Journal of Personality Disorders, 34, Special Issue, 6–24, 2020 © 2020 The Guilford Press NARCISSISTIC AND BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDERS: RELATIONSHIP WITH OXIDATIVE STRESS Royce J. Lee, MD, David Gozal, MD, MBA, Emil F. Coccaro, MD, and Jennifer Fanning, PhD The authors hypothesized that personality disorders characterized by interpersonal hypersensitivity would be associated with an elevated concentration of 8-hydroxy-2'-deoxyguanosine (8-OH-DG), the oxidized form of guanine, and a biomarker of oxidative stress burden. One hundred ninety-five male and female adults underwent semistructured diagnostic interviews, completed questionnaire measures of social cognition and emotional attribution, and had blood drawn for determination of plasma 8-OH-DG. A hierarchical linear regression model revealed that narcissistic and borderline personality disorders predicted 8-OH-DG level independently of the effects of age, gender, recent alcohol and cigarette use, current major depression, and posttraumatic stress disorder. In all subjects, 8-OH-DG level was also correlated with the number of borderline personality disorder symptoms present. Narcissistic and borderline personality disorders predicted oxidative stress burden independently of potentially confounding factors. Keywords: borderline personality disorder, narcissistic personality disorder, oxidative stress, 8-OH-DG, stress, emotion, disgust, personality disorder Personality disorders remain a challenging group of disorders to treat. There are no FDA-approved treatments for these disorders, in part due to a lack of understanding of their underlying biological mechanism. Progress has been made using scientific approaches such as high-resolution brain imaging (Kimmel et al., 2016), neuroendocrinology (Lee et al., 2012), and measures of neurotransmitter function (Coccaro et al., 1989). The results of this work generally point to altered neurophysiological indices of stress reactivity in personality disorder as related to transdiagnostic dimensions such as affective instability, trauma exposure, and aggression. From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, The University of Chicago (R. J. L., E. F. C.); the Department of Child Health, University of Missouri School of Medicine, Columbia, Missouri (D. G.); and the Center for Depression, Anxiety, and Stress, Harvard Medical Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts (J. F.). Address correspondence to Royce J. Lee, MD, The University of Chicago, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5841 S. Maryland Ave., MC 3077, Chicago, IL 60637. E-mail: rlee@yoda.bsd. uchicago.edu 02_G4784_471.indd 6 3/11/2020 1:56:45 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 7 With respect to categorical diagnostic constructs, neurobiological research to date has been primarily focused on borderline personality disorder (BPD). Narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) has received less attention. One consequence of this is that psychological models of narcissism are not yet integrated with neurobiological models. This is likely of some importance, because phenotypic controversies have led to lack of consensus on nosology (Ronningstam, 2013). As an important example, neuroimaging research has provided unexpected evidence of hyperreactivity in highly narcissistic individuals of the social pain network (Cascio, Konrath, & Falk, 2013; Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia, & Neubauer, 2017) and the motor mimicry network (Marcoux et al., 2014) during social interaction tasks. These findings raise important questions about the relationship between NPD and near-neighbor conditions such as antisocial and borderline personality disorders. NPD appears to be situated between these disorders, sharing on the one hand features of callousness with antisocial personality disorder (Gunderson & Ronningstam, 2001; Wilson, Stroud, & Durbin, 2017) and, on the other, showing excessive social reactivity, or interpersonal hypersensitivity, as has been proposed to underlie BPD (Gunderson & Lyons-Ruth, 2008; Ronningstam, 2013). Interpersonal hypersensitivity is a conceptual model of borderline psychopathology that posits that a biological vulnerability in the child disrupts early attachment to caregivers, whose negative responses are internalized by the child and carried into adulthood as a habitually aversive social interaction style. Supportive evidence of the construct in BPD is broad, although the construct is complex and there is not consensus on how it is best measured. For example, individuals with BPD experience more anger and emptiness in response to social interactions when compared to individuals with other personality disorders (Stepp, Pilkonis, Yaggi, Morse, & Feske, 2009). The outward grandiosity of narcissism seems at first glance to be inconsistent with the interpersonal hypersensitivity construct, but there is evidence that it may apply. Suicide attempts by individuals with NPD have been found to be preceded by recent relationship problems and being fired from a job (Blasco-Fontecilla et al., 2010). Interactions of grandiosity and vulnerability predict rejection sensitivity in individuals with NPD (Roche, Pincus, Conroy, Hyde, & Ram, 2013). While grandiose narcissism and vulnerable narcissism have been found to differ, as expected, on dimensions of agency and domination, both subtypes have been found to be associated with lower levels of warmth and communion in their interpersonal relationships, pointing to the kinds of attachment problems that are at the core of the interpersonal hypersensitivity construct (J. D. Miller, Price, Gentile,Lynam, & Campbell, 2012). Neurobiological research may help to shed some light on these issues by revealing commonalities and differences between personality disorder subtypes. One understudied area of research in personality disorder is oxidative stress, the production and accumulation of reactive oxygen species (ROS) in the brain and body through metabolic processes. Although interconnected with what is generally considered to be the stress response system centered on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, oxidative stress is a largely cellular 02_G4784_471.indd 7 3/11/2020 1:56:45 PM 8 LEE ET AL. process (Schiavone, Jaquet, Trabace, & Krause, 2013). Social stressors relevant to personality disorder, such as maternal deprivation, social defeat, and social deprivation, have been found to increase oxidative stress (Markovic et al., 2017; Schiavone et al., 2009; Solanki, Salvi, Patki, & Salim, 2017). We have previously found that impulsive aggression and the presence of personality disorder are both associated with increased oxidative stress, as evidenced by elevated blood levels of 8-hydroxy-2'-deoxyguanosine (8-OH-DG; Coccaro, Lee, & Gozal, 2016). 8-OH-DG is the oxidized form of the nucleoside guanine, derived from mitochondrial and nuclear DNA (Evans, Olinski, Loft, & Cooke, 2010). As measured in peripheral blood, its concentration reflects the balance between the production of oxidative free radicals (H202) and their removal by intrinsic antioxidant activity. Oxidized guanine is mutagenic: It causes transcription errors, leading to carcinogenesis (Hayakawa et al., 1999), and is thus of interest on its own as a risk factor for cancer. Thus, elevated concentration of 8-OH-DG is a biomarker of endogenous oxidative stress load (Valavandis, Vlachogianni, & Fiotakis, 2009). Whether specific personality disorders are associated with oxidative stress is an important question. Given a previous literature linking social stress and oxidative stress, we hypothesized that in a sample of adults with and without a range of personality disorders recruited for transdiagnostic research studies from the community, personality disorders previously associated with interpersonal hypersensitivity (narcissistic and borderline) would be associated with higher oxidative stress. To explore the relationship between oxidative stress and interpersonal hypersensitivity, we assessed the relationship between oxidative stress and two vignette-based instruments measuring hostility and emotional reactivity. METHODS SUBJECTS All research procedures were approved by the institutional review board (IRB) of the Biological Sciences Division at The University of Chicago. Subjects provided written, informed consent using IRB-approved consent forms. Research volunteers came from the general population of the Chicago metropolitan area using media advertisements recruiting for transdiagnostic research studies that included personality disordered, depressed, anxious, and healthy adults. The sample comprised 68 normal controls, 29 psychiatric controls, and 98 personality disordered subjects (N = 195). Psychiatric controls met diagnostic criteria for one or more current or past psychiatric disorders but did not have a personality disorder. The psychiatric control group included 2 subjects with a current intermittent explosive disorder, 1 with an impulse control disorder, 15 with an anxiety disorder, 2 with current depression, 2 with a current adjustment disorder, 1 with an eating disorder, 2 with a past alcohol (EtOH) disorder, and 16 with a past anxiety disorder. Age and gender distributions across the three groups of subjects are described in Table 1. Subjects with a personality disorder met criteria for 1–6 disorders, with a mean of 1.38 disorders. 02_G4784_471.indd 8 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 02_G4784_471.indd 9 25 12.3) 28 (13.8) ObsessiveCompulsive PD NOS 12 (42.9) 14 (56) 2 (66.7) 27 (62.8) 7 (31.8) — — 11 (55) 34.68 (7.082) 34.56 (62.5) 26.0 (7.0) 34.80 (7.311) 36.33 (8.643) 34.95 (8.759) — — 34.5 (9.058) 35.76 (6.26) 32 (9.08) 12.1954 (3.915) 12.550 (4.690) 11.885 (4.795) 15.940 (7.312) 15.19 16.091 (4.488) 14.537 (5.966) 14.95 (4.507) — — 13.398 (3.626) 10.826 (4.45) 10.874 (4.801) Mean Age, years 8-OH-DG (SD) M(SD) PD NOS: personality disorder not otherwise specified. 16 (7.9) 15 (7.4) Narcissistic Avoidant 0 43 (21.2) Borderline 1 (0.5) 22 (10.8) Antisocial 3 (1.5) 0 Schizotypal Histrionic 0 Schizoid Dependent 3 (20) 20 (9.9) Paranoid 19 (65.5) 29 Psychiatric Controls 35 (52.2) 68 Female, n (%) Normal Controls n (%) 4.609 (2.205) 4.816 (2.317) 5.79 (2.709) 6.167 (3.753) 11.5 5.679 (2.577) 5.924 (2.173) 6.222 (2.34) — — 6.750 (2.277) 4.111 (2.105) 3.046 (1.85) Hostile Attribution M(SD) 8.391 (1.576) 8.143 (2.032) 8.615 (1.192) 9.0 (1.0) 7.0 8.357 (1.392) 8.543 (2.020) 8.611 (1.754) — — 8.800 (1.373) 8.370 (1.305) 7.71 (1.863) Instrumental Attribution M(SD) 6.304 (2.363) 4.952 (2.479) 5.154 (2.577) 5.667 (4.933) 3.0 5.643 (2.590) 5.086 (2.769) 5.611 (3.146) — — 5.400 (3.112) 6.222 (2.621) 6.804 (2.385) Benign Attribution M(SD) TABLE 1. Descriptive Information 7.667 (2.259) 8.143 (1.852) 9.307 (2.175) 8.333 (3.215) 9.0 7.429 (2.681) 9.086 (2.020) 9.5 (1.654) — — 10.200 (2.277) 6.889 (2.512) 6.02 (2.154) Angry Affect M(SD) 6.875 (2.401) 6.905 (2.644) 9.154 (2.076) 9.0 (2.646) 12.0 6.857 (2.825) 8.428 (2.367) 7.944 (2.531) — — 8.600 (2.230) 6.519 (2.751) 5.50 (2.71) Embarrassed Affect M(SD) 98.818 (9.333) 97.632 (9.051) 100.80 (8.417) 115 86.0 89.167 (24.033) 96.273 (12.028) 92.333 (11.873) — — 90.0 (22.511) 96.519 (12.014) 98.863 (15.29) Socially Appropriate Response M(SD) 52.546 (11.775) 52.444 (14.694) 56.4 (13.385) 37 — 54 (14.588) 62.0 (17.208) 71.0 (17.094) — — 60.375 (15.315) 47.926 (12.615) 44.824 (13.502) Aggressive Response M(SD) 55.455 (14.654) 54.611 (12.33) 60.5 (12.103) 55 — 54.636 (12.484) 65.524 (14.299) 70.455 (13.344) — — 60.250 (14.330) 53.519 (12.148) 47.294 (14.775) Relationally Aggressive Response M(SD) NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 9 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 10 LEE ET AL. DIAGNOSTIC PROCEDURE Axis I and II diagnoses were made using DSM-IV-TR criteria (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000) based on information gathered during semistructured interviews (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM [SCID; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997] and Structured Interview for the Diagnosis of DSM Personality Disorder [SID-P; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1997]). SCID and SID-P interviews were conducted by clinicians with a master’s or doctoral degree in clinical psychology. Clinical raters underwent training and verification of reliability by random, video-recorded interview and confirmation of reliability. Final diagnosis was made by a consensus procedure in which all available clinical data were compiled in a report and reviewed by a committee that included the research psychiatrist and the clinical raters (Coccaro, Nayyer, & McCloskey, 2012). ELIGIBILITY CRITERIA Eligibility criteria excluded subjects with current medical illness, fever, recent injury, clinical signs of inflammation on physical examination, and positive urine drug screen and/or alcohol breathalyzer test. All subjects were between the ages of 18 and 55. Subjects with a history of psychotic disorder (schizophrenia, bipolar mania with psychosis) and acute suicidality were excluded. 8-OH-DG ASSAY In subjects meeting eligibility criteria, whole blood was obtained by phlebotomy, anticoagulated with EDTA, centrifuged, and stored at −80°C until batch assay. The 8-OH-DG level was measured in duplicate by a commercially available immunoassay (Cayman Chemical; Ann Arbor, MI; detection lower limit = 30 pg/mL; coefficients of variation < 9.2%). Reported measures represented the mean of duplicate assay results. INTERPERSONAL HYPERSENSITIVITY There are not yet questionnaire measures specific to the interpersonal hypersensitivity construct in personality disorder, which includes elements of disorganized attachment, aggression, and affective disturbance (Bureau, Easlerbrooks, & Lyons-Ruth, 2009; Lyons-Ruth, Melnick, Patrick, & Hobson, 2007). Exploratory analyses were undertaken to do preliminary work in this area. Two instruments were selected that assess social behavior by eliciting the direct response of the subject to social cues. The Social Information Processing Questionnaire (SIP-Q; Coccaro, Noblett, & McCloskey, 2009; see also Coccaro, Fanning, Keedy, & Lee, 2016) is a self-report questionnaire that assesses attributional style, emotional response, and response selection in response to a series of eight vignettes depicting interpersonal conflict in which the intent of the antagonist is ambiguous. The first four vignettes depict acts of relational aggression such as social exclusion, while the second four vignettes depict physical aggression. In response to each vignette, subjects select 02_G4784_471.indd 10 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 11 an attribution of hostile intent (the antagonist intends to do harm to the relationship or body), instrumental intent (you are in the way of another goal), or benign intent (the antagonist makes an understandable mistake). The subject then indicates an emotional response (anger or embarrassment) and identifies his or her own likely response in the situation: aggressive response (striking back), relationally aggressive (e.g., silent treatment), or socially appropriate (making an overture). Consistent with social information processing theory of reactive aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1996), the SIP-Q shows convergent validity with measures of interpersonal hostility and childhood trauma and divergent validity with personality measures such as the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS; Coccaro et al., 2009). The Emotional Attribution Questionnaire (EAQ), modified from previous work in patients with brain injuries and impaired social behavior (Blair & Cipolotti, 2000), is a series of 68 brief vignettes describing a variety of social and nonsocial events in which the subject is asked to identify the emotional reaction of the vignette’s protagonist. For the example, subjects are asked to assign an emotional response (angry, happy, fear, disgust, sad) that “Tania” feels in response to the statement, “Tania was told that if she painted the woman’s face, she would receive $100. But after the job was done, the woman only gave her $50.” Responses are scored as correct or incorrect. Scores are calculated for each depicted mood state. Lower scores indicate poorer recognition of the particular affect depicted. Performance on the task has been found to be impaired in subjects with acquired abnormalities in sympathetic nervous tone, which is thought to interfere with “bottom-up” detection of affective states (Heims, Critchley, Dolan, Mathias, & Cipolotti, 2004) and in a sociopathic patient with lesions to the orbitofrontal cortex (Blair & Cipolotti, 2000). STATISTICAL ANALYSIS Personality disorder diagnoses (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000) were assessed as predictors of 8-OH-DG using hierarchical linear regression analysis. Because levels of oxidative stress have been previously associated with age (Maehira, Nakama, Ohmine, & Miyagi, 2004), male sex, smoking (Black, Bot, Scheffer, & Penninx, 2016), drinking (Caimi, Carollo, & Lo Presti, 2003; Götz et al., 2001; Micallef, Lexis, & Lewandowski, 2007), major depressive disorder (Black, Bot, Scheffer, Cuijpers, & Penninx, 2015), and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (M. W. Miller & Sadeh, 2014), these were included in the model. Of note, 22 subjects met criteria for a current depressive episode, and 20 subjects met criteria for current PTSD. In the second step, personality disorder diagnosis was entered if the sample size was more than 10 subjects per diagnosis, coded as a 0 (absent) or 1 (present). For patients who met criteria for more than one personality disorder diagnosis, each diagnosis was accounted for in the model. To assess dimensional relationships between personality disorder symptoms and 8-OH-DG, a separate hierarchical linear regression model was assessed entering the number of personality disorder criteria from the SID-P endorsed by the subject for each personality disorder category in the second step, in place of the categorical diagnoses. Dimensional relationships between 02_G4784_471.indd 11 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 12 LEE ET AL. 8-OH-DG and interpersonal hypersensitivity were tested using hierarchical linear regression models. RESULTS 8-OH-DG values were inspected by histogram, which showed a near-normal distribution (mean = 12.242 pg/mL, SD = 5.020) but with a single, univariate outlier with an 8-OH-DG level of 34.63 pg/mL, greater than 4 standard deviations above the mean. The Kolmogarov–Smirnov (KS) test confirmed nonnormality (KS = .070, df = 196, p = .02). The single outlier was a patient with BPD and no other personality disorder. Exclusion of the outlier reduced the KS test to nonsignificance. Although the value is biologically plausible and in line with the hypothesized relationship between BPD and oxidative stress, all reported analyses excluded the univariate outlier. Exclusion of this subject did not significantly impact the linear regression model. The hierarchical linear regression model for 8-OH-DG based on personality disorder diagnosis found that NPD and BPD predicted variance in this oxidative stress measure, even when accounting for other important sources. The first model included factors previously associated with oxidative stress burden, including age, gender, EtOH consumption, smoking, current major depression, and current PTSD, F(6, 195) = 5.780, p < .001 (Table 2). Male gender and recent alcohol use were associated with 8-OH-DG. The personality disorders with n > 10 included borderline (n = 43), narcissistic (n = 15), antisocial (n = 22), paranoid (n = 20), avoidant (n = 16), obsessive-compulsive (OCPD; n = 25), and personality disorder not otherwise specified (NOS) (n = 28). Addition of the personality disorder diagnoses resulted in an overall model that predicted variance in 8-OH-DG, F(13, 195) = 4.339 p < .001, with a significant F-change statistic, F(8, 181) = 2.779, p = .009, compared to the model without personality disorder diagnoses. Of the seven personality disorders, only BPD (β = 3.439, p = .001; R2 = .054) and NPD (β = 3.731 p = .006; R2 = .049) significantly predicted 8-OH-DG after accounting for potential confounding variables. Both BPD and NPD remain significant predictors after Bonferroni correction for multiple measures (p = .021 and p = .042, respectively; see Figure 1). The plot of predicted × actual 8-OH-DG values (Figure 2) confirmed normality and linearity. The plot of predicted values × residuals was unbiased and homoscedastic (Figure 3). Variation inflation factor (VIF) values for the personality disorder predictors ranged from 1.117 (OCPD) to 1.715 (antisocial personality disorder), ruling out problematic levels of multicollinearity in the predictors. Results of the hierarchical linear regression model for 8-OH-DG based on the count of personality disorder criteria/symptoms in the entire sample found that adding the count of personality disorder symptoms for borderline, narcissistic, paranoid, antisocial, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder resulted in a significant model as well as a significant F-change statistic (see Table 3). The effects were not due to age, sex, major depressive 02_G4784_471.indd 12 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 02_G4784_471.indd 13 .000 .994 −.343 −.031 .072 −.019 −.048 −.013 −.002 .198 .294 .095 .010 −.098 .233 −.280 .049 Standardized Beta .958 1.269 1.310 1.018 1.347 1.339 .984 1.157 1.089 .794 .020 .679 .41 SE 1.038 −.270 −.589 −.192 −.023 2.768 3.495 1.415 .146 −1.438 3.387 −4.007 .706 t .300 .787 −.589 .848 .992 .006 .001 .159 .884 < .152 .001 < .001 .132 Sig. 1.140 1.225 1.598 1.172 1.763 1.208 1.684 1.080 1.127 1.030 1.059 1.095 1.065 VIF Note. Sex (male = 1, female = 2), current major depression (No = 0, Yes = 1), current PTSD (No = 0, Yes = 1), borderline and other personality disorders (No = 0, Yes = 1). MDD = major depressive disorder; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; OCPD: obsessive-compulsive personality disorder; PD NOS = personality disorder not otherwise specified; Sig. = significance; VIF = variation inflation factor. Bold text indicates statistically significant associations. PD NOS −.772 .083 −.019 Paranoid Avoidant −.195 .197 3.731 .028 3.439 1.523 .038 Antisocial .009 OCPD 2.779 .234 .182 .159 −1.142 .068 .146 −.050 .268 .029 −2.720 .223 .486 < .001 β Borderline < .001 7.780 Sig. F Zero-Order Change Correlation Narcissistic Model 2 PTSD MDD Smoking Drinks/week 4.339 (13,194) .128 F Change .132 .394 Adjusted R Square −.276 < .001 R Age 5.780 (5,194) Sig. Sex Model 1 F TABLE 2. Hierarchical Linear Regression Model of 8-OH-DG Based on Personality Disorder Diagnosis NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 13 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 14 LEE ET AL. FIGURE 1. 08-OH-DG level by diagnostic group. FIGURE 2. Expected vs. observed probabilities for 8-OH-DG. 02_G4784_471.indd 14 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 15 FIGURE 3. Scatterplot of residual vs. predicted values for 8-OH-DG. disorder (MDD), EtOH consumption, and recent smoking. Only the count of BPD symptoms emerged as a significant predictor of 8-OH-DG after accounting for age, sex, EtOH consumption, recent smoking history, MDD, and PTSD (β = .727, p = .001) (see Figure 4). Exploratory analyses were conducted to examine the relationship between 8-OH-DG levels and dimensional measures of social cognition. For the SIP-Q, a model predicting 8-OH-DG that included hostile attribution, benign attribution, instrumental attribution, anger response, and embarrassed response did not result in a statistically significant change in F value when added to gender, age, recent EtOH consumption, smoking, current depression, and PTSD. A second linear regression analysis that included response selection as predictors of 8-OH-DG produced a significant model, F(8, 142) = 2.787, p = .005, and F-change statistic, F(3, 134) = 2.744, p = .046. Of the three responses, only a relationally aggressive response predicted 8-OH-DG at a statistically significant level (β = .113, p = .02), but the finding did not survive Bonferroni correction. For the EAQ, the hierarchical linear regression model including the five emotional attributions resulted in a significant model, F(11, 130) = 3.685, p < .001, accounting for variance in 8-OH-DG level above and beyond gender, age, recent EtOH consumption, smoking, current depression, and PTSD, F change (5, 119) = 4.313, p = .001. Correct attribution of sadness (β = .750, p = .029) and fear (β = 2.263, p = .020) was positively correlated with 8-OH-DG, but neither survived Bonferroni correction. Correct attribution of disgust was negative correlated with 8-OH-DG (β = −1.730, p = .001), 02_G4784_471.indd 15 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 02_G4784_471.indd 16 .223 .057 #Paranoid #Avoidant −.333 .100 .115 −.134 −.035 .030 .035 .326 .150 .382 −.083 .010 −.099 .234 −.277 .048 Standardized Beta .257 .348 .257 .326 .244 .206 .771 1.092 .796 .020 .682 .041 SE .445 ..288 .445 −1.302 1.791 3.549 −1.236 .134 −1.449 3.388 −3.952 .691 t .657 .773 .657 .194 .124 <.001 .218 .894 .149 .001 <.001 .491 Sig. 1.498 2.720 1.498 2.615 1.728 2.852 1.168 1.126 1.029 1.059 1.064 1.095 VIF Note. Sex (male = 1, female = 2), current major depression (No = 0, Yes = 1), current PTSD (No = 0, Yes = 1), #Borderline = number of criteria met. MDD = major depressive disorder; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; OCPD: obsessive-compulsive personality disorder; Sig. = significance; VIF = variation inflation factor. Bold text indicates statistical significance. .154 #OCPD .436 .730 −.952 .036 −.424 < .001 #Antisocial 5.015 .146 −1.153 .069 −2.694 .028 β .028 .301 .211 .268 −.052 .312 .506 < .001 Zero Order Correlation #Narcissistic < .001 5.689 Sig. F Change #Borderline Model 2 PTSD MDD Smoking Drinks/week 5.714 .127 F Change .130 .392 Adjusted R Square −.273 < .001 R Sex 5.689 Sig. Age Model 1 F TABLE 3. Hierarchical Linear Regression Model of 8-OH-DG Based on Personality Disorder Severity 16 LEE ET AL. 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 17 FIGURE 4. Scatterplot of 8-OH-DG and borderline personality disorder severity. surviving Bonferroni correction (see Table 4). The VIF statistic for disgust was 1.380, indicating acceptable levels of collinearity. DISCUSSION We report that levels of the oxidative stress marker 8-OH-DG are associated with the diagnosis of NPD and BPD after controlling for age, gender, smoking, alcohol use, depression, and PTSD. These findings support the hypothesis that personality disorders characterized by interpersonal hypersensitivity are associated with increased oxidative stress. The lack of association of 8-OHDG with paranoid and antisocial personality disorder does not support the alternative hypothesis that personality disorders characterized by hostility are associated with oxidative stress, although statistical power to rule out was Type II error is lacking due to the relatively small sample sizes of individual personality disorder diagnoses. Consistent with a dimensional model of BPD, there is a positive correlation between 8-OH-DG and the number of BPD criteria met, but not with narcissistic criteria. This is the first such report identifying a relationship between oxidative stress and narcissistic and borderline personality disorders. If replicated, the findings would provide a new insight into the neurobiology of two disorders that remain incompletely understood. Such findings would also suggest a close biological relationship between them. Oxidative stress is an important pathophysiological component of many medical illnesses (Betteridge, 2000) and has been investigated predominantly in this context. Although there has 02_G4784_471.indd 17 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 02_G4784_471.indd 18 .750 .094 Happy .308 −1.730 ..035 −.307 .139 .215 .190 .369 −.214 .079 Standardized Beta .729 .523 .272 .962 .340 .048 .050 .027 SE .423 −3.306 1.672 2.353 2.208 2.349 −1.315 .928 t .673 .001 .097 .020 .029 .020 .191 .355 Sig. Note. Sig. = significance; VIF = variation inflation factor; SIP-Q = Social Information Processing Questionnaire; EAQ = Emotional Attribution Questionnaire. Bold text indicates statistically significant associations. .154 −.235 Anger Disgust .274 .001 2.263 4.313 .029 .254 .115 .504 Sad < .001 .113 −.067 .025 β Fear EAQ 3.685 .046 Zero Order Correlation .193 2.744 Sig. F Change Relationally Aggressive Response .102 F Change .111 .398 Adjusted R Square Overtly Aggressive Response .005 R .016 2.787 Sig. Socially Appropriate Response SIP-Q Response Selection F TABLE 4. Hierarchical Linear Regression Model of 8-OH-DG Based on Interpersonal Hypersensitivity 1.066 1.380 1.101 1.335 1.335 3.901 4.170 1.136 VIF 18 LEE ET AL. 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 19 been little research in the area of oxidative stress and personality disorder, these findings can be understood in the context of a larger body of research connecting psychological states and biological processes. The findings reinforce previous findings of health problems in personality disordered populations. NPD has previously been associated with increased mortality due to cardiovascular disease, even after controlling for relevant medical comorbidities (Quirk et al., 2015; Samuels, 2011). NPD is also associated with gastrointestinal conditions (Quirk et al., 2015) and generally high health care utilization (Jackson & Burgess, 2002; Samuels, 2011). BPD has previously been associated with nearly twice the rate of metabolic syndrome (Kahl et al., 2013) and elevated rates of chronic pain, obesity, arthritis (Powers & Oltmanns, 2013), hypertension, arteriosclerosis, and gastrointestinal disease (El-Gabalawy, Katz, & Sareen, 2010). Oxidative stress is implicated in all of these chronic conditions. Oxidative stress markers have been investigated in association with other psychiatric conditions. Although no association with depression was found in this study, previous research has found elevated plasma and urinary 8-OHDG in depression (Maes, Galecki, Chang, & Berk, 2011). Negative studies have also been reported (Jorgensen et al., 2013). Data from human studies have linked elevated markers of oxidative stress with life stressors such as bereavement (Aschbacher et al., 2013), academic stress (Cohen, Marshall, Cheng, Agarwal, & Wei, 2000; Gidron, Russ, Tissarchondou, & Warner, 2006), caregiving burden, suicide attempts (Vargas et al., 2013), and psychological stress (Lesgards et al., 2002). There is some evidence that cortisol, released by stress, increases the production of oxidative species (Aschbacher et al., 2013; Simsek, Yüksel, Kaplan, Uysal, & Aktas, 2016). Basic science research in animal models of social stress confirms that aversive social contexts induce oxidative stress (Schiavone et al., 2013; Solanki et al., 2017). The cross-sectional design of this study does not permit causal inferences to be made regarding increased oxidative stress and borderline and narcissistic personality disorders. Nonetheless, a plausible explanation is that neurophysiological activity drives oxidative stress in persons with a hypersensitive neural response to social stress. Although sources of oxidative stress outside the brain cannot be ruled out, brain metabolism accounts for 20% of the body’s oxygen (O2) budget, out of proportion to its mass. This is because the principal sources of oxidative species are neural activity itself (Halliwell, 1992), intracellular energy cycling (Cobley, Fiorello, & Bailey, 2018), nitrosamine signaling between neurons (Finnell & Wood, 2018), and catabolism of monoamine neuromodulators such as serotonin and norepinephrine by the mitochondrial enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A; Kolla et al., 2016). Increased neural signaling during periods of psychological stress would thus be expected to increase the oxidation burden. Interestingly, elevated MAO-A activity is increased in the prefrontal cortex of individuals with BPD (Kolla et al., 2016). Upregulation of MAO-A activity would be expected to generate oxidative species through the oxidation of monoamine neurotransmitters and could increase circulating 8-OH-DG levels. Unfortunately, no measures of MAO-A activity were taken in this study, and this idea remains speculative. It is also possible that the direction of causation is reverse: that oxidative stress itself drives psychopathology. 02_G4784_471.indd 19 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 20 LEE ET AL. A recent epidemiological study found that lifetime exposure to air pollution was associated with elevated risk for personality disorder in the Danish population, accounting for important cofounders such as urbanicity (Khan et al., 2019). In an animal model, induction of phosphodiesterase-2, which reduces oxidative stress, remarkably had the effect of decreasing anxiety-related behaviors (Masood, Nadeem, Mustafa, & O’Donnell, 2008). These connections are fascinating but clearly remain speculative without additional research. Exploratory analyses revealed a possible inverse relationship between 8-OH-DG and attributions of disgust. Persons with higher levels of oxidative stress were less likely to attribute disgust to the protagonist described in this sample vignette: Cathy is eating her dinner when her cat throws up all over the carpet next to her. Because no a priori hypothesis predicted this finding, replication is needed. If replicated, this finding could shed light on a previous literature linking the emotion of disgust and NPD. Narcissism has been associated with alexithymia to all emotions (Fossati, Somma, Pincus, Borroni, & Dowgwillo, 2017). However, specific links have been made between narcissism and specific difficulties with identifying and attributing disgust in others (Marissen, Deen, & Franken, 2012) and self-states of shame (Poless, Torstveit, Lugo, Andreassen, & Sütterlin, 2018). Disgust and shame are two closely associated, socially reciprocal emotional states (Giner-Sorolla & Espinosa, 2010). Thus, the finding of disgust avoidance may be understood in light of the theoretical and empirical literature linking narcissism with shame. It has been hypothesized that narcissistic individuals, in defense against hypersensitivity to shame, have an avoidant response to it (Ronningstam & Baskin-Sommers, 2013). Empirical research has confirmed that avoidance of shame is associated with narcissistic personality traits (Poless et al., 2018). Interestingly, induction of shame has been found to increase inflammatory cytokine markers (Dickerson, Kemeny, Aziz, Kim, & Fahey, 2004). BPD has been found to be associated with elevated endorsement of shame (Peters & Geiger, 2016; Winter, Bohus, & Lis, 2017). Exactly how attributional deficits in disgust are linked to oxidative stress remains speculative without further study. Several limitations should be mentioned. It is possible that the findings of a specific relationship between oxidative stress and narcissistic and borderline personality disorders are in fact driven by issues of statistical power due to low sample sizes. Although collinearity was not estimated to be problematic in the statistical model, correlation between personality disorder predictors could also reduce the ability to detect associations that were present. Some personality disorders were not assessed at all in the model (schizoid, schizotypal, dependent), but should be included in future studies. Assay sensitivity may have reduced power to detect subtle differences that were present. Alternative approaches include measuring the ratio of 8-OH-DG to the unhydroxylated base 2' deoxyguanosine (2-DG) (Bolner, Pilleri, de Riva, & Nordera, 2011). A fully powered follow-up study is needed to rule out Type I and Type II errors, and thus conclusions of specificity in the relationship between oxidative stress and personality disorder subtypes would be premature. The cross-sectional study design precludes causal inferences. If replicated, the results point to the need for longitudinal research to establish the temporal sequence of oxidative stress burden and personality development. Due to the exploratory nature of 02_G4784_471.indd 20 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS 21 analyses related to the interpersonal hypersensitivity construct, formal testing of a mediation model regarding the role of oxidative stress in the relationship between disgust/shame and narcissistic personality disorder was not conducted. This is an important topic for future research. Finally, it is unknown if the findings reported here reflect the relationship between a domain general stress reactivity and borderline and narcissistic personality disorders, given interrelationships between oxidative stress, hypothalamic-pituitary axis function, and inflammation. In summary, preliminary evidence is reported of an association of 8-OHDG, a biomarker of oxidative stress burden, with narcissistic and borderline personality disorders. The fact that both NPD and BPD were associated with 8-OH-DG suggests that the two disorders have a biological relationship. The findings also have implications for dimensional models of psychopathology. Interpersonal hypersensitivity is a promising theoretical construct that may be a useful approach to biological studies of personality disorder. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Aschbacher, K., O’Donovan, A., Wolkowitz, O. M., Dhabhar, F. S., Su, Y., & Epel, E. (2013). Good stress, bad stress and oxidative stress: Insights from anticipatory cortisol reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 1698–1708. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .psyneuen.2013.02.004 Betteridge, D. J. (2000). What is oxidative stress? Metabolism: Clinical and Experimental, 49(2), 3–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S0026-0495(00)80077-3 Black, C. N., Bot, M., Scheffer, P. G., Cuijpers, P., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2015). Is depression associated with increased oxidative stress? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 164–175. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.025 Black, C. N., Bot, M., Scheffer, P. G., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2016). Sociodemographic and lifestyle determinants of plasma oxidative stress markers 8-OHdG and F2-isoprostanes and associations with metabolic syndrome. Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity, 2016, 7530820. https://doi .org/10.1155/2016/7530820 Blair, R. J. R., & Cipolotti, L. (2000). Impaired social response reversal. Brain, 123, 1122–1141. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.6.1122 Blasco-Fontecilla, H., Baca-Garcia, E., Duberstein, P., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Dervic, K., Saiz-Ruiz, J., . . . Oquendo, M. A. (2010). An exploratory study of the relationship between diverse life events and specific personality disorders in a sample of suicide 02_G4784_471.indd 21 attempters. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24, 773–784. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi .2010.24.6.773 Bolner, A., Pilleri, M., de Riva, V., & Nordera, G. (2011). Plasma and urinary HPLC ED determination of the ratio of 8-OHdG/2-dG in Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Laboratory, 57, 859–866. Bureau, J. F., Easlerbrooks, M. A., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2009). Attachment disorganization and controlling behavior in middle childhood: Maternal and child precursors and correlates. Attachment & Human Development, 11, 265–284. https://doi.org/10.1080 /14616730902814788 Caimi, G., Carollo, C., & Lo Presti, R. (2003). Diabetes mellitus: Oxidative stress and wine. Current Medical Research and Opinion, 19, 581–586. https://doi.org/10.1185 /030079903125002324 Cascio, C. N., Konrath, S. H., & Falk, E. B. (2013). Narcissists’ social pain seen only in the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/ nsu072 Cobley, J. N., Fiorello, M. L., & Bailey, D. M. (2018). 13 reasons why the brain is susceptible to oxidative stress. Redox Biology, 15, 490–503. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.redox.2018.01.008 Coccaro, E. F., Fanning, J. R., Keedy, S. K., & Lee, R. J. (2016). Social cognition in intermittent explosive disorder and aggression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 83, 140–150. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.07.010 Coccaro, E. F., Lee, R., & Gozal, D. (2016). Elevated plasma oxidative stress markers in individuals with intermittent explosive disorder and 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 22 correlation with aggression in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 127–135. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.014 Coccaro, E. F., Nayyer, H., & McCloskey, M. S. (2012). Personality disorder-not otherwise specified evidence of validity and consideration for DSM-5. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53, 907–914. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .comppsych.2012.03.007 Coccaro, E. F., Noblett, K. L., & McCloskey, M. S. (2009). Attributional and emotional responses to socially ambiguous cues: Validation of a new assessment of social/emotional information processing in healthy adults and impulsive aggressive patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 915–925. https://doi .org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.01.012 Coccaro, E. F., Siever, L. J., Klar, H. M., Maurer, G., Cochrane, K., Cooper, T. B., . . . Davis, K. L. (1989). Serotonergic studies in patients with affective and personality disorders. Correlates with suicidal and impulsive aggressive behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 587–599. https://doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.1989.01810070013002 Cohen, L., Marshall, G. D., Cheng, L., Agarwal, S. K., & Wei, Q. (2000). DNA repair capacity in healthy medical students during and after exam stress. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 23, 531–544. https://doi.org/10.1023/A :1005503502992 Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002. https://doi.org/10.1111/j .1467-8624.1996.tb01778.x Dickerson, S. S., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., Kim, K. H., & Fahey, J. L. (2004). Immunological effects of induced shame and guilt. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 124–131. https://doi .org/10.1097/01.psy.0000097338.75454.29 El-Gabalawy, R., Katz, L. Y., & Sareen, J. (2010). Comorbidity and associated severity of borderline personality disorder and physical health conditions in a nationally representative sample. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72, 641–647. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY .0b013e3181e10c7b Evans, M. D., Olinski, R., Loft, S., & Cooke, M. S. (2010). Toward consensus in the analysis of urinary 8-oxo-7,8-dihydro-2’-deoxyguanosine as a noninvasive biomarker of oxidative stress. FASEB Journal, 24, 1249–1260. https://doi.org/10.1096/fj.09-147124 Finnell, J. E., & Wood, S. K. (2018). Putative inflammatory sensitive mechanisms underlying risk or resilience to social stress. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 240. https:// doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00240 First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician 02_G4784_471.indd 22 LEE ET AL. Version (SCID-I-CV) for DSMIV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Fossati, A., Somma, A., Pincus, A., Borroni, S., & Dowgwillo, E. A. (2017). Differentiating community dwellers at risk for pathological narcissism from community dwellers at risk for psychopathy using measures of emotion recognition and subjective emotional activation. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31, 325–345. https://doi.org/10.1521/ pedi_2016_30_255 Gidron, Y., Russ, K., Tissarchondou, H., & Warner, J. (2006). The relation between psychological factors and DNA-damage: A critical review. Biological Psychology, 72, 291–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005 .11.011 Giner-Sorolla, R., & Espinosa, P. (2010). Social cuing of guilt by anger and of shame by disgust. Psychological Science, 22, 49–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610392925 Götz, M. E., Janetzky, B., Pohli, S., Gottschalk, A., Gsell, W., Tatschner, T., . . . Böning, J. (2001). Chronic alcohol consumption and cerebral indices of oxidative stress: Is there a link? Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 25, 717–725. https://doi .org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2001.tb02272.x Gunderson, J. G., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2008). BPD’s interpersonal hypersensitivity phenotype: A gene-environment-developmental model. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22, 22–41. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2008.22.1.22 Gunderson, J. G., & Ronningstam, E. (2001). Differentiating narcissistic and antisocial personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 103–109. https://doi.org/10 .1521/pedi.15.2.103.19213 Halliwell, B. (1992). Reactive oxygen species and the central nervous system. Journal of Neurochemistry, 59, 1609–1623. https://doi.org/10 .1111/j.1471-4159.1992.tb10990.x Hayakawa, H., Hofer, A., Thelander, L., Kitajima, S., Cai, Y., Oshiro, S., . . . Sekiguchi, M. (1999). Metabolic fate of oxidized guanine ribonucleotides in mammalian cells. Biochemistry, 23, 3610–3614. https://doi.org/10.1021/ bi982361l Heims, H. C., Critchley, H. D., Dolan, R., Mathias, C. J., & Cipolotti, L. (2004). Social and motivational functioning is not critically dependent on feedback of autonomic responses: Neuropsychological evidence from patients with pure autonomic failure. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1979–1988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .neuropsychologia.2004.06.001 Jackson, H. J., & Burgess, P. M. (2002). Personality disorders in the community: Results from the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. Part II. Relationships between personality disorders, Axis I mental disorders and physical conditions with disability 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM NPD, BPD, AND OXIDATIVE STRESS and health consultation. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37, 251–260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s001270200017 Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Koschutnig, K., Kedia, G., & Neubauer, A. C. (2017). Self-viewing is associated with negative affect rather than reward in highly narcissistic men: An fMRI study. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 5804. https:// doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-03935-y Jorgensen, A., Krogh, J., Miskowiak, K., Bolwig, T. G., Kessing, L. V, Fink-Jensen, A., . . . Jorgensen, M. B. (2013). Systemic oxidatively generated DNA/RNA damage in clinical depression: Associations to symptom severity and response to electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of Affective Disorders, 149, 355–362. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10 .1016/j.jad.2013.02.011 Kahl, K. G., Greggersen, W., Schweiger, U., Cordes, J., Correll, C. U., Frieling, H., . . . Moebus, S. (2013). Prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in patients with borderline personality disorder: Results from a cross-sectional study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, 205–213. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s00406-012-0339-2 Khan, A., Plana-Ripoll, O., Antonsen, S., Brandt, J., Geels, C., Landecker, H., . . . Rzhetsky, A. (2019). Environmental pollution is associated with increased risk of psychiatric disorders in the US and Denmark. PLoS Biology, 17(8), e3000353. https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pbio.3000353 Kimmel, C. L., Alhassoon, O. M., Wollman, S. C., Stern, M. J., Perez-Figueroa, A., Hall, M. G., . . . Radua, J. (2016). Age-related parietooccipital and other gray matter changes in borderline personality disorder: A metaanalysis of cortical and subcortical structures. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 251, 15–25. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10 .1016/j.pscychresns.2016.04.005 Kolla, N. J., Chiuccariello, L., Wilson, A. A., Houle, S., Links, P., Bagby, R. M., . . . Meyer, J. H. (2016). Elevated monoamine oxidaseA distribution volume in borderline personality disorder is associated with severity across mood symptoms, suicidality, and cognition. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11 .024 Lee, R. J., Hempel, J., TenHarmsel, A., Liu, T., Mathé, A. A., & Klock, A. (2012). The neuroendocrinology of childhood trauma in personality disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 78–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .psyneuen.2011.05.006 Lesgards, J. F., Durand, P., Lassarre, M., Stocker, P., Lesgards, G., Lanteaume, A., . . . LehucherMichel, M. P. (2002). Assessment of lifestyle effects on the overall antioxidant capacity of healthy subjects. Environmental Health 02_G4784_471.indd 23 23 Perspectives, 110, 479–486. https://doi. org/10 .1289/ehp.02110479 Lyons-Ruth, K., Melnick, S., Patrick, M., & Hobson, R. P. (2007). A controlled study of hostilehelpless states of mind among borderline and dysthymic women. Attachment & Human Development, 9, 1–16. https://doi.org/10 .1080/14616730601151417 Maehira, F., Nakama, Y., Ohmine, N., & Miyagi, I. (2004). Age-related changes in the oxidation–reduction characteristics and the 8-OHdG accumulation in liver, lung, brain of SAMP1 and SAM R1. International Congress Series, 1260, 259–262. https://doi .org/10.1016/S0531-5131(03)01698-4 Maes, M., Galecki, P., Chang, Y. S., & Berk, M. (2011). A review on the oxidative and nitrosative stress (O&NS) pathways in major depression and their possible contribution to the (neuro)degenerative processes in that illness. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, 676–692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp .2010.05.004 Marcoux, L. A., Michon, P. E., Lemelin, S., Voisin, J. A., Vachon-Presseau, E., & Jackson, P. L. (2014). Feeling but not caring: Empathic alteration in narcissistic men with high psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 224, 341–348. https://doi.org/10 .1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.002 Marissen, M. A. E., Deen, M. L., & Franken, I. H. A. (2012). Disturbed emotion recognition in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 198, 269–273. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.042 Markovic, B., Radonjic, N. V, Jevtic, G., Stojkovic, T., Velimirovic, M., Aksic, M., . . . Petronijevic, N. D. (2017). Long-term effects of maternal deprivation on redox regulation in rat brain: Involvement of NADPH oxidase. Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity, 2017, 7390516. https://doi.org/10 .1155/2017/7390516 Masood, A., Nadeem, A., Mustafa, S. J., & O’Donnell, J. M. (2008). Reversal of oxidative stress-induced anxiety by inhibition of phosphodiesterase-2 in mice. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 326, 369–379. https://doi.org/10.1124/ jpet.108.137208 Micallef, M., Lexis, L., & Lewandowski, P. (2007). Red wine consumption increases antioxidant status and decreases oxidative stress in the circulation of both young and old humans. Nutrition Journal, 6, 27. https:// doi.org/10.1186/1475-2891-6-27 Miller, J. D., Price, J., Gentile, B., Lynam, D. R., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism from the perspective of the interpersonal circumplex. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 507–512. 3/11/2020 1:56:46 PM 24 https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid .2012.04.026 Miller, M. W., & Sadeh, N. (2014). Traumatic stress, oxidative stress and post-traumatic stress disorder: Neurodegeneration and the accelerated-aging hypothesis. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, 1156–1162. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp .2014.111 Peters, J. R., & Geiger, P. J. (2016). Borderline personality disorder and self-conscious affect: Too much shame but not enough guilt? Personality Disorders, 7, 303–308. https://doi .org/10.1037/per0000176 Pfohl, B., Blum, N., & Zimmerman, M. (1997). Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorder: SIDP-IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Poless, P. G., Torstveit, L., Lugo, R. G., Andreassen, M., & Sütterlin, S. (2018). Guilt and proneness to shame: Unethical behaviour in vulnerable and grandiose narcissism. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 14(1), 28–43. https:// doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v14i1.1355 Powers, A. D., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2013). Borderline personality pathology and chronic health problems in later adulthood: The mediating role of obesity. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4, 152–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028709 Quirk, S. E., El-Gabalawy, R., Brennan, S. L., Bolton, J. M., Sareen, J., Berk, M., . . . Williams, L. J. (2015). Personality disorders and physical comorbidities in adults from the United States: Data from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50, 807–820. https://doi .org/10.1007/s00127-014-0974-1 Roche, M. J., Pincus, A. L., Conroy, D. E., Hyde, A. L., & Ram, N. (2013). Pathological narcissism and interpersonal behavior in daily life. Personality Disorders, 4, 315–323. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030798 Ronningstam, E. (2013). An update on narcissistic personality disorder. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26(1), 102–106. Ronningstam, E., & Baskin-Sommers, A. R. (2013). Fear and decision-making in narcissistic personality disorder—A link between psychoanalysis and neuroscience. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15, 191–201. https:// doi.org/10.1038/jid.2015.269 Samuels, J. (2011). Personality disorders: Epidemiology and public health issues. International Review of Psychiatry, 23, 223–33. https:// doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2011.588200 Schiavone, S., Jaquet, V., Trabace, L., & Krause, K.-H. (2013). Severe life stress and oxidative 02_G4784_471.indd 24 LEE ET AL. stress in the brain: From animal models to human pathology. Antioxidants & Redox Signaling, 18, 1475–1490. https://doi. org/10.1089/ars.2012.4720 Schiavone, S., Sorce, S., Dubois-Dauphin, M., Jaquet, V., Colaianna, M., Zotti, M., . . . Krause, K.-H. (2009). Involvement of NOX2 in the development of behavioral and pathologic alterations in isolated rats. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 384–392. https:// doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych .2009.04.033 Sims ek, S., Yüksel, T., Kaplan, İ., Uysal, C., & Aktas , H. (2016). The levels of cortisol and oxidative stress and DNA damage in child and adolescent victims of sexual abuse with or without post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Investigation, 13, 616–621. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jechem.2016.12.004 Solanki, N., Salvi, A., Patki, G., & Salim, S. (2017). Modulating oxidative stress relieves stressinduced behavioral and cognitive impairments in rats. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 550–561. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyx017 Stepp, S. D., Pilkonis, P. A., Yaggi, K. E., Morse, J. Q., & Feske, U. (2009). Interpersonal and emotional experiences of social interactions in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 197, 484–491. https://doi.org/10.1097/ NMD.0b013e3181aad2e7 Valavandis, A., Vlachogianni, T., & Fiotakis, C. (2009). 8-hydroxy-2'-deoxyguanosine (8-OHdG): A critical biomarker of oxidative stress and carcinogenesis. Journal of Environmental Science and Health Part C: Environmental Carcinogenesis and Ecotoxicology Reviews, 27, 120–139. Vargas, H. O., Nunes, S. O. V., De Castro, M. P., Bortolasci, C. C., Barbosa, D. S., Morimoto, H. K., . . . Berk, M. (2013). Oxidative stress and lowered total antioxidant status are associated with a history of suicide attempts. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 923–930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.016 Wilson, S., Stroud, C. B., & Durbin, C. E. (2017). Interpersonal dysfunction in personality disorders: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 677–734. https://doi.org/10 .1037/bul0000101 Winter, D., Bohus, M., & Lis, S. (2017). Understanding negative self-evaluations in borderline personality disorder—A review of self-related cognitions, emotions, and motives. Current Psychiatry Reports, 19(3), 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920 -017-0771-0 3/11/2020 1:56:47 PM