Literature Review Prosocial Behavior and Altruism in Economic Exchanges Behavioral Economics Submitted to Sir Nadeem Sarwar Group Members: Asba Imran (21868) Ayesha Muntaha (22606) Ayman Ansari (22570) Maida Rafiq (22539) Rida Faraz (22590) Introduction The examination of prosocial behavior and altruism in economic exchanges provides an intriguing insight into how human behaviors go beyond self-interest, influencing economic and social connections. Prosocial conduct, as defined in literature, refers to voluntary activities undertaken to assist others, even if it entails personal expenses, so deviating from what is prescribed by economic logic. Prosocial conduct comprises a wide range of behaviors, including immediate assistance in situations, planned acts of assistance, voluntary work, and collaboration within and across organizations (Caserta et al., 2023). The theory of rational economic man, and particularly the assumption of self-interest in economics, is said to be at odds with altruistic and cooperative actions. Donations to charitable causes, voluntary contributions to public goods, and inclinations for justice seem to contradict economic concepts such as greed and the pursuit of self-interest. Prosocial and economic activity, nonetheless, may coexist. Historical development of Prosocial behavior The conventional economic concepts and theories have been challenged in the context of prosocial behavior. It has happened on the basis of self-interest. The reasons why individuals are engaged in activities that benefit others even at their personal loss are beyond self-interest. They include outcome- based prosocial preferences, self-identity, and reciprocity. These theories account for the underlying human behavior in economic exchanges which is beyond self-interest (Boston, 2006). The notion of kin altruism is built on the idea of inclusive fit via action. Getting genes passed down from one generation to another is the main emphasis. The evolutionary advantage of inclusive fitness accruing to individuals that consistently assist their relatives is a result of this behavior (Costantini et al., 2019). The evolutionary benefit of assisting others is attempted to be explained by the idea of reciprocal altruism, which takes a more individualistic perspective. Helping others and expecting reciprocation has evolutionary benefits for humans. Additionally, because some standards of reciprocity are present in every culture, reciprocal altruism might be seen as a manifestation of genetic development. Direct and indirect reciprocity are two ways of looking at this idea. The former depicts cooperation that develops from frequent interactions between the same two people, whereas the latter describes a process where we assist one person, who then chooses to assist another third person, or where we are assisted by another person, and then decide to assist still another third person (Costantini et al., 2019). Seminal Studies in Prosocial Behavior The work of Rosenbaum et al. (2012) on the Ultimatum and Revelation Game is one of the key seminal studies. Understanding prosocial behavior, especially in economic circumstances, relies heavily on this work. Fairness and reciprocity play an important part in economic choices, as shown by the fact that players in the Ultimatum Game generally reject unjust offers, even if it means harming themselves. This has shaken up the field of economics by challenging the longheld belief in completely selfish actors and has far-reaching consequences for our understanding of generosity and equity in trade. Psychological Mechanisms Underlying Altruism Altruism, a component of prosocial behavior, is conceptualized as benefiting others with no expectations of benefits to oneself in Psychology (Lay & Hoppmann, 2015). A study ( Feigin, Owens, & Goodyear-Smith, 2014) conducting through review of scientific theories and articles to gather and summarize research on human altruism in social psychology from 1960 to 2014, highlighted how altruism is defined in various lights over the years. Some argue that in defining altruism, it is essential to separate it from its opposite, egoism. They posit that altruism and egoism are distinct motivational states, differing in their direction of goal-directed motivation because altruism aims to increase another's welfare, while egoism is self-directed. They also underline that despite their potential distinction, egoism and altruism can coexist. On the contrary, others argue that altruism is ultimately driven by selfish motives, seeking one's own well-being. In a different dimension, altruism can also be explained as Normative or autonomous. While Normative altruism consists of everyday acts of kindness driven by the expectation of social rewards or fear of punishments, in contrast, autonomous altruism is not influenced by these factors and may even be considered heroic (Franco, Blau, & Zimbardo, 2011). Altruism is commonly related to concepts such as altruistic punishment, reward, reciprocity, and cooperation. Altruistic punishment acts as a powerful tool to maintain cooperation in society as individuals who violate a social norm are often punished by a third party. A study (Fehr, Fischbacher, & Gachter, 2002) provides significant evidence using a public goods game which suggests that many individuals tend to cooperate willingly, especially when treated fairly, and are inclined to punish those who do not cooperate or treat others unfairly. This behavioral tendency, termed as "strong reciprocity", is shown through empirical evidence to foster universal cooperation in situations where purely self-interested actions would otherwise lead to a breakdown of cooperation. Differences in opinions exist between social psychologists who consider empathy as key motivator behind altruistic behavior and economists who assume that fairness principles drive altruism. In Economic settings, Studies using behavioral paradigms have shown that individuals often act more generously than what rational choice theory would suggest (Filkowski, Cochran, & Haas, 2016). Much of this research used straightforward experiments, with the Dictator Game, Ultimatum game and Trust games. The findings from these studies support the widely embraced notion that altruism is influenced significantly by social norms linked to fairness, which involves seeking equal material benefits for both oneself and others (Klimecki, Mayer, Jusyte, Scheeff, & Schonenberg, 2016). Empathy which includes experiencing compassion and a willingness to alleviate someone else's pain, is also considered a driver of Altruistic behavior ( Feigin, Owens, & Goodyear-Smith, 2014). (Batson C. D., 1992) suggests a psychological explanation for altruistic behavior, centered around empathic concern. According to his empathy-altruism hypothesis, when someone encounters another in need, the potential helper experiences personal distress. This distress can drive the person to seek relief either by aiding the sufferer or by avoiding the situation, both stemming from an egoistic motivation. Contrastingly, Batson also proposes that the potential helper may empathize with the sufferer and choose to help, even when escape is an easier option. In such cases, Batson argues that the helper is altruistically motivated by a genuine desire to alleviate the other person's suffering, rather than being driven by their own distress. Experimental Studies on Prosocial Behavior The study ‘Self-Signaling and Prosocial Behavior: A Cause Marketing Experiment’ examined how people behave when they are offered a charitable donation with a product purchase, testing if ego-related feelings affect their choices more than the charitable act itself. They conducted two experiments where consumers could buy a movie ticket with either a discount, a charitable donation, or both. Surprisingly, when large donations were offered, discounts led to fewer purchases. People reported feeling less altruistic when given bigger discounts with significant donations. This suggests that personal feelings about being altruistic influenced choices more than the act of giving itself. Moreover, the experiment contradicts the idea that larger donations always attract more purchases and highlights the potential downside of combining discounts with charitable donations in marketing campaigns. In the first experiment, different SMS messages were sent to about 10,500 smartphone users, offering discounts, donations, or a combination of both for movie tickets. They observed that out of the 10,500 subscribers who received these messages, 273 ended up purchasing movie tickets. The second experiment, conducted, involved around 30,300 smartphones out of which 694 people bought movie tickets. Additionally, a survey was conducted the day after the SMS offers expired, reaching out to both purchasers and non-purchasers to gather feedback on their experiences. They found that when discounts and charitable donations are combined in promotions, it sometimes leads to fewer people buying the product. Furthermore, when discounts are too big, they overshadow the desire to support charity, affecting people's decisions to purchase. People seem to feel better about themselves when they support a cause rather than the actual act of giving. Discounts and donations don't always work well together, sometimes, discounts can cancel out the positive effect of a donation. However, the study has limitations as it didn't look at long-term effects on sales, and it's uncertain if these findings apply to different types of products. They also didn't explore if feeling good about supporting a cause now leads to more helpful behavior in the future or if people prefer not to be put in situations where they have to show support (Dubé et al., 2017). The study “Prosocial behaviors and economic performance: Evidence from an online mental healthcare platform” investigated the impact of health professionals offering free services on online mental healthcare platforms on their future economic performance. It aimed to explore whether such behaviors affect earnings directly and whether relationship and reputation capital mediate this effect. Data was taken from a Chinese online mental healthcare platform called yidianling.com, where verified counselors offer services to users seeking psychological advice. These counselors provide paid consultations and, sometimes, free advice on relationship problems or work stress. Data from 659 active counselors over 16 weeks was analyzed, observing their behavior in providing free suggestions, the number of paid consultations, thanks received, and new followers gained weekly. The results indicate that these free services result in a 5% increase in counselors' paid consultations. Additionally, the positive impact is partly influenced by the relationships counselors build with users but is not significantly affected by reputation. This means that providing free services directly helps counselors earn more and builds better relationships with users, benefiting their economic performance. In essence, the study reveals that counselors' pro-social behavior directly contributes to their financial success on the online mental healthcare platform, partially due to the relationships they establish with users. Yet, study has limitations. It doesn't consider the quality of free services or generalize its findings beyond active counselors (Yan et al., 2022). The study “Market Interaction and Pro-Social Behavior: An Experimental Study “investigated how market interactions affect people's choices when dividing money between themselves and another person, considering situations where negative consequences affect others. In their experiments, participants have the chance to either buy or sell the right to make decisions about dividing money between themselves and a passive recipient. They aimed to see what happens when this decision-making right is traded in a market-like setting compared to when it's given without market interaction. 11 sessions involving 24 participants each at Florida State University were conducted. They divided participants into buyers, sellers, and recipients. In the Market treatment, participants engaged in a market where sellers could sell the right to decide how to split money, and buyers could buy this right. Then, they played a game where they allocated money between themselves and a passive recipient. In the Random Market treatment, transactions had a 50% chance of being canceled after the market stage. The Control treatment didn't involve market interaction and replicated outcomes from previous Market sessions for comparison. During the experiment, sellers could sell their right to decide how to split the money, and buyers could buy this right. In the second stage, buyers who bought this right and sellers who didn't sell it had to split $16 between themselves and the recipient. The outcomes from the Market sessions were used to guide decisions in the Control sessions. When this decision-making role was traded in a market setting, the overall amount given to others decreased significantly compared to situations where the assignment was direct. There were two possibilities for this decrease: Firstly, participating in a market might have changed individuals' behavior, making them less inclined to be generous. Secondly, the market might have allotted decision-making to individuals less concerned about others' well-being. Reduction in pro-social behavior in the market setting couldn't be solely explained by the selection process. Even when some transactions were canceled, the negative impact on pro-social behavior persisted. Surprisingly, buyers who appeared more generous tended to buy from sellers who were also inclined to be more generous, yet overall, both buyers and sellers were less generous in the market setting compared to other scenarios. This suggests that markets, both directly and through their selection processes, might diminish people's willingness to consider others' well-being when making decisions about sharing resources. The adverse impact of market interactions on altruistic behavior persisted even when participants were conscious of the consequences of their actions. This implies that there could be better outcomes if people are more aware of their behavior in the markets (Collins et al., 2017). In one of the cases, when health professionals exhibited pro social behavior like offering free services online, it benefitted the economy. In another case, mixing discounts with charitable donations in marketing or engaging in market-like situations didn’t always bring the expected positive outcomes. For example, in marketing scenarios, both buyers and sellers exhibited less generosity. This hints at a possible decrease in overall kindness compared to the direct interactions. This suggests that while prosocial behavior can be beneficial in certain economic settings, its impact might vary and might not always result in predictable outcomes across different situations within economic transactions. Applications in Business and Organizations Prosocial behavior comprises actions that are performed by an individual, or a group, with the main goal to benefit others. All types of prosocial behavior have positive outcomes and the categories they focus on can be political, social, environmental, and so on. Any specific person does not necessarily perform prosocial behavior, it can also be done by those perceived to be evil by society, e.g. thieves, and not just altruistic individuals, or those who are selfless. Altruistic behavior consists of “self-sacrifice” which is the personal cost you incur for the benefit of others, whereas prosocial behavior simply means doing good, as it is expected by society. Research states that a pro-social personality sets the tone for the culture, social norms, and values and the inherent characteristics include being optimistic, extending help, and embodying the essence of empathy, which results in feeling good and maximizing satisfaction as people see the rewards of their good actions. (Hazzi) Prosocial benefits are enjoyed by everyone in society. The entrepreneur, e.g., will inculcate business practices such that his prosocial personality is reflected in the customers as well. This is because prosocial values will transfer throughout society, and eventually, they will be reflected in society, building a community where everyone helps each other. A prosocial entrepreneur may provide services that connect people, build relationships, and contribute to a better society. E.g., a business model working towards reducing litter and pollution in the city. (Hindokova) Ng and Van Dyne, (NG, 2005) conducted a study that prosocial behavior results in better performance and overall betterment of the company. However, there is a dark side to this attitude too. Continuously putting oneself before others results in exhaustion, which might impact performance again. Bolino, (Grant, 2016). The bright side of being prosocial at work, and the dark side, too: a review and agenda for research on other-oriented motives, behavior, and impact in organizations. (Luthans & Youssef, 2007) research conducted which suggests that particularly focusing on positivity often results in an imbalance of attitude impacting one’s mental health, positive aspects of prosocial behavior and benefits are often exaggerated. Therefore, prosocial behavior needs to be carried out in balanced measures, considering both the positive and negative impacts. One type of prosocial behavior is social responsibility. The actions taken to bring about positive impacts come under prosocial behavior, whereas the motivation behind these actions is called altruism. The act of organizations being moral towards environmental, economic, and cultural aspects is known as social responsibility. Each stakeholder performs per the expectations set by society, within the capacity of their profession. Due to social responsibility, organizations have a lasting positive impact, hence, it helps governments, companies, and individuals as well. By nature, socially responsible actions are sustainable and have no external costs. (Aziz & Abid, 2018), suggest that an individual needs to be emotionally influenced to conduct themselves. Public Policy and Altruism Altruism has been defined in many ways, but the most consistent definition is improving the welfare of others without having the desire to get anything in return. According to the standard economics model, all rational agents try to maximize their gains and thus exhibit self-interest maximization behavior, eventually leading to an efficient allocation of resources. However, many studies have found that different factors play a dominant role in acting for the public good (Batson D. , 1994) and one of them is altruism. (Gates & Steane, 2009) argue that this notion of self-interest has led to financial crisis, wealth inequality, and extreme capitalization. The authors propose altruism as an alternative approach to policymaking as it would maximize the interests of the society rather than a selected few people. They also lay down a few examples of policies that can be formed using an altruistic approach – Environmental, social welfare, and fair labor policies. Similarly, a study (Zubair, Khan, & Mukaram, 2021) examines the effects of altruism along with other factors on organizational performance. The results obtained from Covariance structural equation modeling show that altruism indirectly leads to higher levels of organizational performance among public sector officials as their motivation to serve the public is fueled by altruism. Prosocial behaviors are those which benefit others or society as a whole. (Costa-Font & Machado, 2021) answers the question of whether prosocial behaviors such as – organ donation, blood donation, health care volunteering, etc. are motivated by altruism or not. We find that altruism indeed motivates pro-social behaviors and thus governments can use them as tools during policy interventions. Simplifying tax forms and creating a community of blood donors are provided as examples to illustrate how policies can be designed to encourage prosocial behavior for societal benefit (Aknin & Whillans, 2021). There is also a stress on the need to balance ethical considerations based on altruistic principles. In the context of leveraging prosocial behavior, especially altruism, for societal benefit, the study (Chell & Mortimer, 2014) investigates the strategies employed by NGOs to encourage blood donation. There is a shift towards a customer-centered approach, and it is found that altruistic values play a crucial role in motivating individuals to donate blood. Besides this other factors such as status and emotional fulfillment are also at play. Recent studies examine whether policies imposed during the COVID-19 pandemic leveraged pro-social behaviors. (Campos-Mercade, Meier, Schneider, & Wengström, 2021) come up with an important finding that the impact of policies depends upon the level of pro-sociality in a population. Different surveys involving an incentivized game (where participants could expose others to risk for their benefit) were conducted which showed that most people are unwilling to harm others for their benefit and thus policies can be designed per the degree of pro-sociality in the population. However, many ethical dilemmas arise when implementing policies based on altruistic principles. Studies have been conducted to address these challenges and ethical considerations. According to (Long, 2021), the empirical estimation of an adjustment to VSL poses a challenge. The results show that VSL measures are underestimated and thus the economic value assigned to prevent a statistical fatality is considerably lower than it would have been if altruistic elements were considered. Therefore, in the case of public health, policymakers must consider altruistic sentiments. Placing a monetary value on human life is regarded as distasteful and this adds complexity to policies based on altruistic principles. Policymakers must weigh economic considerations against ethical principles especially when making decisions for the public’s wellbeing. Conclusion In sum, our knowledge of how people behave in economic settings has been enhanced by research into altruism and prosocial conduct in trades. To reconcile economic rationality with the innate human propensity for altruism, seminal studies have shown that equity and reciprocity are fundamental to economic decision-making. This area has shed light on the intricacies of human motives. Therefore, challenging the conventional knowledge of economically motivated individuals and introducing theories of kinship and reciprocal altruism as well as outcome-based prosocial preferences. This new knowledge challenges long-held assumptions about economics by highlighting the compatibility of altruism with profit. Additionally, there has been substantial focus on healthcare sectors in the past literature exploring altruism. Even though altruism in the workplace has been explored to some extent, there is a need to tap into other sectors to explore the varying levels of altruism and design policies accordingly. References Feigin, S., Owens, G., & Goodyear-Smith, F. (2014). Theories of human altruism: a systematic review. Annals of Neuroscience and Psychology. doi:https://doi.org/10.7243/2055-34471-5 Aknin, L. B., & Whillans, A. V. (2021). Helping and Happiness: A Review and Guide for Public Policy. Social Issues and Policy Review. Aziz, M., & Abid, M. (2018). Social Responsibility: As a Predictor of Altruistic. Psychology and Behavioural Science. Batson, C. D. (1992). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Choice Reviews Online, 29 (08), 29–4797. doi:https://doi.org/10.5860/choice.29-4797 Batson, D. (1994). Why Act for the Public Good? Four Answers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Campos-Mercade, P., Meier, A. N., Schneider, F. H., & Wengström, E. (2021). Prosociality predicts health behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Public Economics. Chell, K., & Mortimer, G. (2014). Investigating online recognition for blooddonor retention: an experiential donorvalue approach. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing. Costa-Font, J., & Machado, S. (2021). How Can Policy Interventions Encourage Pro-Social Behaviours in the Health System? LSE Public Policy Review. Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Gachter, S. (2002). Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature 13, 1–25. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-002-1012-7 Filkowski, M. M., Cochran, R. N., & Haas, B. W. (2016). Altruistic behavior: mapping responses in the brain. Neuroscience and Neuroeconomics, 65-75. Franco, Z., Blau, K., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2011). Heroism: A Conceptual Analysis and Differentiation Between Heroic Action and Altruism. Review of General Psychology, 99113. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022672 Gantt, E. E., Reber, J. S., & Hyde, J. D. (2013). The psychology of altruism and the problems of mechanism, egoism, and. Research Gate. Gates, D., & Steane, P. (2009). Altruism – an alternative value in policy formation and decision making. International Journal of Social Economics, 962-978. Grant, M. C. (2016). The Bright Side of Being Prosocial at Work, and the Dark Side, Too: A Review and Agenda for Research on Other-Oriented Motives, Behavior, and Impact in Organizations. The Academy of Management Annals. Hazzi, O. A. (n.d.). Prosocial Organizational Behaviors: The Lifeline of Organizations. Hindokova, D. (n.d.). Applied pro-social behaviour of companies – social marketing. Klimecki, O. M., Mayer, S. V., Jusyte, A., Scheeff, J., & Schonenberg, M. (2016). Empathy promotes altruistic behavior in economic interactions. Scientific Reports 6. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/srep31961 Lay, J. C., & Hoppmann, C. (2015). Altruism and Prosocial Behavior. Springer eBooks , (1–9). doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-287-080-3_69-1 Long, M. C. (2021). Altruism and the Statistical Value of Human Life for Policy. Springer Nature. Luthans, F., & Youssef, C. M. (2007). Emerging Positive Organizational Behavior. Journal of Management. NG, V. D. (2005). Antecedents and performance consequences of helping behavior in work groups: a multilevel analysis. Saksa, J. (2015). An Investigation of Research on Altruism in Recent Literature of the Three Sectors: Public, Private, and Non-Prof. Zubair, S. S., Khan, M. A., & Mukaram, A. T. (2021). Public service motivation and organizational performance: Catalyzing effects of altruism, perceived social impact and political support. National Library of Medicine. Caserta, M., Distefano, R., Ferrante, L., & Reito, F. (2023). The Good of Rules: A pilot study on prosocial behavior. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 107, 102085. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2023.102085 Costantini, A., Scalco, A., Sartori, R., & Ceschi, A. (2019). Theories for computing prosocial behavior. ResearchGate. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/332173009_Theories_for_Computing_Prosocia l_Behavior Rosenbaum, S. M., Billinger, S., Stieglitz, N., Djumanov, A., & Atykhanov, Y. (2012). Market economies and pro-social behavior: Experimental evidence from Central Asia. Journal of Socioeconomics, 41(1), 64–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2011.10.010 Boston, F. R. B. O. (2006, March 14). A survey of economic theories and field evidence on ProSocial Behavior. Federal Reserve Bank of Boston. https://www.bostonfed.org/publications/research-department-working-paper/2006/asurvey-of-economic-theories-and-field-evidence-on-pro-social-behavior.aspx Dubé, J., Luo, X., & Fang, Z. (2017). Self-Signaling and Prosocial Behavior: A cause Marketing experiment. Marketing Science, 36(2), 161–186. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2016.1012 Yan, Z., Kuang, L., & Qiu, L. (2022). Prosocial behaviors and economic performance: Evidence from an online mental healthcare platform. Production and Operations Management, 31(10), 3859–3876. https://doi.org/10.1111/poms.13792 Collins, S., Hamman, J., & Lightle, J. P. (2017). Market Interaction and Pro‐Social Behavior: An Experimental study. Southern Economic Journal, 84(3), 692–715. https://doi.org/10.1002/soej.12238