doi:10.1093/brain/awv286 BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 | 3793 Minds on replay: musical hallucinations and their relationship to neurological disease Erin C. Golden and Keith A. Josephs The phenomenon of musical hallucinations, in which individuals perceive music in the absence of an external auditory stimulus, has been described sparingly in the literature through small case reports and series. Musical hallucinations have been linked to multiple associated conditions, including psychiatric and neurologic disease, brain lesions, drug effect, and hearing impairment. This study aimed to review the demographics of subjects with musical hallucinations and to determine the prevalence of neurological disorders, particularly neurodegenerative disease. Through the Mayo medical record, 393 subjects with musical hallucinations were identified and divided into five categories based on comorbid conditions that have been associated with musical hallucinations: neurological, psychiatric, structural, drug effect and not otherwise classifiable. Variables, including hearing impairment and the presence of visual and other auditory hallucinations, were evaluated independently in all five groups. The mean age at onset of the hallucinations was 56 years, ranging from 18 to 98 years, and 65.4% of the subjects were female. Neurological disease and focal brain lesions were found in 25% and 9% of the total subjects, respectively. Sixty-five subjects were identified with a neurodegenerative disorder, with the Lewy body disorders being the most common. Visual hallucinations were more common in the group with neurological disease compared to the psychiatric, structural, and not otherwise classifiable groups (P 5 0.001), whereas auditory hallucinations were more common in the psychiatric group compared to all other groups (P 5 0.001). Structural lesions associated with musical hallucinations involved both hemispheres with a preference towards the left, and all but two included the temporal lobe. Hearing impairment was common, particularly in the not otherwise classifiable category where 67.2% had documented hearing impairment, more than in any other group (P 5 0.001). Those with an underlying neurodegenerative disorder or isolated hearing impairment tended to hear more persistent music, which was often religious and patriotic compared to those with a structural lesion, where more modern music was heard, and those with psychiatric disorders where music was mood-congruent. This case series shows that musical hallucinations can occur in association with a wide variety of conditions, of which neurological disease and brain lesions represent a substantial proportion, and that Lewy body disorders are the most commonly associated neurodegenerative diseases. A future prospective study would be helpful to further delineate an association between musical hallucinations and neurodegenerative disease. Department of Neurology, Division of Behavioural Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota, USA Correspondence to: Keith A. Josephs, MD, MST, MSc Professor of Neurology Mayo Clinic 200 1st Street S.W. Rochester, MN, 55905, USA E-mail: Josephs.keith@mayo.edu Keywords: music; psychosis; dementia; depression; Lewy body disease Abbreviation: NOC = not otherwise classifiable Received May 14, 2015. Revised July 27, 2015. Accepted August 9, 2015. Advance Access publication October 7, 2015 ß The Author (2015). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 3794 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 Introduction ‘Music, when soft voices die; Vibrates in the memory’ mused English poet Percy Bysshe Shelley (Shelley, 1901). The relationship between music and memory is particularly highlighted by the curious phenomenon of musical hallucinations (Wieser, 2003; Sacks, 2006). An auditory hallucination, or the perception of sound in the absence of an acoustic stimulus, is not an uncommon phenomenon. Auditory hallucinations usually consist of unformed sounds, such as those heard in tinnitus; however, formed or complex hallucinations are experienced less frequently. Musical hallucinations represent a subset of complex auditory hallucinations in which individuals perceive music in the form of tunes or melodies, which can be continuous or intermittent, in the absence of a corresponding external stimulus. Sometimes, singing voices may accompany the music (Berrios, 1990; Warner and Aziz, 2005). The hallucinations can be transient or long-lasting. Often occurring in cognitively and psychiatrically normal individuals, they are likely under-reported due to concern for being perceived as ‘crazy’. In addition, it may be that the music is not bothersome or disruptive to daily functioning and so does not prompt a discussion with a medical provider. Often, the tunes are familiar to the person perceiving them, but there are reports of novel or unfamiliar tunes (Berrios, 1990; Warner and Aziz, 2005). Even when the melody is unrecognizable to the subject, it may represent a familiar melody that is no longer consciously remembered by the subject (Vitorovic and Biller, 2013), a phenomenon that has prompted important questions as to whether our memory for music is distinct from other memories. Musical hallucinations are an uncommon phenomenon. In one series of general psychiatric admissions, the prevalence rate was 0.16% (Fukunishi et al., 1998). In another series of elderly subjects at an audiology clinic, approximately one-third experienced auditory hallucinations, but of these only one subject (or a total of 0.8%) experienced musical hallucinations (Cole et al., 2002). More detailed reports of musical hallucinations have been published, albeit sparingly. Berrios (1990) published the first series of subjects with musical hallucinations and identified that they were more often seen in females compared to males, and positively correlated with advancing age, deafness, and brain disease. Since the Berrios (1990) study, literature reviews compiling case reports and small case series have been published (Keshavan et al., 1992; Evers and Ellger, 2004); however, no large single centre case series has been published to date. The most commonly accepted predisposing condition for the development of musical hallucinations has been shown to be hearing impairment, however, most people who have significant hearing impairment never develop musical hallucinations. Therefore, hearing impairment represents one variable in what likely represents a complex and E. C. Golden and K. A. Josephs multifactorial pathophysiological process (Evers and Ellger, 2004). Lesions anywhere along the auditory pathway, from the ear to the auditory association cortex, can lead to musical hallucinations (Cope and Baguley, 2009). In keeping with Berrios’ reported association between brain disease and musical hallucinations, case reports detailed musical hallucinations occurring in the setting of cerebral ischaemia (Calabro et al., 2012; Dinges et al., 2013; Woo et al., 2014), encephalitis (Douen and Bourque, 1997), tumour (Scott, 1979; Nagaratnam et al., 1996), postsurgery (Keshavan et al., 1988; Isolan et al., 2010), multiple sclerosis (Husain et al., 2014) and epilepsy (Roberts et al., 2001; Gentile et al., 2007; Ozsarac et al., 2012). Given the strong association between hearing impairment and musical hallucinations, some have suggested that musical hallucinations represent a release phenomenon. According to one proposal, sensory deprivation eliminates the afferent input to the auditory sensory processing system allowing spontaneous activity to occur in a network (Griffiths, 2000), analogous to the Charles Bonnet syndrome, in which visual impairment precipitates the development of visual hallucinations (Burke, 2002). A significant proportion of patients with the Charles Bonnet syndrome eventually develop dementia with Lewy bodies (Lapid et al., 2013). However, the prevalence of neurodegenerative disorders among those who experience musical hallucinations, and how these subjects compare to those with musical hallucinations in the setting of other disorders has not been addressed in the literature to date. Musical hallucinations are often noted to occur in cognitively normal people; however, anecdotally musical hallucinations have been described to occur in the presence of dementia (Mori et al., 2006; Berger, 2015). In this study, we sought to address this knowledge gap by characterizing the conditions associated with musical hallucinations through a large case series. We further aimed to further explore the prevalences and characteristics of neurological diseases, particularly neurodegenerative, in those experiencing musical hallucinations compared to groups without an associated neurological disease. Given the previous literature, we hypothesized that structural lesions would have a significant preference towards the right hemisphere. We further predicted an association between musical hallucinations and neurodegenerative disorders, specifically Lewy body disorders, given their known correlation with hallucinations. Materials and methods Subjects The research project was approved by the Mayo Institutional Review Board. The Mayo Clinic Medical Records Linkage System was used to identify all subjects who underwent evaluation at this institution and had musical hallucinations from 1 January 1996 to 31 December 2013. The system was queried Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 Musical hallucinations and associated neurological conditions to capture all medical records in which the terms ‘musical hallucinations’, ‘music AND hallucinations’, and ‘hear music’ including all tenses of ‘hear’ appeared. The medical records of all subjects identified were then reviewed to determine whether they met criteria for musical hallucinations defined as the perception of music not perceived by others present. Subjects were only included if they were at least 18 years old and the symptom of musical hallucinations was documented in a clinical encounter with a Mayo Clinic provider including physician, nurse practitioner, or psychologist. Subjects who only experienced isolated hypnagogic or hypnopompic hallucinations were excluded. Clinical data were abstracted in all subjects that met the inclusion criteria, including age at onset of musical hallucinations, the presence of visual or other auditory hallucinations, available imaging, documentation of hearing impairment, and audiometry. Subjects were divided into five categories according to the condition felt to be most strongly associated with musical hallucinations, recognizing that often multiple factors may have contributed. The five categories included neurologic, psychiatric, structural, drug toxicity or withdrawal and not otherwise classifiable (NOC) for those not fitting into the other four groups. Subjects with musical hallucinations were grouped in the neurologic category if the hallucinations developed in the setting of a neurological disorder. Within the neurologic category, those with a neurodegenerative disease were further subclassified to assess for associations. All subjects were subclassified based on the diagnosis rendered by the evaluating neurologist. Subjects were subclassified as having a Lewy body disorder (Parkinson’s disease, Parkinson’s disease dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies), Alzheimer’s dementia or frontotemporal dementia based on accepted diagnostic criteria. Others were diagnosed with dementia without a more specific diagnosis (dementia not otherwise specified or dementia NOS.) Sometimes those with a neurologic diagnosis had concomitant psychiatric disease, often as a neuropsychiatric symptom related to the neurological disease, and these subjects remained in the neurologic group given the main aim of our study. Subjects were placed in the structural category if they clearly developed musical hallucinations in the context of a focal brain lesion, with imaging done in close temporal relationship to the hallucinatory experience. Subjects who experienced seizures related to focal lesions were included in the structural category. Subjects placed in the psychiatric category experienced musical hallucinations that were clearly associated with worsening of psychiatric symptoms. For subjects in the drug category, there was a clear temporal relationship between the initiation of a drug or the withdrawal state and the musical hallucinations, which was noted by either patient and/or care provider. Co-morbid psychiatric conditions were collected for subjects in all groups. Statistical analyses Statistical analyses were performed using JMP statistical software (JMP, version 10.0.0; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina), with statistical significance set at P 5 0.05. Sex ratios and binary variables collected (presence or absence of hearing impairment, visual or other auditory hallucinations) were compared across groups and subgroups using the 2 test. Age at musical hallucination onset was compared across groups and subgroups using the analysis of variance test. BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 | 3795 If significance was found, the Tukey-Kramer post hoc test was then used to compare all pairs of groups. We did not adjust for multiple comparisons in our study given that we had an a priori hypothesis for testing for these associations. In addition, adjusting for multiple comparisons would have increased the likelihood of type II errors for associations that were not null. Not adjusting for multiple comparisons has been advocated since it will result in fewer errors in interpretation when the data are not random numbers but actual observations in nature (Rothman, 1990). Therefore, adjusting our data could have resulted in missing important findings. Results A total of 636 subjects were identified by the Mayo Medical Records Linkage System. Of these, 393 met inclusion criteria. The mean age at onset of the hallucinations was 56 years (range 18–98). Two hundred and fifty-seven (65.4%) of these subjects were female. One hundred and five (26.7%) subjects had documented hearing impairment, 77 of whom had audiometry confirming hearing impairment. While most subjects underwent head CT imaging, 207 (52.8%) had head MRI performed. Generalized atrophy, leukoaraiosis, and lacunar infarcts were common findings. After division into groups (Fig. 1), psychiatric disorders predominated (39%) with neurological disorders (25%) the next most common. Drug toxicity or withdrawal, structural lesions, and NOC made up a smaller proportion of the total subjects. Co-existing auditory and visual hallucinations were relatively common, as detailed in Table 1. Examples of specific songs or music genres for each of the five categories are shown in Table 2. Musical hallucinations were sometimes transient particularly when associated with a reversible neurological condition, structural lesion or drug effect. Often however, particularly in the neurodegenerative and NOC groups, the hallucinations were persistent, lasting for months to years at a time. Descriptions of these experiences are provided in Supplementary Box 1. Neurologic group A total of 98 patients had a primary neurologic diagnosis associated with the hallucinations. The mean age in this category was 64 years (range 18–98), which was significantly different than the mean age of all other groups and second oldest to the mean age of the NOC group, and 71.4% were female. Of these subjects, 32.7% had documented hearing loss, and 71.8% of subjects with hearing loss had supporting audiometry. By far, the most common neurological diagnosis seen was a neurodegenerative disorder (67.3%), followed by seizures (16.3%), and encephalitis (9.2%) (Table 3). The most common of the neurodegenerative diseases was Lewy body disease (34.8%) (Table 3). Of the Lewy body disorders, dementia Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 3796 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 E. C. Golden and K. A. Josephs mood, and at least two were associated with a deceased person. Auditory hallucinations were significantly more common in those in the psychiatric group than in all other groups. The frequency of hearing impairment was significantly less than for all other groups excluding structural, for which it was similar. Structural group Figure 1 The proportions of musical hallucinations attributed to different associated conditions. Psychiatric disease represented the most commonly associated condition; however, neurological disease and focal brain lesions together accounted for over a third of the patients. with Lewy bodies was the most common (60.9%), followed by Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia. Often, those with neurodegenerative disorders did not have the insight that their hallucinations represented false perceptions and were convinced that the neighbours were playing music too loudly. One subject was described to go around ‘knocking on neighbours doors and at times harassing them as a result of this symptom’. Songs were often religious or patriotic, and reminiscent of childhood. Visual hallucinations were significantly more common in those in the neurologic group relative to those in the psychiatric, structural, and NOC groups. Thirty-four per cent of those in the neurologic group had a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis: depressive disorders (n = 23), anxiety disorders (n = 6), psychotic disorders (n = 2), and bipolar disorder (n = 2). With the exception of bipolar disorder, many of these diagnoses were neuropsychiatric symptoms secondary to the neurological disease. Psychiatric group The mean age in the psychiatric category was 48.1 years (range 8–90), and 70.3% were female. Of these, 11.6% had documented hearing loss, and 72.2% of subjects with hearing loss had supporting audiometry. Depression and bipolar disorder accounted for the majority of diagnoses and there tended to be waxing and waning of the hallucinations with depressive and manic symptoms (Table 4). Psychotic disorders represented a smaller proportion (17.4%) with schizoaffective disorder being the most predominant of those. Only one person had obsessivecompulsive disorder. Songs tended to be affected by Thirty-seven subjects (9%) had structural lesions associated with the musical hallucinations. Of the 37, all had MRI completed except for two who had a head CT and one who did not have imaging available for review but had a history of a right temporal astrocytoma resection several years earlier, after which he developed seizures manifesting as musical hallucinations attributed to the lesion. The mean age was 43.5 years (range 18–87), and 48.9% were female. Hearing loss was documented in 13.5%, and 80% of these subjects had supporting audiometry. As shown in Table 5, the most common structural lesion noted was tumour (n = 14 subjects), with a lesser frequency of post-surgical lesions (comprised of resections of tumours, vascular anomalies, and a non-structural epileptic focus), mesial temporal sclerosis and undifferentiated lesions. Interestingly, only two subjects experienced musical hallucinations following an infarct documented by imaging. Tumours tended to be low grade (meningioma, oligodendroglioma, low-grade glioma, pilocytic astrocytoma) (n = 9 subjects) compared to high-grade glioma (n = 5 subjects). Temporal lobe involvement was noted with all the structural lesions with the exception of two, a tumour located in the pons with extension into the midbrain and cerebellum and a right parietal infarct. Both the right and left hemispheres were involved. More lesions were noted in the left hemisphere (57%) compared to the right hemisphere, however to an insignificant degree. Examples of lesions resulting in musical hallucinations are shown in Fig. 2. Songs heard were often more modern, including genres like country and rock. Twenty-seven per cent of those in the structural group had a co-morbid psychiatric diagnosis: depressive disorders (n = 6), anxiety disorders (n = 2), psychotic disorders (n = 2). Drug toxicity or withdrawal group Forty-five subjects experienced the onset of musical hallucinations in close temporal relationship to drug initiation or in the setting of withdrawal. The mean age in this group was 54 years (range 19–91), and 51.1% were female. Of these subjects, 24.4% had documented hearing loss, 63.5% of which had supporting audiometry. The most commonly associated subgroups were alcohol withdrawal and steroids. Table 6 is a complete list of drugs. Alcohol abuse was a common confounder in the psychiatric population, often accompanying depression. All cases of stimulants were in association with polysubstance abuse. In 48.9% of subjects, the hallucinations resolved completely upon Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 | 3797 Table 2 Description of music heard across groups Psych = psychiatric; LBD = Lewy body disease; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; FTD = frontotemporal dementia; NOS = not otherwise specified. Post hoc P-values were considered significant if 50.05. *All of the results except for the gender association would have survived Bonferroni correction. a Neuro versus Psych; bNeuro versus Structural; cNeuro versus Drug; dNeuro versus NOC; ePsych versus Structural; fPsych versus Drug; gPsych versus NOC; hStructural versus Drug; iStructural versus NOC; jDrug versus NOC. 50.0001a,b,c,d,g,i,j 50.0173b,c,e,f 50.0001a,b,d,f,g,i,j 50.0001a,e,f,g 50.0001a,b,d,e,h 58 73 (12.9) 39 (67.2%) 39 (67.2%) 17 (29.3%) 7 (12.1%) 37 44 (19.2) 18 (48.9%) 5 (13.5%) 10 (27.0%) 1 (2.7%) 45 54 (19.0) 23 (51.1%) 11 (24.4%) 7 (15.6%) 12 (26.7%) n Age at onset Frequency of Frequency of Frequency of Frequency of (SD) female gender hearing impairment other auditory hallucinations visual hallucinations 98 64 70 32 25 38 (19.6) (71.4%) (32.7%) (25.5%) (38.8%) 23 72 (9.9) 12 (52.2%) 11 (47.8%) 4 (17.4%) 17 (73.9%) 18 78 (8.5) 14 (77.8%) 8 (44.4%) 7 (38.9%) 7 (38.9%) 65 74 46 27 19 32 (11.1) (70.8%) (41.5%) (29.2%) (49.2%) 22 73 (12.7) 19 (86.4%) 8 (36.4%) 7 (31.8%) 7 (31.8%) 2 59 (3) 2 (100%) 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%) 155 48 (15.5) 109 (70.3%) 18 (11.6%) 74 (47.8%) 33 (21.3%) Structural NOC Drugs Dementia NOS All Neurodegenerative Psych FTD AD All neurologic LBD Neurodegenerative Neurological Table 1 Demographic features across all five categories groups P-values* Musical hallucinations and associated neurological conditions Category Neurologic Description Neurodegenerative ‘church choral music’ Structural Psychiatric Drug Not otherwise classifiable ‘echo of last music he heard’ ‘big band’ (n = 2 subjects) ‘single tune with the words “where were you when the Lord was crucified” or “where were you when your Lord was placed in the grave, 24 hours a day” ‘church hymns’ ‘rock music’ ‘old video game’ ‘Fraggle rock’ ‘country music’ (n = 2 subjects) ‘Queen’ ‘hearing a record playing over and over again which appeared to skip at times’ ‘believes it was someone who had passed away’ ‘fun and snappy but scary’ ‘radio playing in another room’ ‘favorite songs of late husband’ ‘like funeral music when depressed’ ‘music from Beauty and the Beast’ ‘hard rock’ ‘elevator music’ ‘Yankee Doodle Dandy’ ‘Bram’s type music’ ‘Christmas carols’ (n = 2 subjects) ‘string instruments’ ‘radio through vent’ ‘school songs and hymns’ ‘Glory, Glory, Alleluia’ ‘Silent Night, O Holy Night, Edelweiss, The Battle Hymn of the Republic’ ‘America the Beautiful sung as by a choir with an orchestra which lasted four to five months. . .[which] changed to a hymn, then a Christmas song, then to presently single notes.’ ‘How much is that doggy in the window, The Star Spangled Banner and Swing Low-Sweet Chariot’ ‘Japanese’ ‘Polish’ ‘Marine call, Silent Night, Little Drummer Boy, isolated trumpet or bugle calls or clarinet or flute music’ ‘almost constantly, I Walk the Line and This Land Is Your Land’ ‘National anthem’ ‘Star-Spangled banner’ Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 3798 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 E. C. Golden and K. A. Josephs Table 3 All neurological diagnoses associated with musical hallucinations Table 4 All psychiatric disorders associated with musical hallucinations Neurological diagnosis n Psychiatric diagnosis n Neurodegenerative Lewy body disorders Dementia with Lewy bodies Parkinson’s disease Parkinson’s disease dementia Dementia NOS Alzheimer’s dementia Frontotemporal dementia Seizure Encephalitis Post-concussive syndrome Amyloid beta related angiitis Migraine Posterior reversible encephalopathy syndrome Traumatic brain injury Leukoencephalopathy Multiple system atrophy 65 23 14 5 4 22 18 2 16 9 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 Depression Bipolar disorder Schizoaffective disorder Anxiety Delirium Psychotic disorder NOS Schizophrenia Spell Post-traumatic stress disorder Obsessive compulsive disorder Chronic fatigue syndrome Dysthymia Adjustment disorder Mood disorder NOS 62 29 12 13 10 9 6 6 3 1 1 1 1 1 cessation of the offending drug or withdrawal syndrome. In two subjects (4.4%), the hallucinations improved, and in another two subjects, no improvement was noted. In the remainder of subjects, follow-up information was not available. A high proportion (64%) of subjects in this group had a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis: depressive disorders (n = 17), anxiety disorders (n = 8), bipolar disorder (n = 3), and psychotic disorders (n = 1). Not otherwise classifiable group For those not otherwise classifiable (NOC), the average age was 73 years (range 36–94), which was significantly greater than the mean age of all other groups, and 67.2% were female. Of these subjects, 67.2% had hearing impairment, 74.4% of which had supporting audiometry. There were no unifying underlying co-morbidities; however, interestingly two subjects developed musical hallucinations after cochlear implants. As in the neurologic category, many of these subjects heard songs familiar to them from childhood, particularly religious or patriotic songs. Twenty-six per cent of subjects in this group had a co-morbid psychiatric diagnosis: depressive disorders (n = 9), anxiety disorders (n = 5), and psychotic disorders (n = 1). Discussion This study represents the largest series of musical hallucinations to date, from which several important points can be highlighted. Similar to previous studies (Berrios, 1990; Keshavan et al., 1992; Evers and Ellger, 2004) most of the individuals experiencing musical hallucinations were female (with the exception of those related to structural lesions and drugs where the genders were fairly equally divided). It remains unclear why there would be a female predominance amongst those with musical hallucinations in these groups, and likely the cause is multifactorial and complex, possibly related to underlying structural or metabolic differences between genders. Data evaluating gender differences in hallucinations associated with psychiatric and neurological disease is sparse; however, there is some literature to suggest that hallucinations are more common in females in psychiatric disease (Rector et al., 1992) and dementia (Xing et al., 2012). The average ages at which the individuals in the neurologic and NOC groups experienced musical hallucinations were significantly higher than other groups, and that of the neurodegenerative disorders group was quite similar to that of the NOC group, namely 74 and 73 years, respectively. Neurological disease and structural lesions were associated with musical hallucinations in a significant proportion of subjects—a total of 135. Several of the subjects included in the psychiatric group presented with new-onset psychosis at an older age but did not have a neurological evaluation or cognitive screening. For some of these subjects, follow-up in the institution was lacking, raising the question as to whether some may have had underlying neurodegenerative disease. Hearing impairment was common across groups, particularly in the relatively small proportion of NOC subjects. The prevalence of documented hearing impairment was significantly higher in the NOC group than the other groups and audiometry was not available for those who denied hearing impairment or for whom it was unknown in this group, which may suggest the prevalence was even higher. As the history and exam had not identified co-morbidities or toxicity in the NOC group, which might otherwise have been associated with the hallucinations, the high incidence of hearing impairment instead supports the established theory that isolated peripheral auditory dysfunction alone can lead to musical hallucinations (Gordon, 1997). However, deafness with normal central functioning does Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 Musical hallucinations and associated neurological conditions Table 5 All structural lesions associated with musical hallucinations Structural Location n Tumour Left temporal Left frontotemporal Left temporoparietal Right parietotemporal Right pons, midbrain, cerebellum Total = 14 8 3 1 1 1 Total = 7 4 1 1 1 Total = 4 2 2 Total = 3 1 1 1 Total = 2 1 1 Total = 4 1 Post-surgical lesion Right temporal Right frontotemporal left temporoparietal Left temporal Mesial temporal sclerosis Right Left Undifferentiated lesion Left frontotemporal Right temporal Left temporal Infarct Left temporal Right parietal Encephalomalacia Arteriovenous malformation Arachnoid cyst Subdural fluid Bilateral parietal, right temporal, occipital, and cerebellum Right frontotemporal Left frontotemporal Left temporal Right temporal Right temporal Right hemisphere 1 1 1 1 1 1 not account for the fact that most people with hearing impairment never develop musical hallucinations and thus an element of abnormal central processing must be implicated even in this group (Stewart et al., 2006). Hearing impairment was also relatively common in the neurologic and drug group (32.7 and 24.4%, respectively), which likely signifies that peripheral pathology can predispose a brain already made vulnerable from central dysfunction to musical hallucinations. Additionally, in the series, 11 of our subjects with musical hallucinations had normal audiometry, but had other associated psychiatric or neurological diagnoses, directly contesting the idea that hearing impairment is a requisite to the development of musical hallucinations (Gordon, 1997). The exact mechanism behind which hearing impairment can predispose an individual to musical hallucinations is unclear but has been hypothesized to be related to disinhibition of the areas involved in musical imagery through the reduction of external auditory input. Neural activation during musical imagery is remarkably similar to that in the actual perception of music (Zatorre et al., 1996; Halpern and Zatorre, 1999; Kumar et al., 2014). However, while activity in the auditory association cortex is seen in both BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 | 3799 musical imagery and perception, the primary auditory cortex is not necessarily activated in musical imagery (Cope and Baguley, 2009). In a model proposed by Griffiths (2000), hearing impairment decreases auditory input to the primary auditory cortex, releasing basal inhibition on the auditory association cortex allowing for spontaneous activity and potentially augmenting connections with areas that encode and recognize patterned sounds such as music. It is suggested that the threshold for spontaneous activity varies between people and may be lowered by disease or toxicity affecting the auditory pathway (Griffiths, 2000; Kumar et al., 2014). This study characterizes a variety of conditions associated with musical hallucinations that may act to lower the threshold for spontaneous activity of the auditory pathway. Notably, the structural lesions we found associated with musical hallucinations involved both hemispheres with an insignificant preference to the left as opposed to previous studies, which have shown a marginal preference toward the right (Berrios, 1990; Keshavan et al., 1992). All lesions involved the temporal lobe with the exception of one subject who had a brainstem tumour with a separate lesion in the cerebellum and another in whom musical hallucinations developed after a right parietal stroke. This case series provides novel support for bilateral brain representation in the localization of musical hallucinations. Our series was able to capture all those who had addressed musical hallucinations with a care provider, even if not their major complaint, and thus provided greater sensitivity for assessing the hemispheric predominance in focal brain lesions associated with musical hallucinations. One subject had a history of central pontine myelinolysis and developed alcohol withdrawal-induced musical hallucinations, and perhaps pre-existing pontine pathology predisposed him to this particular drug effect. These findings match previous studies, which have found lesions located along the auditory pathway and particularly the pons and temporal lobes resulting in musical hallucinations (Douen and Bourque, 1997; Roberts et al., 2001; Isolan et al., 2010; Calabro et al., 2012; Ozsarac et al., 2012; Serby et al., 2013; Woo et al., 2014). It may be that deposition from neurodegenerative pathology can also act to disturb connections within the auditory pathway and lower the threshold for spontaneous activity of the auditory association cortex. In one case of musical hallucinations in a subject with Alzheimer’s disease, regional cerebral blood flow was increased in the left temporal region and left angular gyrus (Mori et al., 2006). In this series, Lewy body disorders accounted for the majority of neurodegenerative disorders, and dementia with Lewy bodies predominated amongst these. The prevalence of visual hallucinations in this series in Alzheimer’s dementia (38.9%) is higher than that reported by McKeith et al. (1992) and possibly some subjects with Alzheimer’s dementia had co-existing dementia with Lewy bodies. Additionally, in the subjects who had an undifferentiated dementia, the high prevalence of visual hallucinations Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 3800 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 E. C. Golden and K. A. Josephs Figure 2 Imaging in subjects with musical hallucinations related to structural lesions. (A) Coronal T2 FLAIR head MRI scan of a 33year-old female with focal epilepsy involving a musical aura with a right superior temporal pilocytic astrocytoma. (B) Coronal T1 gadoliniumenhanced head MRI scan of an 87-year-old female with left temporal lobe radiation necrosis. (C) Axial T2 head MRI scan of a 69-year-old male with high grade astrocytoma in the pons with extension into the midbrain. (D) Axial T1 gadolinium-enhanced head MRI scan of a 77-year-old female with a right temporal arteriovenous malformation. (E) Axial T2 FLAIR head MRI scan of a 74-year-old male with a recent left insular infarction. (F) Coronal T2 FLAIR head MRI of a 47-year-old female after resection of right mesiotemporal meningioma. Table 6 All drugs associated with musical hallucinations, either in toxicity or withdrawal Drug n Alcohol withdrawal Steroids Stimulants Opiates Antidepressants Alcohol Opiate withdrawal Benzodiazepine withdrawal Lisinopril Pramipexole Ramelteon Carbidopa/levodopa Clarithromycin Polysubstance abuse 10 9 5 4 4 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 (31.8%) suggests that many may have underlying Lewy body disease. Lewy body deposition in the temporal lobe, particularly in the amygdala and parahippocampus, strongly correlates with the development of visual hallucinations (Harding et al., 2002); however, the pathological correlation of auditory hallucinations in Lewy body disease has not been demonstrated. The neurochemistry of psychosis in dementia likely involves dysregulation of multiple neurotransmitters (Lanari et al., 2006; Zahodne and Fernandez, 2008); however, a cholinergic deficit in the temporal neocortex has been shown to be associated with hallucinations in both Lewy body and Alzheimer’s disease (Perry et al., 1999; Ballard et al., 2005; Zahodne and Fernandez, 2008; Pinto et al., 2011). Indeed, preferential damage to the nucleus basalis of Meynert, from which a significant proportion of the cholinergic neurons originate, is greater in the Lewy body disorders compared to Alzheimer’s disease where dysfunction appears to be related to cholinergic axonal input to the neocortex, and the cumulative cholinergic deficit is greater in Lewy body disease (Perry et al., 1993). Further supporting a role for acetylcholine in the development of psychosis, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors are effective in both Lewy body and Alzheimer’s psychosis (Cummings, 2005; Zahodne, 2008). Musical hallucinations have been effectively treated using donepezil in at least one subject in the literature (Ukai et al., 2007) suggesting a contribution of the cholinergic deficit to the development of musical hallucinations similar to other hallucinations. We would propose that our finding that Lewy body disorders may be more associated with Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 Musical hallucinations and associated neurological conditions musical hallucinations than other neurodegenerative disorders is due to a greater cholinergic deficit and possibly due to early brainstem involvement, affecting the proximal auditory pathways. Subjects who had musical hallucinations in association with a neurodegenerative disease or with isolated hearing impairment tended to hear music, which was more persistent over a longer time period. Several of these individuals experienced an interesting phenomenon where the music disintegrated over time into increasingly shorter melodies and tunes and sometimes terminating in repetitive single notes well described by one subject who reported that she ‘noted a change in the phenomena. . .such as hearing a certain phrase or line of a song being repeated “back and forth” rather than hearing a full song in its entirety’. This was described in the original series by Berrios (1990) and has later been referred to by Sacks (2007). Sacks made the comparison to telescoping, an analogous phenomenon in phantom limb sensation (another proposed release phenomenon) where sensation of the distal part of the phantom retracts over time (Nikolajsen and Jensen, 2001). Interestingly, qualitative differences emerged between songs heard by subject groups. For those in the neurodegenerative and NOC groups, which also had the highest rates of hearing impairment, songs tended to date back to childhood and also represented likely overlearned material given that they were commonly religious, patriotic, or cultural songs. Those who experienced musical hallucinations secondary to structural lesions, often in the context of a musical aura, heard more modern music, such as country or rock, likely not learned in childhood, whereas in psychiatric causes, songs tended to be mood-congruent, either sad or ‘scary’ music, and sometimes represented a reminder of a close acquaintance or family member who had died (Table 2). This difference may in part be related to musical preferences with age, with older individuals favouring religious or traditional songs. However, the traditional songs heard by the neurodegenerative and NOC groups are distinguished by their relatively high frequency throughout a lifetime and relationship to emotionally-charged situations. Thus, they may be more vulnerable to manifesting as hallucinations in the presence of hearing impairment or neurodegenerative disease. There were a number of limitations associated with this retrospective study. As previously noted by Keshavan and colleagues (1992), dividing subjects creates boundaries for a phenomenon that is likely multifactorial. However, we believe that the groupings are generally instructive when viewed with an understanding that multiple variables likely play a role in the development of musical hallucinations. Subjects were divided into categories according to conditions associated with musical hallucinations; however, psychiatric disorders were a common comorbidity in all categories as detailed. There was an ascertainment bias present in the study given that data collected were based on chart review. Subjects were restricted to those who had addressed their musical hallucinations with a medical care BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 | 3801 provider, sometimes unsolicited and sometimes as part of screening in neurologic or psychiatric review of symptoms, which may have slightly shifted the population. Certainly, there were patients seen at Mayo Clinic during the study time period outside of neurology and psychiatry practices who were not screened for hallucinations. Detail concerning hallucination characteristics and temporal profile was often lacking. Hearing impairment was not always screened for or documented so its prevalence in this population was certainly falsely low. This study reviewed cases seen at Mayo Clinic across specialties, including primary care, neurology, psychiatry, otorhinolaryngology, and audiology, a design which offered a favourably large case series from which we were able to analyse the demographics of musical hallucinations and their association to comorbid conditions. In the future, a cross-sectional population-based study would be informative to eliminate the bias associated with a single centre series. Additionally, a prospective study following those who have musical hallucinations compared to age-matched controls with a standardized assessment of hearing and neurological exam including formal cognitive testing, would be helpful to better understand the relationship between musical hallucinations and neurological disease. Conclusion Musical hallucinations can occur in a wide variety of disease states, of which neurological disorders and brain lesions represent a substantial proportion. Neurodegenerative diseases represent a large subset of neurological disease which can be associated with musical hallucinations and of these, the Lewy body disorders tend to be seen commonly. Hearing impairment likely represents a variable predisposing to the phenomenon, and those groups with the highest rates of hearing loss hear songs (neurodegenerative and NOC) that tend to be derived from childhood compared to other groups. A future prospective study would be helpful to further delineate an association between musical hallucinations and neurological disease. Funding The study was funded by the Mayo Clinic Department of Neurology. Supplementary material Supplementary material is available at Brain online. References Ballard C, Margallo-Lana M, Juszczak E, Douglas S, Swann A, Thomas A, et al. Quetiapine and rivastigmine and cognitive decline Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017 3802 | BRAIN 2015: 138; 3793–3802 in Alzheimer’s disease: randomised double blind placebo controlled trial. BMJ 2005; 330: 874. Berger J The necessity of musical hallucinations. Nautilus Online blog. January 29, 2015. Issue 20. Chapter 4. “Creativity” http://nautil. us/issue/20/creativity/the-necessity-of-musical-hallucinations. Berrios GE. Musical hallucinations. A historical and clinical study. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 156: 188–94. Burke W. The neural basis of Charles Bonnet hallucinations: a hypothesis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002; 73: 535–41. Calabro RS, Baglieri A, Ferlazzo E, Passari S, Marino S, Bramanti P. Neurofunctional assessment in a stroke patient with musical hallucinations. Neurocase 2012; 18: 514–20. Cole MG, Dowson L, Dendukuri N, Belzile E. The prevalence and phenomenology of auditory hallucinations among elderly subjects attending an audiology clinic. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002; 17: 444–52. Cope TE, Baguley DM. Is musical hallucination an otological phenomenon? a review of the literature. Clin Otolaryngol 2009; 34: 423–30. Cummings JL, Koumaras B, Chen M, Mirski D, Rivastigmine Nursing Home Study Team. Effects of rivastigmine treatment on the neuropsychiatric and behavioral disturbances of nursing home residents with moderate to severe probable Alzheimer’s disease: a 26-week, multicenter, open-label study. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother 2005; 3: 137–48. Dinges M, Riemer T, Schubert T, Pruss H Musical hallucinations after pontine ischemia: the auditory Charles Bonnet syndrome? J Neurol 2013; 260: 2678–80. Douen AG, Bourque PR. Musical auditory hallucinosis from Listeria rhombencephalitis. Can J Neurol Sci 1997; 24: 70–2. Evers S, Ellger T. The clinical spectrum of musical hallucinations. J Neurol Sci 2004; 227: 55–65. Fukunishi I, Horikawa N, Onai H. Prevalence rate of musical hallucinations in a general hospital setting. Psychosomatics 1998; 39: 175. Gentile S, Ferrero M, Giudice RL, Rainero I, Pinessi L. Abdominal pain associated with musical hallucinations: a case report. Eur J Neurol 2007; 14: e7–8. Gordon AG. Do musical hallucinations always arise from the inner ear? Med Hypotheses 1997; 49: 111–22. Griffiths TD. Musical hallucinosis in acquired deafness. Phenomenology and brain substrate. Brain 2000; 123(Pt 10): 2065–76. Halpern AR, Zatorre RJ. When that tune runs through your head: a PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cereb Cortex 1999; 9: 697–704. Harding AJ, Broe GA, Halliday GM. Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease relate to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain 2002; 125(Pt 2): 391–403. Husain F, Levin J, Scott J, Fjeldstad C. Recurrent refrains in a patient with multiple sclerosis: Earworms or musical hallucinations? Multiple Scler Relat Disord 2014; 3: 276–8. Isolan GR, Bianchin MM, Bragatti JA, Torres C, Schwartsmann G. Musical hallucinations following insular glioma resection. Neurosurg Focus 2010; 28: E9. Keshavan MS, David AS, Steingard S, Lishman WA. Musical hallucinations: a review and synthesis. Cogn Behav Neurol 1992; 5: 211–23. Keshavan MS, Kahn EM, Brar JS. Musical hallucinations following removal of a right frontal meningioma. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988; 51: 1235–6. Kumar S, Sedley W, Barnes GR, Teki S, Friston KJ, Griffiths TD. A brain basis for musical hallucinations. Cortex 2014; 52: 86–97. Lanari A, Amenta F, Silvestrelli G, Tomassoni D, Parnetti L. Neurotransmitter deficits in behavioural and psychological symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. Mech Ageing Dev 2006; 127: 158–65. Lapid MI, Burton MC, Chang MT, Rummans TA, Cha SS, Leavitt JA, et al. Clinical phenomenology and mortality in Charles Bonnet syndrome. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2013; 26: 3–9. E. C. Golden and K. A. Josephs McKeith IG, Perry RH, Fairbairn AF, Jabeen S, Perry EK. Operational criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Psychol Med 1992; 22: 911–22. Mori T, Ikeda M, Fukuhara R, Sugawara Y, Nakata S, Matsumoto N, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow change in a case of Alzheimer’s disease with musical hallucinations. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006; 256: 236–9. Nagaratnam N, Virk S, Brdarevic O. Musical hallucinations associated with recurrence of a right occipital meningioma. Br J Clin Pract 1996; 50: 56–7. Nikolajsen L, Jensen TS. Phantom limb pain. Br J Anaesth 2001; 87: 107–16. Ozsarac M, Aksay E, Kiyan S, Unek O, Gulec FF. De novo cerebral arteriovenous malformation: Pink Floyd’s song “Brick in the Wall” as a warning sign. J Emerg Med 2012; 43: e17–20. Perry E, Walker M, Grace J, Perry R. Acetylcholine in mind: a neurotransmitter correlate of consciousness? Trends Neurosci 1999; 22: 273–80. Perry EK, Irving D, Kerwin JM, McKeith IG, Thompson P, Collerton D, et al. Cholinergic transmitter and neurotrophic activities in Lewy body dementia: similarity to Parkinson’s and distinction from Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1993; 7: 69–79. Pinto T, Lanctot KL, Herrmann N. Revisiting the cholinergic hypothesis of behavioral and psychological symptoms in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Ageing Res Rev 2011; 10: 404–12. Rector NA, Seeman MV. Auditory hallucinations in women and men. Schizophr Res 1992; 7: 233–36. Roberts DL, Tatini U, Zimmerman RS, Bortz JJ, Sirven JI. Musical hallucinations associated with seizures originating from an intracranial aneurysm. Mayo Clin Proc 2001; 76: 423–6. Rothman KJ. No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology 1990; 1: 43–6. Sacks O. The power of music. Brain 2006; 129(Pt 10): 2528–32. Sacks O. Musicophilia. New York: Vintage Books; 2007. Scott M. Musical hallucinations from meningioma. JAMA 1979; 241: 1683. Serby MJ, Hagiwara M, O’Connor L, Lalwani AK. Musical hallucinations associated with pontine lacunar lesions. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2013; 25: 153–6. Shelley PB. Complete poetical works of percy bysshe shelley. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1901. Stewart L, von Kriegstein K, Warren JD, Griffiths TD. Music and the brain: disorders of musical listening. Brain 2006;129(Pt 10): 2533–53. Ukai S, Yamamoto M, Tanaka M, Shinosaki K, Takeda M. Donepezil in the treatment of musical hallucinations. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007; 61: 190–2. Vitorovic D, Biller J. Musical hallucinations and forgotten tunes - case report and brief literature review. Front Neurol 2013; 4: 109. Warner N, Aziz V. Hymns and arias: musical hallucinations in older people in Wales. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005; 20: 658–60. Wieser HG. Music and the brain. Lessons from brain diseases and some reflections on the “emotional” brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003; 999: 76–94. Woo PY, Leung LN, Cheng ST, Chan KY. Monoaural musical hallucinations caused by a thalamocortical auditory radiation infarct: a case report. J Med Case Rep 2014; 8: 400. Xing Y, Wei C, Chu C, Zhou A, Li F, Wu L, et al. Stage-specific gender differences in cognitive and neuropsychiatric manifestations of vascular dementia. Am J Alzheimer’s Dis other Demen 2012; 27: 433–38. Zahodne LB, Fernandez HH. Pathophysiology and treatment of psychosis in Parkinson’s disease: a review. Drugs Aging 2008; 25: 665–82. Zatorre RJ, Halpern AR, Perry DW, Meyer E, Evans AC. Hearing in the Mind’s Ear: a pet investigation of musical imagery and perception. J Cogn Neurosci 1996; 8: 29–46. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-abstract/138/12/3793/413249/Minds-on-replay-musical-hallucinations-and-their by George Mason University user on 15 September 2017