Information Processing Theory and Learning Disabilities: A Commentary and Future Perspective H. Lee Swanson domain-specific processes related to academic performance include recognizing word patterns, exercising decisions or rules for emitting math responses, formulating heuristic hypotheses about how to spell an unknown word, and producing algorithms for problem solving in math, reading, and spelling (e.g., Gerber & Hall, 1987; Pellegrino & Goldman, 1987; Samuels, 1987). All of these mediating activities are largely under the control of the learner. COMMENTS AND The present article reviews some assumptions related to the information processing CRITIQUE framework. The major implications ofprevious articles are reviewed, as well as some conceptual ambiguities related to the role ofthe automaticity, prior knowledge, and ex-A number of important points reecutivefunctioning in explaining learning disabilities. The ecological validity ofthe inlated to the articles have been made in formation processingframework,as well as the limitations of differential theories, the is introduction of each special issue discussed. Discussion is also given to: (a) identifying common denominations, (b) and sub- will not be repeated here. Howgrouping, (c) isolating mental components underlying academic change, and (d) incorever, one uniformly positive point porating neuropsychological data into future research practice. across all articles is that attempts have been made to understand learning disperusal of the literature suggests As suggested by the articles in this abled children's functioning in terms that differences between learning series, one emerging theoretical of mental processes that contribute to disabled and nondisabled children on framework for describing learning performance. While traditional modcognitive tasks are virtually limitless, disabled children's performance on els of information processing have and it has been possible to describe cognitive tasks has been information focused on speed of processing and those differences in a variety of ways. processing theory. Within this frame- used simple tasks that control for While this state of affairs may reflect work, learning disabled students, as isolated variables (e.g., Jensen, 1981), the generalized deficits of learning well as their nondisabled counter- the present articles have been more indisabled children, it may also reflect parts, are perceived as learning terested in emphasizing aspects of task an amorphous conceptual framework through various intervening stages of performance which include strategies that underlies the discipline. As noted cognition such as encoding, organiz- and/or more complex forms of proby several commentators, a great deal ing, storing, retrieving, comparing, cessing, and have tended to discount of research in learning disabilities is and generating (reconstructing) infor- speed of mental processing. generated out of a "shotgun" approach mation. These stages also include The articles also appropriately caprather than "a carefully planned body higher- and lower-order processes ture a broad continuum of instruction of research guided by either practical (e.g., mechanisms that regulate and for LD students. At one end of the inor theoretical concerns" (Pressley, deploy relevant skills versus elemen- structional continuum, the teacher is Heisel, McCormick & Nakamura, tary processing components). Greatly viewed as one who acts as a model and 1982, p. 139). Likewise, others suggest simplified, learning disabled chil- interrogator of the child's strategic that LD research is plagued by enor- dren's task performance is seen as an thinking, as well as one who engineers mous conceptual problems (e.g., interaction between executive rou- instructional activities that influence Backman, Mamen, & Ferguson, 1984) tines, control processes, and their the child's strategic use of mental reand that it "has yielded little (if any) knowledge base. As suggested in the sources. As the learner's self-regulareliable information for psychology" various articles, the mechanisms in- tory control eventually becomes more (Humphreys, Lynch, Revelle, & Hall, volved in this interaction may involve internalized, the teacher's level of par1983, p. 56). Thus, in considering the the organization and elaboration of ticipation diminishes (e.g., Baker & myriad studies published in the area information (Pressley, Johnson, & Sy- Brown, 1984). It is assumed that cerof learning disabilities, some con- mons, in press), the activation of tain instructional activities influence sideration must be given toward uni- hemispheric resources (Hiscock & the learner's executive control funcfying the diversity of isolated research Kinsbourne, this issue), conscious tions. For example, Palincsar and in order to provide a theoretical awareness of cognitive processes (e.g., Brown (in press) and Pressley et al. (in framework that helps us determine Palincsar & Brown, in press), and at- press) suggest the possibility of enwhich studies are important and tentional capacity constraints (e.g., hancing LD students' metacognitive Samuels, 1987), to name a few. More knowledge about learning as a means which ones are trivial. A Volume 20, Number 3, March 1987 155 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 of further influencing executive control skills that monitor strategies across various tasks. Metacognitive knowledge (i.e., learners' awareness and knowledge of their own learning processes, as well as their abilities and tendencies to control those processes during learning) is viewed as providing mental input to the executive system, which in turn organizes and mobilizes relevant information processing skills and subskills. At the other end of the continum, some articles place a focus on processing skills and subskills that must be performed automatically. It is assumed that the ability to perform deliberate and effortful tasks, such as reading, math, and spelling, requires the automatic and rapid deployment of relevant subskills. Within this context, instruction which includes computer-based drill and practice is viewed as a possible medium for training subskills (e.g., Goldman & Pellegrino, this issue). When combining both ends of the continuum, instruction for the LD child may be conceptualized as moving through a metacognitive training phase in which the learning environment consciously directs, encourages, or elicits learning strategies toward a more automatic and less controlled form of processing. Thus, while some articles focus on raising the LD student's "metacognitive consciousness," others are directed toward reducing the conscious level of activity to an automatic state. This continuum from highly effortful, conscious processing to processing that occurs without awareness, effort, or intention appropriately represents the continuum of difficulties experienced by the LD student. In short, improvement in the learning ability of LD children necessitates not only the deployment of strategies, but also an executive mechanism that automatically accesses and combines learning skills (i.e., information processing components) when they are needed. There is a need, however, to clarify some of the cognitive operations discussed in the various articles, as well as a need to make some comments about the ecological validity of the in- formation processing framework. To begin with, some comments about the role of automaticity, prior information knowledge, and the executive function in processing ability are in order. AUTOMATICITY We note from the literature that the term automaticity has been used to describe several sorts of mental operations. Sometimes the term represents preattentive involuntary processing, initiated by stimulus events (e.g, Neisser, 1967), and at other times it represents processes independent of capacity demands in the information processing system (e.g., Hasher & Zacks, 1979; LaBerge & Samuels, 1974). Shiffrin and Schneider (1977), for example, characterize automaticity as involving rapid, involuntary, parallel processing, as opposed to slow, sequential, and subjectcontrolled processing. The majority of articles in this series assume that cognitive processes place space and/or time demands upon a limited-central capacity system (see Logan, 1978). Processing that becomes more and more independent of this capacity system, because of practice and experience, is assumed to be automatic. For example, learning to read is initially a laborious activity and proceeds on a letter-by-letter basis, requiring much attention on the part of the child. After considerable practice and experience, reading becomes faster, letters are processed in parallel, and words are activated with little attentional load (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974). This conceptualization, however, is in contrast to other accounts which view automaticity as an unlearned process (e.g., Kahneman, 1973; Neisser, 1967). Further, some authors (e.g., Cheng, 1985) have suggested that automatic processing may not represent an activity that becomes capacity free, but rather reflect an efficient restructuring, reordering, and/or reorganizing of tasks. My point is that only when a theoretical model can demonstrate the various learnable and nonlearnable characteristics of automaticity will one have an explanatory sense of LD children's academic prob- lems. Thus, articles (e.g., Gerber & Hall, 1987; Kolligian & Sternberg, 1987; Samuels, 1987) describing the various cognitive activities of learning disabled children as reflecting poor automaticity are further enhanced if their description is embodied in a theoretical framework that accounts for many different sorts of data (see Sternberg & Powell, 1982). From an instructional perspective, of course, the issue may not be whether automatic processing is learnable or capacityfree in learning disabled students, but whether such children can efficiently and quickly monitor and/or smoothly coordinate mental processes on a particular task, such as reading. KNOWLEDGE BASE A second concern is that some articles place little emphasis on the interaction between previously stored information and strategy utilization (however, see Pressley et al., in press). The majority of the articles in this series have tended to focus on subjectcontrolled processing and ignore the "information" part of information processing theory. Some recent studies (e.g., Baker, Ceci, & Herrmann, in press; Swanson, 1986) have indicated that the differences between LD and nondisabled children reflect not only strategic or control processing, but also differences in the basic information stored in long-term memory. This finding has important implications for information processing models (e.g., see Chi, 1978; Chi & Koeske, 1983; Keil, 1982). For example, Chi (1978) demonstrated that very young children proficient in chess or dinosaur classification can show adult-like strategies, whereas adult novices show child-like problem solving behavior. The implication here is that dramatic differences observed in the performance of nondisabled and LD students may often be consequences of differences in underlying knowledge bases (e.g., cognitive structures prior to learning) rather than of basic differences in control processes. A child's knowledge base places formal restrictions on the class of logically possible strategies that can be used within a Journal of Learning Disabilities 156 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 given academic domain (e.g., Chi, 1978). Thus, one may assume that LD students bring to a learning task a set structure that limits the class of strategies they are likely to use. tualized as having, at best, an indirect effect on executive functioning. dards, there is still a reluctance to consider new "processing" paradigms. From a practical standpoint, teachers ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY are not likely to become interested in theories of the learning process when Criticisms related to validating the LD children are going to be placed in information processing model to the standardized programs anyway. That EXECUTIVE FUNCTION field of learning disabilities are also in is, when LD children's functioning is A final concern is the issue over order. Two comments can be made conceptualized in terms of behavioral whether the executive control function related to the articles. First, only objectives and/or performance outcan be trained (on this issue, see also minimal information was given on the comes, a specification of such chilGreeno & Simon, 1984; Sternberg, explicit basis for selecting a task in dren's "processing abilities" may be 1983). The research reviewed in this order to understand learning dis- seen as impractical and unnecessary. series (e.g., Palincsar & Brown, in abilities. No doubt, the tasks selected Further, there are time constraints, as press; Pressley et al., in press) suggests should clearly identify and isolate well as administrative pressures, that that an instructional focus be placed mental components that correlate prevent practitioners from engaging in upon enhancing LD students' meta- highly with performance parameters the luxury of clearly conceptualizing cognitive knowledge about learning as in the real world. This is no trivial task the processing difficulties of children a means for developing executive con- when one considers that information suspected of having learning distrol skills involved in the maintenance processing research to date has not abilities (e.g., Doris, 1986). and transfer of learning "actics. A given much attention to the context in theme that has emerged from some of which learning occurs. Thus, the ques- EMERGENCE OF these articles is that executive skills tion of which repertoire of informa- INFORMATION may not be trained easily, but must be tion processing tasks can better pre- PROCESSING gradually developed and automated dict later performance over more over time (see Pressley, Borkowski, & traditional psychometric measures The idea that learning disabled stuO'Sullivan, 1984). Some authors (e.g., will remain unsettled (see Jensen, dents are deficient in information proGagne, 1980a, 1980b), however, argue 1981) until clear specification is given cessing is certainly not a new one. At that executive functions cannot be to the cognitive processes required for the inception of information processdirectly taught. For example, while LD real-life activities. Further, even if ing model development, several austudents can be induced to rehearse, these processes are identified, it is un- thors (e.g., Senf, 1972) were acknowltake notes, outline a text, and so on, as certain whether these processes can be edging that learning disabled students well as respond to explicit instructions easily reduced to stable information had restricted access to information concerning the significance of strat- processing components. An obvious processing activities. Perhaps the reaegies and the range of their utility, ex- example of such instability is noted son why information processing has ecutive function capability (e.g., the when parents and teachers report that not been viewed as a dramatic feature ability to weigh and choose from sev- learning disabled children cannot at- of the learning disabled field up until eral strategies) is much more difficult tend to instruction in the classroom, now is that the deficits being identified to impart. Further, because the execu- but can watch television over an ex- were tolerable. Children are not likely tive control function involves the com- tended period of time and remember to be placed in an LD classroom bining of relevant prior knowledge, detailed information. because they suffer strategy and metaand because these knowledge strucSecond, little discussion was given cognitive difficulties or because they tures evolve slowly through years of to the resistance of practitioners, as suffer from problems in executive proschooling, such a mechanism is not well as countertheorists, to the infor- cessing (i.e., self-regulation of cogni(or is less) susceptible to direct train- mation processing framework (see tion). It is my opinion that one of the ing. In addition, it may also be argued Farnham-Diggory, 1986, for further reasons for the recent upsurge of inthat a positive influence of the execu- discussion on this point). This resis- terest in information processing thetive function or routine occurs when tance can occur at many levels and ory is that, for three decades now, the child can monitor or self-regulate from many perspectives. For example, research on LD children has not prolower-order mechanisms (e.g., phono- from a historical perspective one vided significant theoretical or logical coding) that are stable (e.g., Bel- could argue that remedial programs practical directions toward a science mont, Butterfield, & Ferretti, 1982; based upon earlier models of informa- of learning disabilities (see Kavale & Brown & Campione, 1981). Unfortun- tion processing were largely unsuc- Forness, 1985b). Few, if any, substanately, instability of such mechanisms cessful. Although many models of tive scientific outcomes have been acis most likely a hallmark of being remediation were conceptualized in complished in terms of operationalizlearning disabled (see Swanson, the 40s and 50s and the theoretical ing, preventing, or ameliorating 1984a; Swanson, 1985). Thus, meta- models for such programs would be learning disabilities. To the researcher cognitive training may be concep- considered incorrect by today's stan- interested in cognition, the term 157 Volume 20, Number 3, March 1987 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 "learning disabilities" is still fluctuating and elusive. It is my opinion that part of the reason for this elusiveness has been due to the field's over-reliance on differential or psychometric theories. LIMITATIONS OF DIFFERENTIAL THEORY Differential theories are based upon the assumption that latent sources of individual differences are related to mathematically derived factors. While interpretations of factor analyses are equivocal, the goal of such procedures is to isolate global constellations of LD children's functioning. In a practical sense, this approach assumes that standardized tests assess individual differences along some continuum, and that test items reflect a particular construct; otherwise the test will be of little interest or value as a descriptive and/or predictive instrument. Although a psychometric approach does predict academic success fairly well (e.g., Jensen, 1981), such an approach has difficulty in (a) defining the mental processes necessary for effective learning, (b) identifying subprocesses underlying academic tasks as well as global processes required for school learning, and (c) providing a link between cognitive theory and educational practices (see Wagner & Sternberg, 1984, for a review). Further, it has been suggested that standardized tests have questionable ecological validity (e.g., Swanson, 1984a) and that clinical interpretations for popularly used tests have poor experimental support (e.g., see Kavale & Forness, 1984). Perhaps the most serious limitation to a differential approach, at least in the LD field, is that such an orientation obscures the specific learning disability we are trying to find. We know, for example, that LD children perform less well than nondisabled students on a multitude of cognitive tasks. Recognizing this, most practitioners concerned with the cognitive problems of the learning disabled focus on a differential deficit, that is, a greater deficit in one (or more) subtest than in another. Unfortunately, poor subtest performance does not necessarily repre- sent a differential deficit in ability. It may instead reflect learning disabled children's generalized performance deficits, coupled with the fact that one or some of the subset items measure these generalized deficits better than others do. Thus, low scores on various test items cannot be interpreted as indicating a specific learning disability. Stated differently, standardized tests or measures may provide incomplete representations of children's knowledge bases and possibly obscure our understanding of the processes LD children use to acquire knowledge. In search of a better conceptualization of the cognitive deficits in learning disabled children, many researchers have turned to an information processing approach. It is not my intent, however, to suggest that psychometric approaches be abandoned, but rather put into perspective. Thus, psychometric research programs (e.g., research emanating from the University of Minnesota Learning Disability Institute) may be rather limited, at least in a theoretical sense, in helping the profession create a science of learning disabilities (see McKinney, 1983). Therefore, some consideration must be given to how an information processing framework can contribute to our understanding of learning disabilities. We will briefly consider four possibilities. COMMON DENOMINATOR Information processing theory may provide a possible framework for identifying common processing patterns in all children identified as learning disabled. This approach is in contrast to current approaches which search for unitary or specific deficits (see Poplin, 1984, for a critique of this approach); here, the focus is on specifying how the components at various stages and levels—for example, the relationships between micro-, macroand metacomponents (see Sternberg, 1983 for discussion)—interact with learning conditions (e.g., Detterman, 1980). Not until we understand how these components interact, and hence become coordinated, will we have a sense of atypical or inefficient patterns of processing in LD children. I assume that if a cogent theory and instructional program for LD children is to emerge, some common denominators or patterns of information processing must be identified in order to conceptualize, unify, and predict such children's performance. Thus, I assume that certain inefficient patterns of processing are commonly shared among children who have learning disabilities. Of course, this assumption is in contrast to previous commentaries (e.g., Ysseldyke et al., 1983), which have suggested that we abandon efforts at finding out "what learning disability really is" and get on with the practical task of instructing LD children. Unfortunately, it appears that such an approach is unsatisfactory. The practical task of instruction represents, in many cases, a standardized program in a school district not motivated by guidelines related to learning theory or cognitive training. In addition, one of the major tenets of the practical task of instruction—individualization—in many cases only represents a partial approximation to the concept (e.g., see commentary by Doris, 1986, in which individualization unfortunately represents the speed at which the LD child advances through a fixed curriculum). Beyond these contradictions, however, I would suggest that if any science of the practical aspects of teaching learning disabilities is to exist, we must converge on some unified descriptive explanation of the behaviors that such children exhibit so that meaningful instruction and/or intervention can occur (see Sternberg, 1983; Sternberg, Ketron, & Powell, 1982, for related discussion). As suggested earlier, one research area that may yield valuable information in identifying common patterns among LD children has to do with how LD children access, execute, and coordinate several mental components across various academic tasks. It has traditionally been assumed that LD children are of normal or above-normal intelligence but suffer from specific clusters of learning problems revealed on intelligence and/or academic tests. The assumpJournal of Learning Disabilities 158 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 tion is that these specific, identifiable related to self-evaluation (e.g., predictclusters of abilities are involved in ing outcomes, organizing strategies, learning impairment (e.g., reading, using various forms of trial and error), math). Unfortunately, this assumption and certain subprocesses are relatively is questionable (e.g., Wagner & Stern- familiar or automatized (see Spear & berg, 1984), and it may be further Sternberg, 1986; Sternberg & Wagner, argued that specific skill deficiencies 1982), training attempts are successful do not represent difficulties in specific(e.g., Palincsar & Brown, 1981, 1984; processes, but rather reflect difficulties Wong & Swatsky, 1984). In addition, in the coordination of such processes these children suffer from deficits in (see Swanson, 1984a). Likewise, others such mental operations as logically (e.g., Palincsar & Brown, in press) have organizing and coordinating incomalso suggested this possibility, by sug- ing information and carrying out gesting that the LD field focus on mental operations (e.g., Swanson, decision-making mechanisms (i.e., 1982). Thus, the research clearly sugmetacognition) that influence the co- gests that a focus on isolated deficienordination of mental processes. Fur- cies (i.e., specific processing deficienther, several studies within the last few cies) fails to capture the regulative years have supported the assumption nature of LD children's functioning. that LD children have difficulty regu- That is, learning disabilities may be lating (i.e., accessing and coordinating) the result of a unique regulation or a number of mental activities. The coordination of mental processes rathresearch in this area may be sum- er than a specific type of processing marized as follows: LD children ex- deficiency (see Swanson, 1985, for a perience difficulty with such self- related discussion). regulating mechanisms as checking, planning, monitoring, testing, revis- SUBGROUP SELECTION ing, and evaluating during an attempt to learn or solve problems (e.g., Bauer, Information processing theory may in press; Barclay & Hagen, 1982; provide a tentative framework for the Brown & Palincsar, 1982; Hallahan, selection of subgroups. Contrary to Lloyd, Kosiewicz, Kaufman, & what was.called for in the previous disGraves, 1979; McKinney & Haskins, cussion (i.e., identification of common 1980; Scruggs, Mastropieri, & Levin, in information processing patterns), press; Swanson, 1984b; Wong, in present evidence indicates that press). Such children perform poorly school-labeled learning disabled chilon a variety of tasks that require the dren routinely contradict the assumpuse of general control processes or tion that some common denominator strategies for solution (e.g., Bauer, underlies performance (e.g., Keogh, 1979, 1982; Bauer & Emhert, 1984; Major-Kingsley, Omori-Gordon, & Brainerd, Kingman, & Howe, 1986; Reid, 1982). Given that more than half Dallego & Moely, 1980; Howe, Brain- of the published research in learning erd, & Kingma, 1985; Tarver, Halla- disabilities is based upon students han, Kaufman, & Ball, 1976; also see receiving services from established Bauer, in press; Worden, 1983, for public school programs for the learnreview). Under some conditions, well- ing disabled, it is reasonable to ask designed strategy training improves what characterizes this population. In performance (e.g., Torgesen & Houck, my home state, it was estimated that 1980; Torgesen, Murphy, & Ivey, 1979; over 50% of the students receiving serWong, 1978,1979,1982), while at other vices for the learning disabled did not times general cognitive constraints match conventional definitions of prevent effective use of control pro- learning disabilities (Shepard, Smith, cesses (e.g., Shankweiler, Liberman, & Vojir, 1983). In a companion study, Mark, Fowler, & Fischer, 1979; Swan- Shepard and Smith (1983) analyzed son, 1984b; Torgesen & Houck, 1980; records of 1,000 pupils receiving serWong, Wong, & Foth, 1977). However, vices in programs designed for the when training of information process- learning disabled; they reported that ing components includes instructions only 28% of the students met defini- tions of learning disabilities. More than 50% of the students were better described by other indicators (e.g., non-English dominant, minor behavior disorders, slow learners). Recent attempts to remedy this state of affairs have resulted in reports on learning disability definitions (e.g., Hammill, Leigh, McNutt, & Larsen, 1981; Smith et al., 1984). Unfortunately, such reports have not converged on learning paradigms in which to "test out" or operationally refine such definitions. Because LD students have high intersubject as well as intrasubject variability on most cognitive measures, one strategy to control for some of the subject variability has been to identify subgroups within learning disabilities. There are obvious advantages to subgrouping in our understanding of learning disabilities: (a) Subgroups reduce a large amount of data about LD subjects to a manageable size and (b) subgrouping forces researchers to specify in a precise manner the important parameters of LD functioning. Some of the recent subgrouping schemes have included behavioral characteristics (McKinney, 1984; Speece, McKinney, & Appelbaum, 1985), memory performance (Torgesen & Houck, 1980; Torgesen, Rashotte, Greenstein, Houck, & Portes, in press), language ability (Ceci, Lea, & Ringstrom, 1980), achievement ability (e.g., Siegel & Linder, 1984), lexical ability (Swanson, in press), and neuropsychological profiles (e.g., Lyon & Watson, 1981), to mention a few. If subgrouping is successful, variability should be less within one group and greater from one subgroup to another. There are, however, two major stumbling blocks to the subtyping enterprise. First, a taxonomy of subtypes will not be readily accepted by fellow researchers until the definitional issues are resolved and some theoretical framework is agreed upon. In terms of definitional issues, one problem noted in the literature is that the apparent heterogeneity of the learning disabled population may not be the result of "real heterogeneity" but, rather, may reflect definitions that are not specific (see Farnham-Diggory, 1986; Keogh et al., 1982). Thus, it appears logical to 159 Volume 20, Number 3, March 1987 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 conclude that subgrouping studies on poorly specified LD samples merely obscure the commonalities that may exist within the learning disability population. In terms of theoretical issues, it may be argued that the majority of subgrouping studies use psychometric measures or clinical instruments that do not reflect a theoretical framework of learning or cognition. Thus, tasks are chosen arbitrarily which in turn give rise to a post-hoc theory of task performance. An example of this post-hoc conceptualization is most likely to occur in studies which have been based upon empirically derived subgroups. This occurs because, at present, there is no satisfactory method for determining the number of clusters (subgroups) for any type of cluster analysis (Everitt, 1980). Thus, when definitions are nonspecific, when there is a lack of theoretical integration in the choice of measures, and when the number of subgroups must be decided upon somewhat arbitrarily, Nimzovitch's dictum comes into operation: When there is no good move, a botch will come along to fill the breach. Second, the establishment of subtypes is based upon the assumption that learning disabilities can be assigned to a category having some unitary causes and well-defined effects. A review of the subtyping literature has not yielded such conclusions (e.g., Siegel & Heaven, 1986). For example, one of the major difficulties with attempts at subdividing LD children into more homogeneous groups is that low or high performance on the various standardized measures (e.g., reading tests) may be due to several underlying causes (e.g., see Siegel & Heaven's [1986] discussion on the various causes of poor performance on standardized reading tests). Perhaps one solution to the problem of determining unitary causes is to study the relationships between information processing constructs that synthesize several skills (e.g., metacognition, executive function, working memory) in learning disabled populations as a whole (see Jorm, 1983, for a discussion). Thus, a focus is placed on overlapping disabilities rather than on separating the population in terms of isolated or specific processing deficiencies or disabilities. This does not mean that a subgrouping of overlapping disabilities on macrocomponents of information processing should merely reflect graduations in severity. Rather, particular subgroup classifications are only meaningful when it is discovered after intervention that one LD subgroup's performance differs in one direction and another subgroup differs in the opposite direction form the NLD group. MENTAL COMPONENTS UNDERLYING ACADEMIC CHANGE The information processing framework may provide for a clarification of the changes that occur in mental processing as a result of classroom learning. Farnham-Diggory (1986) has suggested that the issue of definition and identification of learning disabilities will subside once "the processes identified in the laboratory enter into the performance of school tasks" (p. 134). Therefore, research needs to be directed toward identifying the mental processes required on selected classroom tasks. Likewise, complex models for assessing mental processes must be sensitive to changes that occur during actual instruction on a day-to-day basis. One step in this direction is to use measures of processing activity during classroom instruction. That is, one can assume that, in order to adequately assess whether a particular intervention has facilitated a child's learning, changes in the supporting processes must be measured. It is assumed that, in reading, math, or spelling, certain processes mediate lowerlevel classroom competencies (rate of work), as well as higher-level activities (e.g., reading). To illustrate how this can be done, let us consider a pilot study recently completed in a LD classroom (Swanson, Kosleski, & Stegink, in preparation). Two learning disabled adolescents, who had serious memory and reading (i.e., comprehension) problems, were given two tasks. One task required remembering critical details of various stories read orally from a newspaper column on a daily basis. The second task, which assessed any possible generalization of training effect from the primary task, required the retrieval of critical information on a social studies assignment. Intervention on the primary task required the child to use a mnemonic strategy to organize main ideas and to link the main ideas in terms of an outline. Mental processes were assessed by asking the children to verbally report (see Ericsson & Simon, 1980, for a discussion related to the validity of verbal protocol procedures) the "ways" they were remembering the information. Student responses during task performance were tape recorded and the verbal protocols were coded and categorized in terms of the information processing strategies used. As shown in Figure 1, the students were using various processing strategies to sustain and/or direct their prose recall. A majority of these strategies emerged over training sessions (i.e., rehearsal), and in many cases, were sustaining the imagery or visual mnemonic. In addition, it appears that the teacher's intervention strategy had little impact on the social studies assignment, suggesting that the mental processes activated for the primary task were not directly relevant to the secondary activity. Such information is critical in our attempts to isolate some of the mental mechanisms involved in successful and unsuccessful classroom intervention (see ForrestPressley & Gillies, 1983, for a more detailed discussion of the components necessary for transfer). There are several other, more direct, ways to isolate information processing components underlying classroom performance. One obvious approach is to determine whether invariant processing mechanisms—hardware—or strategies and learning under the control of the performer—software—are responsible for performance difficulties. This approach has been discussed elsewhere (e.g., Swanson & Watson, 1982, chapter 6) and includes assessing basic structures (i.e., structure describes the nature of information—sensory memory, short-term 160 Journal of Learning Disabilities Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 components related to psychometric instruments used to assess academic change (e.g., tests used in a pre/post fashion). For example, Carroll (1976) has compiled a list of component processes that arbitrarily define academic and intellectual domains in terms of memory, long-term memory) as well as the operations and/or voluntary strategies under control of the child, such as rehearsal, organizing, or categorizing of information. Another approach, the cognitivecomponent method, compiles a list of GREG 100-j Cognitive Intervention 80-1 10 Strategy 11 12 13 14 13 14 Components * a a -e 8060-1 o a 40-1 20-1 7 8 9 Reading 10 11 12 Comprehension 10080 40 I # | STRATEGY COMPONENTS | 20-J A 10 11 12 13 14 JULIE Cognitive 10 11 trategy -I 1 1 1 1 1 1 I I I I 10 11 f) IMAGERY O *8S0CI*TI0HS/L0H0Tt*M M Q »D»»N«0 0.0.-.IE. Intervention 12 13 14 Components I r — • 12 13 14 Reading Comprehension 100-j oS •2 t • °5« f 80-^ eo-j U ?9 40-j •>« 20-1 e e H 1 1 1 1 1 1 h H 10 1 11 1 — I • 12 13 14 Figure 1. Prose recall, verbally reported strategies, and comprehension as a function of training sessions. information processing components. In order to assess these components, Carroll included psychometric tests from French, Ekstrom, and Price's (1963) Kit of Reference Tests for Cognitive Factors. This set includes 24 factors of cognitive abilities which were further analyzed by Carroll (1976) in terms of operations (i.e., strategies) that must be performed in order for the task to be mastered successfully. These operations are of three kinds: attentional, memorial, and executive. Attentional operations relate to visual searches (e.g., as measured by eye movements); memorial operations relate to storage; searching and retrieving and executive operations relate to judgments about stimulus attributes. Another, although not final, way to understand the influence and/or relationship between mental components and classroom learning activities is to use tasks from the contemporary information processing literature which are sensitive to individual differences. Gadow and Swanson (1986) have listed some of these tasks in hopes of facilitating the inclusion of such measures in future intervention studies. Before any task can be selected, however, several criteria must be kept in mind: 1. The task has to have a history in the information processing literature and have empirical support as a device for isolating the sources of individual differences. 2. There has to be a theoretical rationale for the task (e.g., construct validity), and that task must assess elementary processing mechanisms. 3. The task must be adaptable to moderately language handicapped children, and must lend itself to paper and pencil format, computer simulation, and/or group administration. 4. The task must be logically related and interrelated to performance on a number of academic tasks. In general, it is suggested that these tasks be described in a series of mental operations or processes that the executive function operates upon. Tests of these operations are provided below: Volume 20, Number 3, March 1987 161 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 1. Encoding: The process by which input information is initially analyzed. The child matches the input against past learning. This process also involves the extraction of different types of information from a stimulus. For example, consider a child who is presented a series of words to be remembered for a spelling test. The child may process the words by their orthographic features, the phonological features represented by the printed word, and/or the semantic features represented by the meaning of the word. 2. Elaboration: The process by which connections with the material to be learned are made to other information previously stored. For example, when the child is presented spelling words to remember, he or she forms associations to those words by use of extra ways of mediating the information (visualizing the word "boy", i.e., the process of imagery), proposing or answering questions about the word (e.g., "is a boy the same as a man?"), and categorizing information (e.g., the word "boy" represents one of the two genders) or associating the word with a context (e.g., the teacher uses the word "boy" in several sentences). 3. Transformation: The process by which rules, algorithms, or heuristics are applied to the incoming information (e.g., the word "perceive" follows the I before E except after C rule). In contrast to encoding and elaboration, this process requires the application of previously stored rules about information processing. 4. Storage: The process by which input information is added to the existing information within the mental system. This process forms a memory trace. Forgetting this memory trace can be attributed to an interference from other learning (e.g., the child has difficulty remembering how to spell the word "perceived" for the present spelling test because the previous week's spelling test included the word "received", i.e., proactive interference). The effects of interference can be assessed by the degree to which the child has overlearned and reviewed the material. 5. Retrieval: The process by which information that was previously stored can be made available. The process generally requires the reproduction of information with minimal aids (i.e., free recall, serial recall). For example, the child is asked to spell a word with all letters in their correct order. 6. Searching: The process by which information is accessed by determining the presence or absence of additional properties. For example, a child is asked if any of the word spellings that were forgotten rhyme with the word "receive" (i.e., cued recall). The child may internally develop an aid to remembering the word, or the aid may be external (e.g., teacher induced). 7. Comparing: The process by which information is judged or recognized to be either old or new, same or different, and so on, to information that has been previously stored. For example, a child is asked to pick out 10 words for a review spelling test from a list of 20 words in which 10 words are new. 8. Reconstruction: The process by which the recalled information is based on concepts that tie together fragments or pieces of stored information. It is generally assumed that information recall is not a duplicate of the information encoded (Neisser, 1981). The important point of the previous discussion is that if a meaningful link is to be obtained between information processing abilities and classroom performance, a fine-grained analysis of the learning process is necessary. This fine-grained analysis should occur on school tasks which the learning disabled child is having difficulties mastering. Further, evaluation of instruction related to processing efficiency is not merely a matter of asking whether such instruction directly influences the "macro-components" that improve the oral reading, math computation, etc., but whether such instruction incorporates the information processing components that influence and sustain instructional performance that is appropriate for learning on a day-to-day basis (see Belmont et al., 1982; Campione & Armbruster, 1985, for a related discussion). HEMISPHERIC PROCESSING Information processing theory may provide a means of conceptualizing the neurological deficiencies noted in learning disabled children. We must expect that few theories of learning disabilities will be validated without recourse to neuropsychological data. This notion is supported in recent definitions of learning disabilities that include the qualification that such disabilities are "intrinsic to the individual and presumed to be due to central nervous system dysfunction" (Hammill, Leigh, McNutt, & Larsen, 1981). Further, a committee of the Association for Children and Adults with Learning Disabilities has indicated that the prevention of learning disabilities is related to our understanding of neuropsychology (ACLD Scientific Studies Committee, 1980). Unfortunately, findings of neuropsychologists have not been operationalized in terms of constructs that are usable for the cognitive psychologist and/or educator (see Hiscock & Kinsbourne, this issue). It appears reasonable, however, that trying to understand the mind of the learning disabled child by focusing exclusively on neuropsychological data would be like trying to understand a large time-sharing computer by focusing on a lesion in one of the terminals. Hopfield (1984) has captured this analogy by pointing out that it is impossible to understand the processing functions of a computer by focusing on a computer chip and ignoring its circuitry. One of the ways information processing theory may enhance our understanding of learning disabilities is by providing some unification to the mixed results on hemispheric specialization (Hiscock & Kinsbourne, this issue). Lateral hemispheric specialization of the brain has been employed as a physiological indicator of learning disabled children's processing of information for some time (e.g., Hynd, Obrzut, & Bowen, in press; Obrzut, Hynd, & Boliek, 1986, for a Journal of Learning Disabilities 162 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 review). Some studies show that disabled and nondisabled children show left hemispheric specialization (rightear advantage) for linguistic tasks; however, disabled children are limited in their performance relative to nondisabled children (e.g., Newell & Rugel, 1981; Witelson, 1977). Others suggest that the evidence is inconsistent on whether ability groups actually differ in how they utilize the two hemispheres (e.g., see Hiscock & Kinsbourne, this issue; Young & Ellis, 1981, for a review). Coupled with these mixed results, there have also been many "levels of analyses" which attempt to associate the constructs of hemispheric specialization with cognitive ability (see Hiscock & Kinsbourne, this issue). Thus, conclusions related to disabled children's hemispheric processing and cognitive abilities are extremely mixed and quite diverse (see Obrzut et al., 1986, for a review). Fortunately, information processing frameworks are emerging that account for individual differences on various neuropsychological indices (e.g., see Friedman & Poison, 1981). For example, Friedman and Poison view hemispheric resources as those processes, strategies, or mechanisms from two independent cerebral hemispheres that influence information processing performance (Friedman & Poison, 1981; Friedman, Poison, Dafoe, & Gaskill, 1982; also see Hellige & Wong, 1983). A focus on how these two sources combine has been of recent theoretical interest in learning disabilities (e.g., Kershner, Henninger, & Cooke, 1984; Swanson, 1986; Swanson & Mullen, 1983). The wide range of ability group differences suggests that information processing is characterized by different hemispheric resource combinations, which may or may not require supplies primarily from one or both cerebral hemispheres. For example, some information tasks may require a competition for independent hemispheric resources, while others may be related to the coordination of hemispheric resources (see Friedman et al., 1982). Therefore, because the human system develops a number of alternative means for combining hemispheric resources, it appears necessary to use an information processing model to capture ability group differences. Thus, the poor association between cognitive and behavioral indices of hemispheric specialization, as noted by Hiscock and Kinsbourne (this issue), may reflect individual variations in the use of alternative information processing routes. There are additional neuropsychological indices that lend themselves to an information processing perspective. For example, variations in processing styles may be related to patterns of spontaneous electroencephalographic (EEG) asymmetry (e.g., Ehrlichman & Wiener, 1980). For subjects performing activities that draw resources primarily from the left hemisphere (resources required for verbal analytic processing), there is an increase in alpha waves or idling rhythm over the right hemisphere, while in individuals accessing resources that draw primarily from the right hemisphere (e.g., resources required for spatial synthetic processing) there is usually an increase in alpha or idle rhythm over the left hemisphere. The implication is that some individuals use a different means of processing information, and idling rhythm may be used as one of the many indices of such processing (e.g., Galin & Ornstein, 1972). Additional measures of hemispheric processing differences have been related to reflective eye movements and pupillary responses (e.g., Hardyck, Dronkers, Chiarello, & Simpson, 1985; Schluroff et al., 1986). For example, when subjects are asked a question demanding a certain amount of time to respond (pause time), they avert their eyes before finally answering the question. In sum, an LD student's information processing difficulties may be indirectly reflected in such behavioral indices as laterality, EEG, and eye movement data. It is likely that the use of sophisticated computer-aided techniques will make it possible to incorporate data on evoked potentials (Kolc, Vrjver, & Bouma, 1985; Thatcher & April, 1976). Such procedures would provide a concep- tualization of cerebral activity during learning that goes beyond simply localizing certain processing activity to an area of the brain. It is also possible that computer-based, individualized, instructional programs could be monitored by physiological measures in order to better assess automatic processing. Such procedures could further refine traditional assessment of learning disabled children's performance. There is some evidence that demonstrates the superior validity of psychophysiological and mental indices over standardized achievement measures for predicting student performance (e.g., Ackerman, Anhalt, Dykman, & Holcomb, 1986; Lewis, Rimland, & Callaway, 1976). Consequently, it is possible that physiological correlates of learning, conceptualized within an information processing framework, will advance our understanding and assessment of learning disabilities. CONCLUSIONS If our profession is to fulfill the promise of resolving the acute issues of teaching learning disabled children, it will be necessary to converge on a paradigm for the study of such children. In the absence of such a paradigm, researchers, teachers, and diagnosticians are confronted with a wide range of phenomena. These phenomena are described and interpreted in many different ways. As a result, fact-gathering appears to be random. According to several authors (e.g., Kavale & Forness, 1985a, 1985b), the LD field can be best characterized as being in the very earliest stages of scientific development. In view of this state of affairs, adoption of a paradigm is necessary as the first step en route to a normal science (e.g., Kuhn, 1974). Although the field is far from a consensus on the most appropriate theoretical model, the articles in this series suggest that there is an implicit adherence to a theoretical infrastructure, namely, the language and methods of information processing models of memory and cognition. Within this infrastructure, there are many commonalities. In many cases, the instruc- Volume 20, Number 3, March 1987 163 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 tional implications are complementary (e.g., Goldman & Pellegrino, this issue, vs. Pressley et al., in press), and the characterizations of LD children's performance remain fairly consistent (e.g., metacognitive and automaticity problems). Of course we do not know what model might best characterize learning disabilities. Rather, it is only suggested that the articles represented in this series provide a basic map or route toward a normal science. It should also be recognized that the story of learning disabilities has been one of traveling down some blind alleys. Of course, it is not known whether the information processing model will prove to be a valuable avenue. The research is fairly consistent, however, in suggesting that the efficient academic performance required of students requires them to integrate several kinds of mental capabilities and that, unfortunately, LD children do not efficiently incorporate and/or coordinate mental components into final correct responses. ABOUT THE AUTHOR H. Lee Swanson received his PhDfrom the University of New Mexico. Current research interests include the assessment of learning disabled children's intelligence, semantic memory, and working memory abilities. Address: H Lee Swanson, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Northern Colorado, Greeley, CO 80639. REFERENCES Ackerman, P.T., Anhalt, J.M., Dykman, R, & Holcomb, P. (1986). Effortful processing deficits in children with reading and/or attention disorders. Brain and Cognition, 5, 22-40. ACLD Scientific Studies Committee. (1980). Encounters backgrounder. Pittsburgh, PA: Association for Children and Adults with Learning Disabilities. Backman, J.E., Mamen, M., & Ferguson, H.B. (1984). Reading level design: Conceptual and methodological issues in reading research. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 560-568. Baker, J.G., Ceci, SJ., & Hermann, N.D. (in press). Semantic structure and processing: Implications for the learning disabled child. In H.L. Swanson (Ed.), Memory and learning disabilities. Greenwich, CT.JAI Press. Baker, L, & Brown, A.L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In D. Pearson, R. Ban, M. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 353-394). New York: Longman. Barclay, C.R., & Hagen, J.W. (1982). The development of mediated behavior in children: An alter- native view of learning disabilities. In J.P. Das, RF. Mulcahy, & A.E. Wall (Eds.), Theory and research in learning disabilities (pp. 61-84). New York: Plenum Press. Bauer, R (1979). Memory, acquisition, and category clustering in learning disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 365-383. Bauer, KH (1982). Information processing as a way of understanding and diagnosing learning disabilities. Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities, 2, 33-45. Bauer, R.H. (in press). Control processes as a way of understanding diagnosing and remediating In H.L. Swanson (Ed.), Memory and learning disabilities. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Bauer, R.H., & Emhert, J. (1984). Information processing in reading-disabled and nondisabled children. Journal ofExperimental Child Psychology, 37, 271-281. Belmont, J.M., Butterfleld, EC, & Ferretti, R.P. (1982). To secure transfer of training instruct selfmanagement skills. In D.K. Detterman & R.J. Sternberg (Eds.), How and how much can intelligence be increased?(pp. 147-154). Norwood, NJ.Ablex. Brainerd, CJ, Kingma, J, & Howe, M.L (1986). Long-term memory development and learning disability: Storage and retrieval loci of disabled/ nondisabled differences. In SJ. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neurological aspects of learning disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, A., & Campione, J.C. (1981). Inducing flexible thinking: The problem of access. In M. Friedman, J. Das, & N. O'Conner (Eds.), Intelligence and learning (pp. 515-530). New York: Plenum Press. Brown, A.L., & Palincsar, AS. (1982). Inducing strategic learning from texts by means of informed, self-control training. Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities, 2, 1-18. Campione, J.C, &Armbruster, B. (1985). Acquiring information from texts: An analysis of four approaches. In J.W. Segal, S. Chipman, & K Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning skills (Vol. 1, pp. 297-317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Carroll, J.B. (1976). Psychometric test as cognitive tasks: A new structure of intellect. In LB. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence (pp. 27-57). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ceci, SJ., Lea, S.E.G., & Ringstrom, M.D. (1980). Coding characteristics of normal and learningdisabled 10-year-olds: Evidence for dual pathways to the cognitive system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 785-797. Cheng P.W. (1985). Restructuring versus automaticity: Alternative accounts ofskill acquisition. Psychological Review, 92, 414-423. Chi, M.T.H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In K Siegler (Ed.), Children's thinking: What develops? (pp. 73-96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Chi, M.T.H, & Koeske, KD. (1983). Network presentation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19, 29-39. Dallego, M., & Moely, B. (1980). Free recall in boys of normal and poor reading levels as afunction of task manipulation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 62-78. Detterman, D.K. (1980). Understanding cognitive components before postulating meta components. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 589. Doris, J. (1986). Learning Disabilities. In SJ. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (pp. 3-54). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ehrlichman, H, & Wiener, M.S. (1980). EEG asymmetry during covert mental activity. Psychophysiology, 17, 228-235. Ericsson, KA., & Simon, HA. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215-251. Everitt, B.S. (1980). Cluster analysis. London: Heinemann. Farnham-Diggory, S. (1986). Commentary: Time, now, for a little serious complexity. In SJ. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (pp. 123-158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Forrest-Pressley, D.L., & Gillies, LA. (1983). Children's flexible use of strategies during reading. In M. Pressley & JR. Levin (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research: Educational applications. New York: Springer-Verlag. French, J. W, Erkstrom, K, & Price, L (1963). Kit of reference testsfor cognitivefactors. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Friedman, A., & Poison, M. (1981). Hemispheres as independent resource systems: Limited-capacity processing and cerebral specialization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1031-1058. Friedman, A., Poison, M.C., Dafoe, C, & Gaskill, S. (1982). Dividing attention within and between hemispheres: Testing a multiple resources approach to limited-capacity information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 625650. Gadow, KD, & Swanson, H.L (1986). Assessing drug effects on academic performance. In KD. Gadow (Ed.), Methodological issues in human psychopharmacology (pp. 247-279). Greenwich, CT.JAI Press. Gagne, KM. (1980a). Learnable aspects of human thinking. InA.E. Lawson (Ed.), The psychology of teaching for thinking and creativity. 1980 Yearbook, Association for the Education of Teachers in Science. Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education, Ohio State University. Gagne, KM. (1980b). Learnable aspects of problem solving. Educational Psychologist, 15, 84-92. Galin, D., & Ornstein, K (1972). Lateral specialization of cognitive mode: An EEG study. Psychophysiology, 9, 412-418. Gerber, M„ &Hall, KJ. (1987). Information processing approaches to studying spelling deficiencies. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 34-42. Greeno, J.G., & Simon, HA. (1984). Problem solving and reasoning (Tech. Rep. No. UPTT/LRDC/ ONR/APS-14). Pittsburgh, PA: Learning Research and Development Center. Hallahan, DP, Lloyd, J, Kosiewicz, M., Kaufman, J, & Graves, J. (1979). A self monitoring of attention as a treatmentfor learning disabled boy's off task behavior. Learning Disability Quarterly, 2, 24-32. Hammill, D.D., Leigh, J.E., McNutt, G, & Larsen, S. (1981). A new definition of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 164 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 development research (Vol 2, pp. 125-159). New Learning Disability Quarterly, 4, 336-420. information processing and specific learning disYork: Springer-Verlag. Hardyck, C, Dronkers, N, Chiarello, C, & Simpabilities: A triarchic synthesis. Journal of LearnPressley, M., Johnson, C, & Symons, S. (in press). son, G. (1985). The eyes have it: Exposure times ing Disabilities, 20, 8-17. Elaborating to learn and learning to elaborate. and saccadic movements in visual half-field ex- Kuhn, TS. (1974). The structure of scientific Journal of Learning Disabilities. periments. Brain and Cognition, 4, 430-438. revolutions. Chicago: University ofChicago Press. Hasher, L, & Zacks, RT. (1979). Automatic and ef- LaBerge, B., & Samuels, SJ. (1974). Toward a Samuels, SJ. (1987). Information processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20,18theory of automatic information processing. fortful processes in memory. Journal of Ex22. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293-323. perimental Psychology: General, 108, 356-388. Hellige, J.B., & Wong T.M. (1983). HemisphereLewis, G.W., Rimland, B., & Callaway, E. (1976). Schluroff, M., Zimmerman, T.E., Freeman, R, Hofmeister, K, Lorscheid, T, & Weber, A. (1986). specific interference in dichotic listening: Task Psychobiological predictors of success in a Navy Pupillary responses to syntactic ambiguity ofsenvariables and individual differences. Journal of remedial program (Report No. NPRDC-TR-77tences. Brain and Language, 27, 322-344. Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 21813). San Diego, CA: Navy Personnel Research and Scruggs, T.E., Mastropieri, MA, & Levin, J.R (in 239. Development Center. press). Transformational mnemonic strategies for Hopfield, J J. (1984). Collective processing and Logan, G. (1978). Attention in character-classifilearning disabled students. In H.L. Swanson neural states. In C. Nicolini (Ed.), Modeling and cation tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of (Ed.), Memory and learning disabilities. Greencomponent stages. Journal of Experimental Psyanalysis in geomedicine (pp. 369-389). New York: wich, CT.JAI Press. chology: General, 107, 32-63. World Scientific. Howe, M.L., Brainerd, C.J., & Kingma, J. (1985). Lyon, R, & Watson, B. (1981). Empirically derived Senf G.M. (1972). An information-integration theory and its application to normal reading acsubgroups of learning disabled readers: DiagnosStorage-retrieval processes of normal and learnquisition and reading disability. In N.D. Bryant & ing disabled children: A stages-of-learning tic characteristics. Journal of Learning DisC.E. Kass (Eds.), Leadership Training Institute in analysis of picture-word effects. Child Developabilities, 14, 256-261. learning disabilities: Final report (Vol. 2, pp. 305McKinney, D. (1983). Contributions of the institutes ment, 56, 1120-1133. 391). Tucson: University of Arizona. Humphreys, M.S., Lynch, M.J., Revelle, W., &Hall, for research on learning disabilities. Exceptional Shankweiler, D, Liberman, I.Y., Mark, S.L., FowEducation Quarterly, 4, 129-144. J.W. (1983). Individual differences in short-term ler, LA., & Fischer, F.W. (1979). The speech code memory. In R Dillon & R Schmeck (Eds.), In- McKinney, J.D. (1984). The search for subtypes of and learning to read. Journal of Experimental dividual differences in cognition (pp. 35-63). New specific learning disability. Journal of Learning Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, Disabilities, 17(1), 43-51. York: Academic Press. 531-545. Hynd, G. W., Obrzut, J.E., & Bowen, S.M. (in press).McKinney, J, &Haskins, R (1980). Cognitive trainNeuropsychological basis of attention and ing and the development of problem solving Shepard, LA., & Smith, M.L. (1983). An evaluation of the identification of learning disabled students memory in learning disabilities. In H.L. Swanson strategies. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 1, in Colorado. Learning Disability Quarterly, 6(2), (Ed.), Memory and learning disabilities. Green41-51. wich, CT.JAI Press. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: 115-127 Shepard, LA, Smith, M.L., & Vojir, C.P. (1983). Appleton -Century-Crofts. Jensen, A. (1981). Reaction time and intelligence. In Characteristics of pupils identified as learning M. Friedman, J. Das, & N. O'Connor (Eds.), In- Neisser, U. (1981). John Dean's memory: A case disabled. American Educational Research Jourstudy. Cognition, 9, 1-22. telligence and learning (pp. 39-51). New York: nal, 20(3), 309-331. Plenum Press. Newell, D, & Rugel, R. (1981). Hemispheric specialization in normal and disabled readers. Shiffrin, RM., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled Jorm, A.F. (1983). Specific reading retardation and and automatic human information processing: Journal of Learning Disabilities, 14, 296-297. work memory: A review. British Journal of PsyII. Perceptual learning automatic attending and Obrzut, J.E., Hynd, G.W., & Boliek, CA. (1986). chology, 74, 311-342. general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127Lateral asymmetries in learning disabled chilKahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. 190. dren: A review. In SJ. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects Siegel, L, & Linder, BA. (1984). Short-term Kavale, KA., & Forness, S.R (1984). A metamemory processing in children with reading and of learning disabilities (pp. 441-474). Hillsdale, analysis of the validization of Wechsler scale proarithmetic learning disabilities. Developmental NJ: Erlbaum. file and recategorizations. Learning Disability Psychology, 20, 200-207. Palincsar, A, & Brown, A.L. (1981). Training Quarterly, 7, 136-156. comprehension-monitoring skills in an interac- Siegel, L.S., & Heaven, RK (1986). Categorization Kavale, KA., & Forness, S.R (1985a). The historical of learning disabilities. In SJ. Ceci (Ed.), tive learning game. Unpublished manuscript, foundation oflearning disabilities: A quantitative Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsyUniversity of Illinois. synthesis assessing the validity of Strauss and chological aspects of learning disabilities (pp. Palincsar, AS., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal Werners exogenous versus endogenous distinc95-121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. teaching of comprehension-fostering and tion ofmental retardation. Remedial and Special comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition Smith, D, Deshler, D., Hallahan, D., Lovitt, T, Education, 6, 18-25. and Instruction, 1, 117-175. Robinson, S, Voress, J, & Ysseldyke, J. (1984). Kavale, KA., & Forness, S.R (1985b). The science of Palincsar, AS. & Brown, A. (in press). Enhancing Minimum standards for the description of sublearning disabilities. San Diego: College-Hill instructional time through attention to metajects in learning disabilities research reports. Press. Learning Disability Quarterly, 7, 221-225. cognition. Journal of Learning Disabilities. Keil, F.C. (1982). Intelligence and the rest of cogniPellegrino, J, & Goldman, S. (1987). Information Spear, L.C, & Sternberg RJ. (1986). An information. Intelligence, 6, 1-21. processing and math. Journal of Learning Distion-processing framework for understanding Keogh, B.K, Major-Kingsley, S.M., Omori-Gordon, reading disability. In SJ. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of abilities, 20, 23-32, 57. H, <fe Reid, HP. (1982). A system of marker cognitive, social and neuropsychological aspects Poplin, M. (1984). Summary rationalizations, variables for the field of learning disabilities. of learning disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. apologies and farewell: What we don't know Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press. about the learning disabled. Learning Disability Speece, D.L., McKinney, J.D, & Appelbaum, M.E. Kershner, J, Henninger, P., & Cooke, W. (1984). (1985). Classification and validation of behaviorQuarterly, 7, 130-134. Writing induces a right hemisphere linguistic adal subtypes of learning disabled children. Journal Pressley, M., Borkowski, J.G., & O'Sullivan, J.T. vantage in dysphonetic dyslexic children: Imof Educational Psychology, 77, 66-77. (1984). Memory strategy instruction is made of plications for attention and capacity models of Sternberg RJ. (1983). Criteria for intellectual skills this: Metamemory and durable strategy use. laterality. Brain and Language, 21, 105-122. training. Educational Researcher, 12, 6-12. Educational Psychologist, 19, 94-107. Kolc, A, Vijver, R, & Bouma, A. (1985). Effects of Sternberg RJ, Ketron, J.L., & Powell, JS. (1982). visual-field and matching on event-related poten- Pressley, M„ Heisel, B.E., McCormick, C.B., & Nakamura, G. V. (1982). Memory strategy instruc- Componential approaches to the training of intial and reaction time. Brain and Cognition, 4, telligent performance. In D.K Detterman & RJ. tion. In C.J Brainerd &M. Pressley (Eds.), Verbal 377-387. Sternberg (Eds.), How and how much can inprocesses in children: Progress in cognitive Kolligian, J, & Sternberg, RJ. (1987). Intelligence, 165 Volume 20, Number 3, March 1987 Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015 telligence be increased? (pp. 155-172). Norwood, any processes change because of intervention? right hemispheres and none left. Science, 195, NJAblex. Swanson, H.L, & Mullen, RC (1983). Hemispheric 309-311. Sternberg, R.J., & Powell, J.S. (1982). Theories of in- specialization in learning disabled readers' recall Wong, B. Y.L (1978). The effects ofdirective cues on telligence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook on as a function of age and level of processing. Jourthe organization of memory and recall in good human intelligence (pp. 975-1005). New York: and poor readers. Journal of Educational Renal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 457Cambridge University Press. 477. search, 72, 32-38. Sternberg, R.J., & Wagner, R.K. (1982). Automatiza- Swanson, H.L, & Watson, B. (1982). Educational Wong, B.Y.L. (1979). Increasing retention of main assessment and psychological assessment of exideas through questioning strategies. Learning tion failure in learning disabilities. Topics in Disability Quarterly, 2(2), 42-47. ceptional children. St. Louis: Mosby. Learning and Learning Disabilities, 2, 1-11. Swanson, H.L. (1982). Conceptual processes as a Tarver, S, Hallahan, D, Kauffman, J., & Ball, D. Wong, B. Y.L. (1982). Strategic behaviors in selecting (1976). Verbal rehearsal and selective attention in retrieval cues in gifted, normal achieving and function of age and enforced attention in learning children with learning disabilities: A development learning disabled children. Journal of Learning disabled children: Evidence for deficient rule lag. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Disabilities, 15, 33-37. learning. Contemporary Educational Psychol22, 375-385. Wong, B. Y.L. (in press). Directions of future research ogy, 7, 152-160. in metacognition and learning disabilities. In Swanson, H.L (1984a). Process assessment of in- Thatcher, R. W, & April, R.S. (1976). Evoked potential correlates ofsemantic information processing H.L. Swanson (Ed.), Memory and learning distelligence in learning disabled and mentally rein normals and aphasics. In R. Rieber (Ed.), The abilities. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. tarded children: A multidirectional model. Eduneurospychology of language: Essays in honor of Wong, B.Y.L, & Swatsky, D. (1984). Sentence cational Psychologist, 19, 149-162. elaboration and retention of good, average and Eric Lennebert. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Swanson, H.L. (1984b). Semantic and visual poor readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 6, memory codes in learning disabled readers. Jour- Torgesen, J, & Houck, G (1980). Processing 229-236. deficiencies in learning disabled children who nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37(1), 124perform poorly on the digit span test. Journal of Wong, B.Y.L., Wong R., & Foth, D. (1977). Recall 140. Educational Psychology, 72, 141-160. and clustering of verbal materials among normal Swanson, H.L (1985). Assessing learning disabled children's intellectual performance: An informa- Torgesen, J., Murphy, H, & Ivy, G. (1979). The effects and poor readers. Bulletin of the Psychonomic of an orienting activity on the memory performSociety, 10, 375-378. tion processing perspective. In K. Gadow (Ed.), ance of reading disabled children. Journal of Worden, P.E. (1983). Memory strategy instruction Advances in learning and behavior disabilities Learning Disabilities, 12, 396-401. for the learning disabled. In M. Pressley & JR. (Vol. 4, pp. 225-272). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Swanson, H.L. (1986). Do semantic memory Torgesen, J.K., Rashotte, CA., Greenstein, J, Houck, Levin (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research: PsyG, & Portes, P. (in press). Academic difficulties of chological foundations (pp. 129-153). New York: deficiencies underlie learning disabled readers' learning disabled children who perform poorly on Springer-Verlag. encoding process? Journal ofExperimental Child memory span tasks. In H.L. Swanson (Ed.), Young A., & Ellis, A. (1981). Asymmetry of cerebral Psychology, 41, 461-488. Memory and learning disabilities. Greenwich, hemisphericfunction in normal and poor readers. Swanson, H.L (in press). Verbal-coding deficits in CT: JAI Press. the recall of pictorial information by learning disPsychological Bulletin, 89, 183-190. abled children: The influence of a lexical system Wagner, R.K., & Sternberg R.J (1984). Alternative Ysseldyke, J.E., Thurlow, M.L., Graden, J.L., conceptions of intelligence and their implications for input operations. American Educational Wesson, C, Deno, S.L., & Algozzine, B. (1983). for education. Review of Educational Research, Research Journal Generalizations from five years of research on 54, 179-221. assessment and decision making. Exceptional Swanson, H.L, Kosleski, E., & Stegink, P. (inprepaEducation Quarterly, 4, 75-93. ration). Disabled readers' processing ofprose: Do Witelson, S. (1977). Developmental dyslexia: Two REACTION TO KOLLIGIAN AND STERNBERG'S TRIARCHIC SYNTHESIS A pplication of Sternberg's triarchic theory to the LD field provides a useful and timely illustration of the value of such conceptual activity (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1987). And while the specific implications for "specific learning disabilities" are not new, some are so widely ignored throughout the field that another statement of them is always welcome. This is not to say that the theory itself or this particular application is completely satisfactory. The presentation's importance is as a stimulus for discussion, focused not on the conclusions drawn but on the usefulness of the theory itself for understanding learning disabilities. In this specific connection, the following brief reactions are meant to balance the authors' stress on the strengths of this theoretical application by underscoring a few major deficiencies. First, it is likely that Kolligian and Sternberg would agree that efforts to understand the nature of specific learning disabilities require a broader theoretical base than can be provided by a theory of human intelligence (Adelman & Taylor, 1983; Deci & Chandler, 1986). Sternberg's theory certainly goes beyond other cognitive theories that have been applied in the LD field in the way he includes environmental and motivational factors. However, as with other applications of cognitive theory, Kolligian and Sternberg pay too little systematic attention to the role of affect and ongoing reciprocal transactions (e.g., see Neisser, 1976). Second, since the discussion is based on the authors' definition of learning disabilities, one would as- 166 sume that the theoretical application is specific to those individuals who fit that definition, namely those having "localized impairments of cognitive functioning." However, this assumption is not supported by the indiscriminate way the term learning disabilities is used throughout the presentation. The matter is confounded further because certain key concepts are not well defined and differentiated from each other; for example, the discussion fails to clarify whether impairment, the key term in the authors' definition of LD, is comparable to deficit, a key term used in the triarchic theory. Additional confusion about this matter results from failure to distinguish carefully among terms used to indicate problems and pathology, for example, disorders, disabilities, impairments, deficits. Whatever the reason, the fact is that the term specific learning disabilities is used indiscriminately to describe inJournal of Learning Disabilities Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at UNIV ARIZONA LIBRARY on May 25, 2015