COMMUNICATION IN LONGDISTANCE RELATIONSHIPS Patterns and relationships DECEMBER 2023 BARUCH COLLEGE Com 4906 by professor. Richard Wilkins 1 Introduction My research topic revolves around the problems that occur in long distance dating relationships and long-distance friendships. The research is meant to find more about what aspects of a relationship are most affected by long distance and the conflicts that may arise as a result. In addition to different coping techniques and problems of idealization and uncertainty. Concluding with providing some solutions to the most occurring problems. Li, S., Zhang, Y., & Zhao, D. (2005). A Longitudinal Study: Does Synchronous Channel Use Affect the LongDistance Relationship? Conference Papers -- International Communication Association, 1–18 This study focuses mainly on the importance of communication methods used in long distance relationships and their relationship to satisfaction. The study compares the effectiveness of synchronous vs asynchronous communication based on two variables, overall relationship satisfaction and temporary communication satisfaction. The study has found that using preferred synchronous channels was directly related to increased feeling of happiness and decreased feelings of anger, which subsequently lead to higher levels of relationship satisfaction. Moreover, researchers found that the results differ among the synchronous channels as there are synchronous channels preferred and synchronous channels used, but not necessarily preferred by both parties. In general, the study didn’t find a significant difference between synchronous vs asynchronous channels used. However, there was noticeable positive relationship between relationship satisfaction and synchronous channels preferred. Kuske, M. R. (2020). Social Media Use in the Maintenance of Long-Distance Romantic Relationships in College. UWL Journal of Undergraduate Research XXIII (2020). https://www.uwlax.edu/globalassets/offices-services/urc/juronline/pdf/2020/kuske.morgan.cst2020.pdf The study focuses on the emerging dominance of social media usage in long distance relationships communication. The study subjects were mainly college students who are the most prone to experiencing long distance relationships because of relocating for college, in addition to their high social media activity. The study compares the quality and type of communication between LDRC (Long-Distance Romantic Couple) and GCC (Geographically Close Couple) explaining that LDRC relies heavily on verbal communication to compensate for physical distance. The study also addressed other problems faced by LDRC such as idealization, jealousy, and uncertainty. The study compared the differences between selecting some specific social media applications and their type of content, ease of usability, and lack of technical issues. It also mentioned how sharing content on said apps in starting conversations and avoiding stagnation. Hence, in this category, apps with news feed were preferred. Nadin Kenawy 2 Studying relationships is a complex area in communication research. I'm particularly interested in long-distance relationships because they heavily rely on communication to keep the connection strong. Understanding how couples manage communication when they can't meet in person is important. My research focuses on figuring out how couples in long-distance relationships communicate. I want to understand not just the usual communication patterns but also the problems that might come up due to the distance. Additionally, I'm looking into different ways these couples resolve conflicts to see how well these methods work for them. A crucial part of my study is evaluating how satisfied people in long-distance relationships are with their communication. I'm doing a detailed analysis of the happiness levels reported by individuals in these relationships. Through this research, I hope to provide valuable insights into how long-distance relationships function and how couples in these situations are doing. In the next parts, I'll explain some ideas (hypotheses) I've come up with based on my research questions. I'll use data from surveys and analysis to draw conclusions, aiming to contribute to a better understanding of what goes on in long-distance relationships. Hypothesis: H0: The frequency of in-person meetings does not affect the extent of idealization between couples. Explanation: This hypothesis is grounded in the idea that the way people communicate through various media, such as phone calls, video calls, or chat, allows for a high degree of selectivity in what individuals choose to reveal about themselves. In these virtual interactions, individuals can Nadin Kenawy 3 carefully curate which aspects of their personality they present and which ones they may intentionally conceal or emphasize. Contrastingly, when couples meet in person, it provides an opportunity for a more unfiltered view of each other's true personalities, even if attempts are made to hide certain traits. The hypothesis suggests that face-to-face meetings offer a more authentic insight into a person's character, which might not be fully conveyed or perceived during mediated communication. Moreover, the hypothesis posits that communication through media may inadvertently contribute to idealization, where individuals unconsciously overlook or downplay the shortcomings of their partner. In essence, the communication medium may create a distorted, idealized image of the partner, as individuals might choose not to acknowledge certain flaws, unintentionally placing their partner on a pedestal. In summary, the hypothesis posits that the frequency of in-person meetings does not exert a significant influence on the degree of idealization individuals have about their partners. Contrary to the notion that in-person interactions might reduce idealization, the hypothesis maintains that neither the frequency of face-to-face meetings nor mediated communication significantly affects the extent of idealization. Instead, it suggests that idealization may persist irrespective of the mode of communication—whether in person or through mediated channels—due to the selective nature of information sharing and perception. The questions used to measure the relationship between those two variables are as follows: The study used several questions to explore the connection between two variables: 1. Q4: How often do you meet your long-distance partner face to face? Nadin Kenawy 4 2. Q16: How do you perceive your partner's actions or decisions, even when they make a mistake? 3. Q6: How would you describe the impact of your partner's presence on your overall wellbeing and happiness? 4. Q18: How often do you discuss your partner's positive qualities with friends or family? The frequency of face-to-face meetings (Q4) was used to gauge how often couples physically met. Questions 16 and 18 (Q16 & Q18) were employed to assess the degree of idealization by understanding how participants perceive their partner's actions, especially in moments of mistake, and how frequently they discuss their partner's positive qualities with others. The chi-square test results presented the observed number of individuals displaying tendencies towards idealizing their partners, categorized by the frequency of face-to-face meetings. The data was divided into two groups: those who meet frequently and those who don't. The test aims to determine if there is a significant relationship between the frequency of in-person meetings and the tendency to idealize a partner. Observed Meet frequently Idealistic Non-Idealistic couple Totals Meet Infrequently Totals 5 32 37 14 11 25 19 43 62 Expected Idealistic Non-Idealistic couple Totals Chi square test: Nadin Kenawy Meet frequently Meet Infrequently Totals 11.33870968 25.66129032 37 7.661290323 17.33870968 25 19 43 62 0.000371359 5 The findings indicate that, given the chi-square value falling below 0.05, we have sufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis. This implies that there is, indeed, a statistically significant relationship between the frequency of in-person meetings among long-distance couples and the inclination toward idealization. Visualized below is a graph illustrating both the observed and expected numbers of couples as measured in this hypothesis. Idealizing Vs. Non-idealizing couple (Observed) 35 32 30 25 20 14 15 11 10 5 5 0 1 2 Idealizing couple Non-idealizing couple Idealizing Vs. Non-idealizing couple (Expected) Idealizing couple 30 Non-Idealizing couple 25,66129032 25 20 15 17,33870968 11,33870968 7,661290323 10 5 0 1 Nadin Kenawy 2 6 To further prove our findings, we have conducted a t-test or in this case a Z-test to test the difference in the means between our two variables and the p-value result. The difference in means between the two variables present the following results: The mean of the first variable, which is the frequency of in-person meeting, is 2.4. Meaning that most respondents are more likely to meet their partners infrequently. The mean of the second variable which is how idealistic participants view their partners is 1.8, meaning that 1.8, which means that on average, respondents describe the impact of their partner's presence on their overall well-being and happiness as very positive, leaning towards the most positive end of the scale. In addition, the p-value of 0.007, which is way below our significance level of 0.05, further proves our earlier findings, proving that the two variables have a significant relationship with one another, hence, they are dependent. As for the correlation result of the t-test, the Pearson correlation coefficient is -0.46, indicating a moderate negative correlation between the two variables. As the frequency of inperson meetings decreases, the idealistic view of the partner's impact on well-being and happiness tends to increase (and vice versa). Mean Variance Observations Pearson Correlation Hypothesized Mean Difference df t Stat P(T<=t) one-tail t Critical one-tail Nadin Kenawy 3 2.392156863 0.923137255 51 -0.463393631 0 50 2.540845918 0.007104294 1.675904967 1 1.862745098 0.600784314 51 7 P(T<=t) two-tail t Critical two-tail 0.014208588 2.008559072 The second hypothesis examined in this research is as follows: H0: There is no association between the frequency of in-person meetings and relationship satisfaction among long-distance couples. My rationale for this hypothesis stems from the belief that face-to-face communication holds a unique value compared to mediated forms. In-person interactions reveal more authentic human reactions, which are often less visible in chats or calls. People tend to express their emotions and natural responses more openly in real-life encounters than behind screens. Consequently, my hypothesis suggests that the infrequency of in-person communication may have a negative impact on relationship satisfaction. The variables for testing the second hypothesis were assessed using the following questions: 1. Q4: How often do you meet your long-distance partner face to face? 2. Q21: When thinking about your relationship, which word best describes your feelings? As previously mentioned, Q4 gauged the frequency of in-person meetings between couples. Regarding Q21, participants were prompted to select one word that best encapsulated their current long-distance relationship, with choices ranging from joyful to unfulfilled. The combined responses from both questions were analyzed to investigate whether partners who meet less frequently are more inclined to describe their relationship negatively, thereby determining if there exists a connection between face-to-face meetings and relational satisfaction. Nadin Kenawy 8 Below are the results of the chi-square test performed to measure the difference between the observed and expected outcomes of measuring the relationship between the values. Observed 6 months or less 6 months or more Total satisfied Observed 6 months or less 6 months or more Total satisfied Chi-square test: 0.567292636 dissatisfied 12 14 26 14 22 36 26 36 62 15.10 20.90 36 26 36 62 dissatisfied 10.90 15.10 26 The chi-square statistic yielded a value of approximately 0.57, surpassing the significance level of 0.05. This indicates that the observed data does not provide sufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis, which posits no relationship between the variables. In simpler terms, the results suggest that there is no statistically significant association between the frequency of in-person meetings and relational satisfaction among long-distance couples. In practical terms, this implies that couples who meet less frequently may not necessarily experience negative feelings about the overall success of their relationship. The lack of statistical significance suggests that variations in the frequency of face-to-face interactions do not reliably predict changes in relational satisfaction, as observed in the study. Consequently, these findings challenge the assumption that the frequency of in-person meetings directly correlates with how couples perceive the success of their long-distance relationships. Nadin Kenawy 9 Below is a paragraph that helps with visualizing the results of our findings, Series 1: couples who meet every 6 months or less. Series 2: couples who meet every 6 months or more. In-person meeting & relational satisfaction(observed) 25 22 20 15 14 12 14 10 5 0 1 2 Ряд1 Ряд2 In-person meeting & relational satisfaction(expected) 25,00 20,90 20,00 15,10 15,00 15,10 10,90 10,00 5,00 0,00 1 2 Ряд1 Our third hypothesis is: Nadin Kenawy Ряд2 10 H0: There is no difference in the distribution of compromising and avoidance behaviors between genders when facing disagreements. The third hypothesis explores the potential influence of gender on the manifestation of compromising and avoidance behaviors during disagreements within romantic relationships. The assumption is rooted in the speculation that gender-based differences may shape individuals' approaches to conflict resolution. Specifically, the hypothesis posits that there is an absence of significant variation in the prevalence of compromising and avoidance behaviors between different genders. Compromising behavior involves finding a middle ground or mutually agreeable solution during disagreements. This could manifest as a willingness to negotiate, consider the partner's perspective, and reach a consensus that accommodates both parties. The hypothesis questions whether one gender, on average, engages in compromising behavior to a similar extent as the other. Avoidance behavior, on the other hand, encompasses strategies where individuals may sidestep or evade conflict rather than directly addressing it. This could involve ignoring the issue, withdrawing from discussions, or postponing conflict resolution. The hypothesis questions whether there is a gender-related distinction in the propensity to employ avoidance behaviors during disagreements. To test the hypothesis, we used the following questions: 1. Q30: What is your gender? 2. Q13: How do you typically manage conflicts related to differences in future plans or goals? These two questions were used to entail whether specific genders tend to have certain responses towards relationship conflicts. Nadin Kenawy 11 The chi-square test results that would show us whether there’s a significant relationship between the variables being tested or not, and they’re at follows: Observed Male Female Totals Observed Male Female Totals Avoidance behavior Compromising behavior 7 0 7 8 25 33 Avoidance behavior 2.625 4.375 7 Compromising behavior 12.375 20.625 33 Chi square test: Totals 15 25 40 Totals 15 25 40 0.000169569 Based on this result, we must reject the null hypothesis. Meaning, there’s a statistically significant relationship between the two variables. The chi-square test yielded a highly significant result (p = 0.000169), indicating a substantial relationship between gender and the choice of avoidant or compromising behaviors in the context of romantic long-distance relationships. Female Participants: Women demonstrated a clear preference for compromising behaviors, suggesting a tendency to seek middle ground and find diplomatic solutions during disagreements. Notably, none of the female participants exhibited a preference for avoidance behaviors, indicating a reluctance to avoid conflicts, possibly rooted in their perceived diplomatic nature. Male Participants: Nadin Kenawy 12 In contrast, male participants displayed a higher inclination toward avoiding conflicts and showed less tendency to adopt compromising behaviors compared to their female counterparts. This inclination towards avoidance in men may be attributed to their affirmative nature, while the lesser inclination towards compromising behaviors aligns with traditional gender expectations. These findings underscore the presence of distinct patterns in conflict resolution behaviors based on gender in romantic long-distance relationships. While women lean towards compromise and eschew avoidance, men tend to exhibit avoidance tendencies and display less inclination towards finding middle ground during disagreements. Below is a graph visually representing the results: Genter and compromise/avoidance (observed) 30 25 25 20 15 10 8 7 5 0 0 1 2 Ряд1 Male s Female Ряд2 Genter and compromise/avoidance(expected) 25 20,625 20 12,375 15 10 5 4,375 2,625 0 1 2 Ряд1 Male s Nadin Kenawy Ряд2 Female 13 The fourth hypothesis we’ll be presenting tests whether there’s a relationship between the quality of communication and the frequency of disagreements in long-distance relationships. H0: there's no relationship between couples’ communication quality and the frequency of their disagreements. This hypothesis is built on the conjecture that the quality of communication plays a pivotal role in influencing the frequency of disagreements among couples in long-distance relationships. Given the heightened reliance on communication in the absence of physical proximity, it is speculated that the effectiveness of communication directly impacts the overall dynamics of the relationship. The questions that were used to measure this relationship is as follows: 1. Q7: How often do you face disagreements in your long-distance relationship? 2. Q8: How would you describe the way you and your partner communicate? Below are the chi-square results: Observed quality communication Problematic communication Totals Rarely disagree Expected quality communication Problematic communication Totals Rarely disagree frequently disagree Totals 22.65 8.35 Chi-square test: frequently disagree Totals 29 2 31 9 38 21 52 15.35 38 12 14 5.65 14 31 21 52 0.000052634362 The results of the chi-square test conducted on the two variables lead to a non-rejection of the null hypothesis. With a chi-square p-value below the significance level, it indicates a statistically significant Nadin Kenawy 14 relationship between the quality of communication in long-distance relationships and the frequency of disagreements. The responses to Question 7, ranging from 1 (Never) to 4 (All the time), gauged the frequency of disagreements. Question 8, ranging from 1 (We communicate openly and frequently) to 4 (We rarely communicate, and it's often problematic), captured the perceived quality of communication. The statistical analysis suggests that the perceived quality of communication is linked to the frequency of disagreements in long-distance relationships. The answers to question 7 ranged between 1. Never to 4. All the time. As for question 8 the answers ranged between 1. We communicate openly and frequently and 4. We rarely communicate, and it's often problematic. Below is a t-test performed to test the relationship between the two variables: q7 Mean Variance Observations Pearson Correlation Hypothesized Mean Difference df t Stat P(T<=t) one-tail t Critical one-tail P(T<=t) two-tail t Critical two-tail q8 2 2.37254902 0.318431373 51 0.459568211 1 1.784313725 0.73254902 51 0 50 5.39163866 9.48757E-07 1.675904967 0.0000018975 2.008559072 The t-test results suggest a statistically significant relationship between the perceived quality of communication and the frequency of disagreements in long-distance relationships. The mean difference of 0.59 indicates that, on average, there is a substantial difference in how individuals perceive the quality Nadin Kenawy 15 of communication when considering the frequency of disagreements. The extremely low p-value strengthens the evidence, supporting the conclusion that the perceived quality of communication is associated with the frequency of disagreements in the context of long-distance relationships. Nadin Kenawy 16 References: Li S, Zhang Y, Zhao D. A Longitudinal Study: Does Synchronous Channel Use Affect the LongDistance Relationship? Conference Papers -- International Communication Association. May 2005:1-18. Accessed December 13, 2023. https://search-ebscohost- com.remote.baruch.cuny.edu/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ufh&AN=18654896&site=ehost-live Kuske, M. R. (2020). Social Media Use in the Maintenance of Long-Distance Romantic Relationships in College. UWL Journal of Undergraduate Research XXIII (2020). https://www.uwlax.edu/globalassets/offices-services/urc/juronline/pdf/2020/kuske.morgan.cst2020.pdf Cameron, J. J., & Ross, M. (2007). In Times of Uncertainty: Predicting the Survival of Long-Distance Relationships. Journal of Social Psychology, 147(6), 581–606. https://doi- org.remote.baruch.cuny.edu/10.3200/SOCP.147.6.581-606 Pattie, B. (2009). The Impact of Conflict Management Styles on Relational and Communication Satisfaction Comparing Long-Distance and Geographically Close Dating Relationships. Conference Papers -- National Communication Association, 1. Nadin Kenawy