INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION The Effects of Induced Guilt on Motivation Level to Increase Academic Expenditure by Thuy Mai Bui Dr. Seiji Takaku Senior Capstone Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of B.A. in Liberal Arts Social & Behavioral Sciences Psychology 2022 1 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 2 Abstract The current study applied Festinger’s (1957) Cognitive Dissonance Theory to explain the relationship between induced guilt and self-directed motivation in an academic context. While existing literature has been widely discussing about the effects of induced guilt on the motivation to conduct prosocial behavior, the connection between guilt and academic motivation is yet to be examined. Therefore, this study hypothesized that, as students experience the cognitive discrepancy between their actual and expected academic effort expenditure based on family expectations, the induced guilt will elevate their motivational level to increase academic effort expenditure. The results partially supported the hypothesis, indicating that the guilt-induced dissonance had marginally significant effects on students’ motivation level to increase academic effort expenditure. Possible limitations and directions for future research were discussed. Keywords: induced guilt, cognitive dissonance, self-directed motivation INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 3 The Effects of Induced Guilt on Motivation Level to Increase Academic Expenditure Motivation plays a pivotal role in students’ academic success, and researchers have discussed different methods to increase academic motivation: student-teacher interaction (Trolian et al., 2016), peer influence (Molloy et al.,2010) or improving curriculum programming (Saeedi & Parvizy, 2019). Besides the fore-mentioned methods, inducing guilt may be another method to increase academic motivation that previous studies have yet to propose. In fact, current literature only mentioned the linkage between guilt and prosocial behaviors (see e.g., Lindsey, 2006; Erlandsson et al., 2016; Cunningham et al., 1980, Lazarus, 1991; O’Keefe, 2000). In other words, literature seems to suggest that guilt provokes other-directed motivation, which is acting for the sake of others rather than for oneself. However, academic motivation is mainly self-directed, as it serves to improve oneself and prepare them to succeed in their own life. Although current studies do not provide sufficient evidence, induced guilt, on occasions, seems to motivate students to increase their academic expenditure, hence acting for their own sake. Under what conditions, then, does guilt play the role of influencing self-directed academic motivation? This present study will investigate this research question, thereby generating a possible explanation for the causal relationship between induced guilt and self-directed academic motivation. The Conceptualization of Guilt Regarding the definition of guilt, Ortony (1987) proposed two different meanings of guilt: a socio-legal one and an emotional one. The definitions of legal guilt are based on the notion of violating the legislated rules, and are independent of subjective feelings or sense of responsibility for past actions (Baumeister et al., 1994). In other words, people could be guilty without having any feelings of guilt (Ortony, 1987). However, the guilt that this study focuses on, is emotional guilt. In this study, we refer to guilt as an aroused, unpleasant emotional state INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 4 that stems from one’s distress about the possibility of being in the wrong (e.g., objections to action, inaction, or intentions) or being perceived to be wrong by others (Baumeister et al., 1994). It is natural that guilt feels unpleasant, painful, hence triggering self-aggression and selfpunishing attitude (Wallington, 1973) as well as tension and remorse. However, due to this particular nature, guilt plays an important part in regulating humans’ moral behaviors, encouraging prosocial behaviors, and correcting past mistakes (Lazarus, 1991; O’Keefe, 2000). Guilty feelings have been linked with the thoughts of apologizing (Baumeister et al., 1994), wishing one had acted differently, making amends (Lazarus & Smith, 1990; Tagney et al., 1996; Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Freedman et al. 1967), and the efforts to undo or resurrect past actions and to prevent oneself from conducting harmful behaviors in the future (Lazarus & Smith, 1990; Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz, 1994). Feelings of guilt can also arise when one imagines or recalls committing a wrongdoing, or when one is aware that another person, might or might not be physically present, may suffer adversely from the consequences of one’s action or inaction (Baumeister et al., 1994). In other words, emotional guilt is based on one’s capacity to know or to anticipate others’ suffering and the self-attribution that oneself if the reason for that suffering (Lindsey, 2005; Baumeister et al., 1994; Hoffman, 1982). Kugler and Jones (1992) proposed that theoretically, guilt can result in both adaptive and maladaptive consequences. The increased feelings of guilt have been proven to increase compliance and motivate prosocial behaviors (Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Cunningham et al., 1980; Freedman et al. 1967; Lindsey, 2006; Erlandsson et al., 2016; O’Keefe, 2000). One’s sense of responsibility over their actions or inactions, as well as their sense of controllability over the situation have been found to be the factors that cause one to self-blame, hence experiencing guilt and wanting to engage in guilt-reducing activities (Weiner et al. 1982; Lazarus & Smith, 1990; INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 5 Kugler & Jones, 1992). On the other hand, excessive or inappropriate feelings of guilt are assumed to cause “dysfunctional and disruptive experiences” (Kugler & Jones, 1992), occasionally leading to clinical disorders (Kugler & Jones, 1992; Breslavs, 2013). The Multidimensional Model of Guilt Kubany and Watson (2003) have reviewed the multiple determinants of trauma-related guilt proposed by existing literature. These determinants include (1) the distress about a negative outcome (2) the sense of responsibility for causing harm (Kubany et al., 1995) (3) justification or lack of justification (Kubany et aI., 1995) (4) beliefs about outcome foreseeability and preventability (Kubany, 1994) and (5) beliefs about violating central personal values. In other words, the level of guilt will increase when (1) the individual’s situation-related distress is high (2) the individual believes that he or she is responsible for the fore-mentioned negative situation (3) the individual believes that his or her actions were unjustifiable (4) the individual believes that the situation was preventable if he or she had or had not taken action and (5) the individual believes he or she has violated central personal values (Kubany & Watson, 2003). This present study focuses on commonplace or “everyday” guilt, as Kubany and Watson (2003) named it, which shares the same model as trauma-related guilt on a smaller scale, with significantly lower level of distress (Kubany et al., 2002). Other researchers (Baumeister et al., 1994; Hoffman, 1982; Weiner et al., 1982) also proposed the basic predictors of guilt, including (1) the individual’s capacity to anticipate or feel others’ suffering or distress (Baumeister et al., 1994) (2) the level of self-attribution for such distress due to the sense of responsibility (Hoffman, 1982; Weiner, 1982) and (3) the individual’s belief in the controllability and preventability of the situation. Theoretically, the more severe the distress is, the more guilt the individual feels; the more one feels responsible for the suffering, the higher level of guilt is INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 6 induced; the more one believes that they could have controlled the situation and prevented the suffering from happening, the more guilt they experience. Guilt & Self-Directed Motivation As the study mentioned, previous research has suggested that guilt increases motivation to engage in prosocial, other-directed behaviors (Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Cunningham et al., 1980; Erlandsson et al., 2016; Freedman et al. 1967; Lindsey, 2006; O’Keefe, 2000). Lindsey (2005) conducted research on the effects of induced anticipated guilt on individuals’ willingness to help unknown others through bone marrow donation. In the experiment, conditioned participants received messages with induced perceptions of (1) a threat to the unknown others (2) a prescribed solution to the threat and (3) self-efficacy in an individual's ability to help. The results aligned with the hypothesis, which stated that these factors would increase the level of anticipated guilt the participants experienced, thereby increasing their willingness to conduct prosocial behavior - donating to help strangers. However, in an academic context, studying hard and obtaining high academic achievements are mainly self-directed behaviors, as they serve the benefits of the student themselves, not of other people. Yet, at times, induced guilt still seems to increase such selfdirected motivation. What might be the similarity between prosocial behaviors (e.g., helping strangers), and self-directed behavior (e.g., studying and getting good grades)? To provide an explanation, Batson (1987) proposed a type of prosocial behavior that serves to reduce aversive arousal, or in other words, the feelings of guilt. In the previous research (Lindsey, 2006), the participants’ willingness to help unknown people depended on their level of anticipated guilt if they had not helped. In this case, their prosocial action was partially self-directed, as they were trying to avoid the unpleasant guilt that might disturb them. In the case of students, their self- INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 7 directed motivation probably increases along with the awareness that they are expected to conduct self-directed behaviors (i.e., studying harder). Thus, the present study proposed a sequence of emotions as follows: Family expectations of students’ academic achievements ↓ Students are reminded of those expectations and how they failed to meet them ↓ Students are aware of their responsibility for causing distress to their family ↓ Tension between being aware of their current studying behaviors and of their family expectations arises ↓ A need to reduce this tension arises because they want to make amends ↓ Self-directed motivation to study hard to meet family expectations to reduce this tension However, there are many potential factors that can interfere with this hypothesized relationship. For example, if a student does not feel the need to fill up the gap between their family’s expectations and their current studying behavior, the sequence from guilt to selfdirected academic motivation is interrupted. Similarly, this sequence is probably irrelevant to those who do not think their family is feeling disappointed or sad about them failing, or who do not think they are responsible for causing such distress. Therefore, for this sequence to occur, a few requirements need to be satisfied: (1) students are aware of their family’s disappointment (2) students must feel responsible for it and (3) students must perceive the gap between their present INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 8 academic effort level and the effort level they are supposed to reach to meet their family’s expectations. In short, guilt can only increase self-directed motivation to pursue academic excellence when students recognize a need to fill the discrepancy between their current and expected levels of academic effort expenditure induced by the feelings of guilt towards their family. Such discrepancy is conceptualized by the term Cognitive Dissonance. The present study will elaborate on the hypothesized role of guilt in encouraging self-directed academic motivation using Festinger’s (1957) Cognitive Dissonance Theory. Cognitive Dissonance, Guilt and Motivation Dr. Leon Festinger (1957) presented that pairs of cognitions can be consonant or dissonant to one another. Cognitions that follow each other are consonant, whereas cognitions that contradict each other are dissonant. Further studies on dissonance and consonance led him to develop and publish A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (1957). Cognitive Dissonance Theory proposed that cognitive dissonance is an uncomfortable tension caused by the inconsistency between an individual's beliefs and behaviors. To test this theory, Festinger and Carlsmith (1959) conducted an experiment and investigated the level of dissonance created when individuals are forced to act against their personal beliefs. They divided 71 participants into three groups, asked them to do a series of dull and repetitive tasks before offering to pay them either $1 or $20 to tell the lady who was waiting to perform the same tasks that they had just experienced were interesting and enjoyable. A control group was not offered any money to, and never did tell the lie to the waiting lady. After the experiment, participants were asked to rate their experience with the tasks they had performed earlier, the participants who were in the $20 condition and control condition INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 9 responded with honesty, saying that the tasks were dull and unenjoyable and that they would not participate in a similar experiment. On the other hand, those who were in the $1 condition changed their opinion, saying that the tasks were interesting and that they were willing to participate in another similar experiment. In short, those in the $1 condition thought that the $1 was not enough to justify their action of lying to the lady, so they had to change their opinion to reduce the dissonance between their action (i.e. lying that the task were enjoyable) and their belief (i.e. the tasks were boring). In the $20 condition, the participants received sufficient reward to justify their behavior, so they did not have to change their honest opinion to bring into correspondence with what they said to the lady. Following the experiment, researchers have also studied on different ways of reducing cognitive dissonance: (1) changing the behavior (2) changing existing beliefs (3) reducing the importance of the dissonance and (4) justifying the dissonance (Aaronson, 1976; Festinger, 1957). Present Study Because guilt is a form of cognitive dissonance (see e.g., Breslavs, 2013; Stice, 1992), the ways by which people reduce guilt would be similar to how people reduce dissonance. In this study, we are going to examine the effects of this dissonance induced guilt on increasing academic achievement motivation. Specifically, if students are reminded of their family’s expectations of their academic effort expenditure and feel the obligation to study hard to satisfy those expectations, and if their current studying behaviors are not as dedicated as they are supposed to, students must feel guilty about this inconsistency. Thus, they will feel the need to reduce the gap by changing their current studying behavior, which means their motivational level INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 10 to increase academic effort expenditure also grows. Thus, we have come up with the following hypothesis: Hypothesis. When students are reminded of the inconsistency between their family's expectations of their academic achievements and their actual achievements, they will experience dissonance induced guilt and to reduce this feeling of guilt, they are more likely to increase their academic achievement motivation than when they are not reminded of the inconsistency. Method Participants One hundred and twenty participants of all racial and gender backgrounds from the student body of Soka University of America, Aliso Viejo (SUA-AV) took part in the experiment. The participants were limited to those who are 18 years old or older. All participants who complete the study are eligible to enroll in a raffle to win a $25 Amazon gift card. Design The present study employed a randomized three-groups experimental design. The independent variable was dissonance-induced guilt. The participants were randomly assigned to either the induced guilt condition, the induced-pride condition, or the control condition in which they were not induced any emotion. Guilt-induced dissonance manipulation was conducted by either reminding the participants of the inconsistency between their current studying behavior and their family’s expectations (either exceeded or didn’t exceed) or not remined of any inconsistency. A manipulation check was included. The study also included one dependent variable: self-directed motivation, measured by self-assessed tentative academic effort expenditure. INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 11 Procedure/ Materials The present study took place at Soka University of America, Aliso Viejo, during the spring semester of 2022. The online survey was distributed to the whole SUA campus through a campus email, which briefly explained the research topic, participant age requirement and withdrawal rights, estimated survey time, incentives, and guaranteed confidentiality. The study consisted of a four-section questionnaire, conducted by the principal investigator and a research assistant. The first section examined participants’ current attitude towards academics, the second section involved a set of questions to manipulate guilt-induced dissonance, the third section included a question to check the efficiency of the manipulation, and the last section involved a set of questions to measure participants’ motivation level to increase academic effort. In the first section, all participants were required to rate their current attitude towards academic work (i.e., to what extent they value academic work, and how much they think academics benefits them in the future). The second section, all participants were randomly assigned into two experimental and one control group. The first group’s participants were asked whether or not they had ever failed to meet their family’s academic expectations. If the participant answered yes, they were then required to write about the experience, and rate how guilty they felt recalling that experience. The second group’s participants were asked whether they had ever exceeded their family’s academic expectations, and if the answer was yes, they were then asked to clarify that experience, and rate how guilty they felt recalling the experience. The control group did not receive any manipulation, they were only asked to rate their overall academic experience and how willing they were to study harder. INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 12 The third section was reserved for two experimental groups as a manipulation check. Participants from these two groups were required to rate their level of guilt regarding the experience. Participants were expected to experience a stronger sense of guilt in the guiltinduced condition as the condition satisfied the determinants to maximize the level of guilt: (1) participant’s family are let down by their lack of achievements (2) the participant was directly responsible for causing such distress (3) the participant are aware of the gap between their current and expected academic effort expenditure (see Appendix A, B, and C for the survey). As a result of the manipulation, these participants were also expected to give higher ratings in the final section, where the questionnaire measured their motivation level to increase academic effort expenditure. As participants experience cognitive dissonance after realizing the gap between their current studying effort and the studying effort expected by their family, they were then expected to feel the need to reduce the dissonance by changing their behavior (i.e., increase their academic effort). In contrast, the participants from the second experimental group were expected to give a lower rating of guilt level, as they were reminded of a positive experience: exceeding family’s academic expectations. As a result, these participants were expected to give lower ratings regarding academic motivation level compared to the first group, as they neither experiences cognitive dissonance nor the need to reduce the dissonance by changing their behavior. By comparing the academic effort expenditure between the two groups, the effect of guilt-induced dissonance on self-directed motivation (i.e., pursue academic excellence) was examined. The informed consent form at the start of the survey did not reveal the true objective of the study to avoid any potential biases that could disrupt the study result. Upon completing the survey, participants received a debriefing statement that thoroughly explained the research INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 13 question and any forms of manipulations involved in the research process. Lastly, participants were enrolled in a raffle for an Amazon gift card on a voluntary basis. The raffle page was separated from the data analysis to protect participants’ anonymity. Only completed responses were used for analysis. Results Creating Motivation Indices for Post-Guilt Manipulation To create the first motivation index, all ten items of the post-manipulation measure were analyzed for internal consistency. A principle components factor analysis with varimax rotation was conducted, and it revealed that three items (i.e., items 2, 7, and 9) were loaded on one dominant factors (26.8% of variance) (see Table 1). The Chronbach’s alpha for the selected three items was .74, showing strong reliability. To distinguish this motivation index from the others, the three-item index was named the Attitude Towards Education Index. In addition, three items (i.e., items 3,4, and 5) were also loaded on one dominant factor (16.7% of variance) (see Table 2). The Chronbach’s alpha for the selected three items was approximately .6, showing sufficient reliability. To distinguish this motivation index from the first index, the three-item index was named the Engagement Index. Item 1 was used for analysis as a separate variable. The remaining three items (i.e., items 6,8 and 10) were excluded from further analysis. Guilt-Induced Dissonance Manipulation Check To test the validity of the guilt-induced dissonance manipulation, an independent samples t-test was performed on the Guilt item. As the manipulation check question was included only in the two experimental groups, only those two experimental groups were examined in this analysis. The analysis showed that the participants experienced significantly higher level of guilt INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 14 in the “induced-guilt” condition (M = 2.90, SD = 1.73), t(48) = 6.44, p <.001. Thus, the guiltinduced dissonance manipulation was effective (see Table 3). Testing the Hypothesis One Way ANOVA To test the hypothesis, a one-way ANOVA was conducted to examine the effects of dissonance-induced guilt manipulation on motivation to increasing academic achievement motivation among all three groups in terms of the following post-manipulation variables: (1) Number of hours participants intend to spend studying (2) Attitude towards education and (3) Engagement motivation. As for the attitude towards education variable, no statistically significant difference between groups was detected (p = .89). However, the study found a marginally statistically significant difference between groups regarding the engagement level variable (F(2, 117) = 1.78, p = .17). A Tukey post hoc test showed that participants in the induced-pride condition express a slightly higher level of academic engagement motivation than the control group (p = .15). There was no statistically significant difference between the induced-guilt and control groups (p = .85) or between the induced-guilt and induced-pride groups (p = .49). Regarding the number of hours students intend to spend studying variable, there was also a marginally statistically significant difference between groups as demonstrated by one-way ANOVA (F(2,117) = 1.62, p = .202). A Tukey post hoc test showed that on average, participants in the induced-guilt group intended to spend slightly more time studying than those in the control group (p = .19). There was no statistically significant difference between the induced-guilt and induced-pride groups (p = .37) or between the induced-pride and control groups (p = .96) (see Table 4). INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 15 Correlation Analyses The study conducted a Pearson Correlation analysis to further investigate the relationships among several factors regardless of the dissonance manipulation. In terms of motivation level to increase academic effort expenditure, the data indicated that on average, regardless of guilt-induced dissonance manipulation, students who value academic work tend to have significantly better attitude towards education and academic engagement. In the inducedguilt condition, the study found that students who value their academic work highly tend to have better academic attitude, r(30) = .41, p < .05 (see Table 5). Similarly, the data signified that the more participants’ value academic work, the better their attitude towards education, r(35) = .37, p < .05, as well as their engagement motivation, r(35) = .39, p < .05. are in the induced-pride condition (see Table 6). Lastly, in the control condition, students’ attitude towards education was also found to be higher in those who value their education (55) = .40, p < .01 (see Table 7). Discussion The present study applied Festinger’s (1957) Cognitive Dissonance Theory to explore the effects of induced guilt on students’ self-directed motivation to increase academic effort expenditure. The manipulation check was effective, which showed that on average, students felt significantly more guilty in the condition when they recalled not meeting their family’s academic expectations than in the other condition when they recalled exceeding their family’s expectations. The effective induction of guilt may be attributed to the overall academic environment that the participants have been immersed in. All participants come from environment where academic achievements are deemed highly, and academic competence is considered an essential virtue. In such environment, they are exposed to expectations coming from school officials, teachers, family, as well as peer pressure from other students who are INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 16 academically ambitious. Therefore, as participants were reminded of a time when their academic achievements did not fulfill expectations, they generally felt guilty. However, despite the effectiveness of guilt-induced manipulation on students’ feelings of guilt, the results only partially supported the hypothesis. The research revealed that students’ academic motivation level was only marginally affected by the induced-guilt dissonance manipulation. One possible explanation for this result is that the sample size was too small for any significant difference to be observed. Each of these conditions only included 30-35 participants; therefore, the data obtained, although very close, could not reach the .05 threshold of significance level. If a bigger sample size was available, the study could probably detect a statistically significant effect. The second explanation is that the aforementioned induced-guilt manipulation might not be effective enough to manifest feelings of guilt into a fuel to increase participants’ motivation to pursue academic excellence. If one wants to improve themselves academically, they need to enhance their studying habits that they have already gotten used to, which requires a remarkable amount of self-discipline and will. Humans tend to persist in habits (Jager, 2003), and breaking a habit only by a temporal manipulation is challenging. It is possible that the induced guilt manipulation was not powerful enough to make up for the lack of willingness to change their study routine; therefore, instead of changing their behavior to reduce cognitive dissonance (i.e., increase academic motivation), the participants probably justified their attitude or tolerated the cognitive dissonance. Another explanation for this result involves the cultural factor that might have intervened with the expression of guilt in this study. According to Hofstede’s Cultural Dimensions Theory (Hofstede, 1984) regarding the individualism-collectivism dimension, collectivist cultures value group harmony and prioritize common needs rather than individual needs, therefore, members of INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 17 these cultures develop an interdependent perception of self (Hofstede, 2011). Meanwhile, members of individualist cultures have a more independent self-schemata. Therefore, they tend to be autonomous and prioritize their personal goal rather than that of their in-groups. (Triandis, 2002; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1989, 1995; Triandis, McCusker & Hui, 1990) Some research has shown the correlation between collectivism-individualism syndrome and the expression of social emotions (i.e., expression of guilt). Markus and Kitayama (1991) clearly stated that collectivists, with their interdependent selves, tend to experience and express other-focused emotions (i.e., sympathy, shame). Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) study also found the following: “Among those with interdependent selves (often those from hierarchical cultures), positive emotional expressions are most frequently used as public actions in the service of maintaining interpersonal harmony and, thus, are not regarded as particularly diagnostic of the actor's inner feelings or happiness” (p.236) Feelings of guilt are classified as other-focused emotion (Markus and Kitayama, 1991); Considering the research finding, an assumption can be made: people of collectivist cultures are more likely to express and experience guilt than those of individualist cultures. Nevertheless, as Markus and Kitayama (1991) suggested, their public expressions of guilt might serve as a method of promoting pro-sociality rather than an accurate reflection of their true inner feelings. The study recruited participants from the diverse SUA’s student body, with students from both collectivist and individualist cultures. Therefore, it is possible that collectivist participants’ ratings of their level of guilt did not accurately reflect their true feelings, which resulted in the weak correlation between their induced guilt level and their academic motivation. INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 18 The correlation analysis also indicated a positive correlation between participants’ valuation of academic work before the guilt-induced manipulation and their post-manipulation motivation level to achieve higher academically regardless of conditions. Although this result was not expected in the hypothesis, the correlation between these variables is natural. As students understand the value of academic achievements to their lives, whether at present or in the future, they will be more motivated to study harder. The culture of SUA has always emphasized the importance of learning in helping individuals to become global citizens, thereby contributing to society. As students are constantly immersed in these teachings, they generally are motivated to study harder. Besides, as explained above, SUA is a competitive environment, when students are expected to perform well academically, and those who perform poorly tend to experience peer pressure, which enhances their anxiety pertaining to education (Kadir, Atmowasdoyo & Salija 2018) and affects their self-esteem (Moldes et al., 2019). In this case, the value of studying is not only limited to improving students’ knowledge, but is also reducing the influence of peer pressure. Possible Limitations As mentioned earlier, one of the limitations of the present study lies in the sample size. As the survey was released during the busiest time of the academic year, only 120 students out of over 400 SUA students took part in the experiment. These participants were later randomly assigned into three groups, one of which received no manipulation. Thus, the guilt-induced dissonance condition (i.e., “failed to meet family’s expectations” condition) ended up having only 30 participants; the pride-induced dissonance condition (i.e., “exceeded family’s expectations” condition) had only 35 participants; and the remaining 55 participants were assigned to the control group. Such sample size was not large enough to represent the larger INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 19 target population, hence potentially being a threat to both external and internal validity of the experiment. Another limitation is attributed to the distinguishable characteristics of the sample body. The participants were limited to SUA students, who are immersed in SUA’s educational environment. This lack of diversity might have affected the external validity of this research, as this sampling model presents SUA’s unique characteristics (e.g., small class size, liberal arts education, global citizenship and SGI ideology, the dominance of religion, and limited curriculum). With a sampling model with such small sample size and unique characteristics, even the significant findings of the current study cannot be generalized to a larger population. Last but not least, a possible limitation of this study is the lack of within participants measurement of self-directed academic motivation level, or in other words, pre- and postmanipulation measurements. The present study’s only pre-manipulation measurement was that of participants’ valuation of academic work, and the motivation level was only measured postmanipulation. Therefore, the study only compared the academic motivation level between two experimental groups. A perceived academic effort expenditure level (e.g., how much students feel that they are dedicating to academic work, or how well students think they are engaging to academic activities) might have been an important pre-manipulation variable, as it would have helped reflecting the effectiveness of the guilt-induced dissonance manipulation within each group. Directions for Future Research The present study examined the effect of guilt on self-directed motivation, yet it could not find a strong relationship between these variables due to some methodological flaws and situational confounds, as described in the previous section. Apart from fixing existing INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 20 limitations, it is important to further investigate confounding variables that may interact with either part of the hypothesis (e.g., culture, gender). Although the cultural factor was mentioned in the previous part of the discussion section as one of the possible causes of the reported results, the experiment did not include culture as an independent variable for analysis. Previous research suggests a variation of opinions and findings regarding the influence of culture on cognitive dissonance reduction. Heine and Lehman (1997) found that individualists tend to experience cognitive dissonance and engage in dissonance reduction behaviors more often than collectivists due to their different construals of self. Individualists perceive themselves as autonomous entities, thus they are responsible for their own actions and thoughts. As a result, individualists experience higher frequency of cognitive dissonance as they perceive the inconsistency between their attitude and behavior as a direct threat to the self. Meanwhile, collectivists tend to experience less psychological discomfort due to the inconsistency between their own actions and attitudes, as their behaviors are largely influenced by social and situational factors. They do not perceive themselves as the only one responsible for all of their decisions that reflect their values; therefore, their inconsistency between attitudes and behaviors is not a direct threat to the self. However, a good amount of cross-cultural research has also concluded that, in certain theoretical paradigms, despite possessing different motives, collectivists and individualists are equally likely to experience and be motivated to reduce cognitive dissonance (Takaku, Weiner, Ohbuchi, 2001). For example, in a hypocrisy research paradigm, Takaku, Weiner, and Ohbuchi (2001) clarified their finding as follow: “Whereas collectivists are more concerned about maintaining a good relationship with others or maintaining social norms regarding how one ought to behave in a INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 21 particular situation, individualists are more concerned about protecting their self-identity or maintaining justice regarding what is fair” (p. 147) Similarly, Browne et al. (2005) and Heine and Lehman (1997) also found that: “East Asians would experience dissonance and are motivated to reduce the psychological tension by engaging in justification of their choices when their choices threatened their interdependent self. In other words, when one’s peers or close others are implicated in their choices, interdependent East Asians are most likely to feel dissonance and are motivated to engage in dissonance reduction and restoration of the culturally important self-image.” (p. 133) In short, whether the individualist-collectivist syndrome and cognitive dissonance are culturally relative is still in question. Thus, it may be important for future research to further investigate the role of individualism-collectivism syndrome in the relationship between induced guilt dissonance and self-directed motivation. Furthermore, aside from the Cognitive Dissonance approach, there are other approaches to explain the linkage between induced guilt and self-directed academic motivation. For example, Weiner’s attribution theory (Weiner, 1985, 1986, 2006; Hareli & Weiner, 2002) can be applied as another approach to explain the relationship between guilt and self-directed motivation. According to Weiner’s theory, guilty feelings suggest the individual’s awareness of their lack of effort, which is an internal controllable cause for failure (Weiner, 1985). As a result, self-directed motivation is induced (i.e., “I feel guilty as I did not put enough effort, I will put more effort next time to succeed”). Another factor that future research can consider to examine, is the effect of interpersonal closeness, or in other words, emotional bond, on inducing guilt. Baumeister (2007) proposed that INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 22 individuals can only experience guilt to the extent that they can sympathize with the other person’s feelings. Therefore, it is generally harder for individuals to feel guilty towards a stranger than towards their family or someone they are emotionally close to. Increasing self-directed motivation in an academic context has long been the goal of education leaders, and finding out the relationship between guilt and self-directed academic motivation will be beneficial to some extent, as it helps facilitate academic motivation, pushing students to become academically competent. However, as mentioned earlier, guilt is not a positive emotion to experience long term, as it can result in anxiety and psychological issues. Therefore, if future studies can explore other methods of increasing students’ academic motivation, inducing guilt should become the last method to use. INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 23 References Aaronson, E. (1976). Dissonance theory: Progress and problems. In E. P. Hollander & R. C. Hunt (Eds.), Current perspectives in social psychology (4th ed., pp. 316–328). New York: Oxford University Press. Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 20, pp. 65–122). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60412-8 Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: an interpersonal approach. Psychological bulletin, 115(2), 243–267. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243 Breslavs, G.M. (2013). Moral emotions, conscience, and cognitive dissonance. Psychology in Russia, 6, 65-72. Carlsmith, J. M., & Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11(3), 232–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027039 Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford University Press. Freedman, J. L., Wallington, S. A., & Bless, E. (1967). Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7(2, Pt.1), 117–124. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025009 Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (2019). An introduction to cognitive dissonance theory and an overview of current perspectives on the theory. In E. Harmon-Jones (Ed.), Cognitive dissonance: Reexamining a pivotal theory in psychology (pp. 3–24). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000135-001 Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). From Cognitive Dissonance to the Motivational Functions of Emotions. In R. A. Wright, J. Greenberg, & S. S. Brehm (Eds.), Motivational analyses of INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 24 social behavior: Building on Jack Brehm's contributions to psychology (pp. 39–55). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(4), 389–400. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297234005 Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of Prosocial Motivation: Empathy and Guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The Development of Prosocial Behaviour (pp. 281-313). New York: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-234980-5.50016-X Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. H. (1984). Hofstede’s Culture Dimensions: An Independent Validation Using Rokeach’s Value Survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15(4), 417–433. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022002184015004003 Hoshino-Browne, E., Zanna, A. S., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Kitayama, S., & Lackenbauer, S. (2005). On the cultural guises of cognitive dissonance: The case of Easterners and Westerners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 294–310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.294 Jager, W. (2003). Breaking 'bad habits': a dynamical perspective on habit formation and change. Julle-Danière Eglantine, Whitehouse Jamie, Vrij Aldert, Gustafsson Erik and Waller Bridget M. 2020The social function of the feeling and expression of guiltR. Soc. open sci.7200617200617http://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200617 Kadir, H., & Salija, K. (2018). The Influence of Peer Groups on Students’ Anxiety in EFL Learning. INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 25 Kubany, E.S., & Watson, S.B. (2003). GUILT: ELABORATION OF A MULTIDIMENSIONAL MODEL. Psychological Record, 53, 51-90. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. Lindsey, L. L. M. (2005). Anticipated Guilt as Behavioral Motivation An Examination of Appeals to Help Unknown Others Through Bone Marrow Donation. Human Communication Research, 31(4), 453–481. https://doi.org/10.1093/hcr/31.4.453 Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253. Matsumoto, D., & Juang, L. (2004). (3rd ed.). Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Matsumoto, D., & Yoo, S. H. (2006). Toward a New Generation of Cross-Cultural Research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3), 234–250. http://www.jstor.org/stable/40212168 Molloy, L. E., Gest, S. D., & Rulison, K. L. (2011). Peer Influences on Academic Motivation: Exploring Multiple Methods of Assessing Youths’ Most “Influential” Peer Relationships. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 31(1), 13–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431610384487 O’Keefe, D.J. (2000). Guilt and Social Influence. Annals of the International Communication Association, 23, 101 - 67. Ortony, A. (1987). Is guilt an emotion? Cognition and Emotion, 1(3), 283–298. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938708408052 Princeton University. (2013, October 8). Weighed down by guilt: Research shows it's more than a metaphor. ScienceDaily. Retrieved April 19, 2022 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2013/10/131008132858.htm INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 26 Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 206– 221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.206 Saeedi, M., & Parvizy, S. (2019). Strategies to promote academic motivation in nursing students: A qualitative study. Journal of education and health promotion, 8, 86. https://doi.org/10.4103/jehp.jehp_436_18 Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1990). Emotion and adaptation. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 609–637). The Guilford Press. Stice, E. The similarities between cognitive dissonance and guilt: Confession as a relief of dissonance. Current Psychology 11, 69–77 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02686829 Takaku, S., Weiner, B., & Ohbuchi, K.-I. (2001). A cross-cultural examination of the effects of apology and perspective taking on forgiveness. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 20(1-2), 144–166. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X01020001007 Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1256–1269. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1256 Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism & collectivism. Westview Press. Triandis, H. C. (2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality, 69(6), 907–924. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.696169 Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C. H. (1990). Multimethod probes of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5), 1006–1020. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.1006 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 27 Trolian, T.L., Jach, E.A., Hanson, J.M., & Pascarella, E.T. (2016). Influencing Academic Motivation: The Effects of Student–Faculty Interaction. Journal of College Student Development 57(7), 810-826. doi:10.1353/csd.2016.0080. Vangie M. Moldes, Cherry Lyn L. Biton, Divine Jean Gonzaga, Jerald C. Moneva (2019); Students, Peer Pressure and their Academic Performance in School; International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications (IJSRP) 9(1) (ISSN: 2250-3153), DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.29322/IJSRP.9.01.2019.p8541 Van Lange, P. A., Kruglanski, A. W., & Higgins, E. T. (2012). Handbook of theories of social psychology: volume 1. (Vols. 1-1). SAGE Publications Ltd, https://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215 Wallington, S. A. (1973). Consequences of transgression: Self-punishment and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035576 Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548–573. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548 Weiner, B. (1986). An Attributional Theory of Achievement Motivation and Emotion. In: An Attributional Theory of Motivation and Emotion. SSSP Springer Series in Social Psychology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4948-1_6 Weiner, B. (2006). Social motivation, justice, and the moral emotions: An attributional approach. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410615749 Weiner, B., Graham, S., & Chandler, C. (1982). Pity, anger, and guilt: An attributional analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8(2), 226–232. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167282082007 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION Zaccari, V., Aceto, M., & Mancini, F. (2020). A Systematic Review of Instruments to Assess Guilt in Children and Adolescents. Frontiers in psychiatry, 11, 573488. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.573488 28 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 29 Appendix A A Survey on Students’ Guilt and Academic Effort Expenditure Motivation Level with GuiltInduced Dissonance Manipulation 1. What is your race? ▢ White or Caucasian ▢ Black or African American ▢ Hispanic or Latino ▢ Asian or Asian American ▢ American Indian or Alaska Native ▢ Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander ▢ Another race 2. What is your gender identity? ▢ Male ▢ Female ▢ Non-binary / third gender ▢ Androgyne ▢ Bigender ▢ Genderfluid INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION ▢ 30 Other/Prefer not to say Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements using the scale provided 3. To what extent do you value academic work? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 4. To what extent do you think your academic work benefits your future career? 0 1 Not at all 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very Much 5. Have you ever failed to meet your family’s expectations of your academic work? o Yes o No 6. Write down in the space provided below your experience of when your academic performance failed to meet your family’s expectations? ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 31 ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements using the scale provided 7. How guilty do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 8. How proud do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 9. How sad do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 Not at all Much 1 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 32 10. How happy do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 11. I felt responsible for the negative experience I wrote down in the previous section. 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much Please read and answer the following questions about your study habits in general 12. How many hours did you spend studying by yourself per week outside of classroom when you had the experience you wrote down in the previous section? _______hours/week on average 13. How many hours do you think you should have spent studying by yourself per week outside of classroom when you had the experience you wrote down in the previous section? _______hours/week on average INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 33 14. How many hours do you intend to spend studying by yourself per week outside of classroom during the next semester? _______hours/week on average 15. Which best reflects your attitude towards studying? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative 1 Neutral 2 Positive Very positive 16. How often do you zone out in class? 0 1 Never 2 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 5 6 Often Always 17. How well do you concentrate in class? 0 1 Not well at all 2 3 4 Slightly well Well Very well 18. How often do you engage in irrelevant activities in class? 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION Never Sometimes 34 Often Always 19. How willing are you to get better at concentrating in class? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 6 Quite Very 1 2 Much 20. Which best reflects your attitude towards school? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative Neutral Positive Very positive 21. How often do you procrastinate on finishing class assignments? 0 1 2 Never 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 Often Always 22. Which best reflects your attitude towards class assignments? -2 Very negative positive -1 0 Negative 1 Neutral 2 Positive Very INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 35 23. How willing are you to achieve higher GPA? 0 1 Not at all 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very Much Appendix B: A Survey on Students’ Guilt and Academic Effort Expenditure Motivation Level with Pride-Induced Dissonance Manipulation 1. What is your race? ▢ White or Caucasian ▢ Black or African American ▢ Hispanic or Latino ▢ Asian or Asian American ▢ American Indian or Alaska Native ▢ Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander ▢ Another race 2. What is your gender identity? ▢ Male ▢ Female INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION ▢ Non-binary / third gender ▢ Androgyne ▢ Bigender ▢ Genderfluid ▢ Other/Prefer not to say 36 Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements using the scale provided 3. To what extent do you value academic work? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 4. To what extent do you think your academic work benefits your future career? 0 1 Not at all 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite Much 5. Have you ever exceeded your family’s expectations of your academic work? o Yes 6 Very INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 37 o No 6. Write down in the space provided below your experience of when your academic performance exceeded your family’s expectations? ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________ Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements using the scale provided 7. How guilty do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 8. How proud do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 Not at all Much 1 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 38 9. How sad do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 10. How happy do you feel about the experience you just described above? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 11. I take credit for the negative experience I wrote down in the previous section. 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much Please read and answer the following questions about your study habits in general 12. How many hours did you spend studying by yourself per week outside of classroom when you had the experience you wrote down in the previous section? _______hours/week on average INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 39 13. How many hours do you think you should have spent studying by yourself per week outside of classroom when you had the experience you wrote down in the previous section? _______hours/week on average 14. How many hours do you intend to spend studying by yourself per week outside of classroom during the next semester? _______hours/week on average 15. Which best reflects your attitude towards studying? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative 1 Neutral 2 Positive Very positive 16. How often do you zone out in class? 0 1 Never 2 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 5 6 Often Always 17. How well do you concentrate in class? 0 1 2 3 4 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION Not well at all Slightly well 40 Well Very well 18. How often do you engage in irrelevant activities in class? 0 1 2 Never 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 5 6 Often Always 19. How willing are you to get better at concentrating in class? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly Quite Very 1 2 Much 20. Which best reflects your attitude towards school? -2 Very negative positive -1 0 Negative Neutral Positive Very INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 41 21. How often do you procrastinate on finishing class assignments? 0 1 2 Never 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 Often Always 22. Which best reflects your attitude towards class assignments? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative 1 Neutral 2 Positive Very positive 23. How willing are you to achieve a higher GPA? 0 1 Not at all 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very Much Appendix C: A Survey on Students’ Guilt and Academic Effort Expenditure Motivation Level without Manipulation 1. What is your race? ▢ White or Caucasian ▢ Black or African American ▢ Hispanic or Latino INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION ▢ Asian or Asian American ▢ American Indian or Alaska Native ▢ Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander ▢ Another race 42 2. What is your gender identity? ▢ Male ▢ Female ▢ Non-binary / third gender ▢ Androgyne ▢ Bigender ▢ Genderfluid ▢ Other/Prefer not to say Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements using the scale provided 3. To what extent do you value academic work? 0 Not at all Much 1 2 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 43 4. To what extent do you think your academic work benefits your future career? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 5. How positive does education here at school make you feel? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly 5 Quite 6 Very Much 6. How willing are you to study harder at school? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 Slightly 4 5 Quite 6 Very Much Please read and answer the following questions about your study habits in general 7. How many hours did you spend studying by yourself per week outside of classroom when you had the experience you wrote down in the previous section? _______hours/week on average INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 44 8. How many hours do you think you should have spent studying by yourself per week outside of classroom when you had the experience you wrote down in the previous section? _______hours/week on average 9. How many hours do you intend to spend studying by yourself per week outside of classroom during the next semester? _______hours/week on average 10. Which best reflects your attitude towards studying? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative 1 Neutral 2 Positive Very positive 11. How often do you zone out in class? 0 1 Never 2 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 5 6 Often Always 12. How well do you concentrate in class? 0 1 2 3 4 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION Not well at all Slightly well 45 Well Very well 13. How often do you engage in irrelevant activities in class? 0 1 2 Never 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 5 6 Often Always 14. How willing are you to get better at concentrating in class? 0 1 2 Not at all 3 4 Slightly Quite Very 1 2 Much 15. Which best reflects your attitude towards school? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative Neutral Positive Very positive 16. How often do you procrastinate on finishing class assignments? 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION Never Sometimes 46 Often Always 17. Which best reflects your attitude towards class assignments? -2 -1 Very negative 0 Negative 1 Neutral 2 Positive Very positive 18. How willing are you to achieve higher GPA? 0 Not at all 1 2 Slightly 3 4 Quite 5 6 Very Much INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 47 Author Note I would like to thank Professor Seiji Takaku for supporting this study as a faculty mentor and principal investigator. I would also like to give my appreciation to all the student participants at Soka University of America, Aliso Viejo for taking part in my experiment, thereby helping me complete my research project. Correspondence regarding the present research should be addressed to Thuy Mai Bui, Soka University of America, 1 University Drive, Aliso Viejo, CA 92656 (e-mail: tbui@soka.edu). INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 48 Table 3 Effects of Dissonance-Induced Guilt Manipulation on Level of Guilt Induced-guilt Dependent variable Guilt Induced-pride M SD M SD t(47.5) 2.90 1.73 .54 1.09 6.44*** Note. NInduced-guilt = 30, NInduced-pride = 35 ***p < .001. Table 4 Effects of Dissonance-Induced Guilt Manipulation on Main Dependent Variables Induced-guilt Induced-pride Control Dependent variable M SD M SD M SD F(2,117) Attitude towards education .43 .58 .41 .83 .36 .76 .116 Engagement 3.29 .94 3.56 1.12 3.17 .87 1.776* Hours intend to spend 18.07 13.06 14.34 8.72 13.69 11.07 1.619* studying Note. Higher numbers indicate greater degree of each variable *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Table 5 Correlation Coefficients for Variables Entered in Bivariate Analysis (Induced-Guilt Condition) 1. Valuation of academic work 2. Hours intend to spend studying 1 2 3 4 __ .126 .414* .149 .126 __ .326 .012 INDUCED GUILT & SELF-DIRECTED ACADEMIC MOTIVATION 3. Attitudes towards education .414* .326 4. Engagement .149 .012 __ .463** .463** __ 49 Note. N = 30 *p <.05. **p < .01. Table 6 Correlation Coefficients for Variables Entered in Bivariate Analysis (Induced-Pride Condition) 1 2 3 4 __ .332 .367* .392* 2. Hours intend to spend studying .332 __ .253 .221 3. Attitudes towards education .367* .253 __ .247 4. Engagement .392* .221 .247 __ 1. Valuation of academic work Note. N = 35 *p < .05. Table 7 Correlation Coefficients for Variables Entered in Bivariate Analysis (Control Condition) 1 2 3 __ .222 .399** .019 2. Hours intend to spend studying .222 __ .254 .009 3. Attitudes towards education .399** .254 __ -.091 4. Engagement .019 1. Valuation of academic work Note. N = 55 *p < .05. **p < .01. .009 -0.91 4 __