Learners need to perceive the information judiciously to evaluate their factual accuracy. The last chapter, chapter 7, motivates ELT syllabus writers and teachers for the balanced implication of lower-order and higher-order thinking skills because both of the skills involve in the working model of critical thinking in ELT presented by Dummett and Hughes. This concluding chapter gives readers acute discussion on three topics that are commonly addressed and asked by English language teachers around the world. These topics are evaluating lessons for critical thinking, teaching critical thinking at lower language levels, and promoting critical thinking. Overall, the book Critical Thinking in ELT: A Working Model for the Classroom is a worth reading textbook and it bridges critical thinking and English language education. Having explained concepts and definitions of critical thinking activities followed by example lessons, the book is an evidence-based manual for the incorporation of critical thinking in ELT curriculum. At the end of the last chapter, there are suggested answers to sample exercises and ask yourself question. The book will surely stand as a successful course book in training ELT 88 EFLIJ Volume 25 Issue 4 July 2021 89 teachers for increasing awareness to the application of critical thinking activities in their classrooms. Bio data Myanmar academic, Thiri Soe studies at the Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University, Japan. thirisoe6@gmail.com 89 View publication stats