Journal of Family Psychology 2010, Vol. 24, No. 2, 135–144 © 2010 American Psychological Association 0893-3200/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018974 Parental Reactions to Children’s Negative Emotions: Prospective Relations to Chinese Children’s Psychological Adjustment Annie Tao and Qing Zhou Yun Wang University of California Beijing Normal University The prospective relations between five types of parental reactions to children’s negative emotions (PRCNE) and children’s psychological adjustment (behavioral problems and social competence) were examined in a two-wave longitudinal study of 425 school-age children in China. Parents (mostly mothers) reported their own PRCNE. Parents, teachers, and children or peers reported on children’s adjustment. Parental punitive reactions positively predicted externalizing problems (controlling for baseline), whereas emotion- and problem-focused reactions were negatively related to internalizing problems. Parental minimizing and encouragement of emotion expression were unrelated to adjustment. Concurrent relations were found between PRCNE and parents’ authoritative and authoritarian parenting dimensions. However, PRCNE did not uniquely predict adjustment controlling for global parenting dimensions. The findings have implications for cultural adaptation of parent-focused interventions for families of Chinese origin. Keywords: culture, parental reactions to children’s emotions Children of all ages experience negative emotions in daily life. How parents respond to children’s negative emotions is an important pathway to socialize children’s emotion regulation and expression (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998). Previous research based on Western samples has documented systematic relations between parental reactions to children’s negative emotions (henceforth labeled as PRCNE) and children’s adjustment (see Eisenberg et al., 1998 for a review). However, it is unclear whether these findings can be generalized to other cultures, especially the cultures with different values on emotion expression. Moreover, few researchers have examined whether PRCNE (i.e., context-specific parenting behaviors) have unique relations to adjustment above and beyond global parenting dimensions. The present study addressed these gaps by examining the prospective relations of PRCNE to Chinese children’s adjustment. We also tested the relations of PRCNE to global parenting dimensions, and whether PRCNE uniquely predict children’s adjustment controlling for global parenting. PRCNE In research based on Western samples, two clusters of PRCNE have been identified: a) unsupportive PRCNE, which include punitive and minimizing responses, and b) supportive PRCNE, which include emotion-focused responses or comforting, problem-solving, and encouraging emotion expression (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994). Unsupportive PRCNE are expected to predict poorer adjustment because they invalidate children’s emotional experience, thereby teaching them to suppress negative emotions (e.g., Eisenberg, Fabes, & Murphy, 1996; Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997). Suppression of emotional expression may be adaptive in the moment to evade punishment, but over time, unexpressed or “stored” negative emotions may accumulate and become more difficult to manage (Roberts & Strayer, 1987). Moreover, punitive responses increase tensions in the parent-child relationship (Power, 2004), which in turn impairs children’s emotion regulation. Indeed, punitive and minimizing responses have been related to children’s lower social competence and higher externalizing problems (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Supportive PRCNE are hypothesized to predict better adjustment because they promote positive parent-child relationships, which foster children’s emotional security (Cassidy, 1994). Moreover, supportive PRCNE enable children to accept and learn to manage emotions through modeling and coaching (Gottman et al., 1997; Power, 2004). Indeed, problem-focused and emotion-focused responses have been positively related to children’s social skills (Eisenberg et al., 1999; Gottman et al., 1997). However, the findings for parental encouragement of expression are relatively weak (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Some researchers found that it related negatively to preschoolers’ aggression (e.g., Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994) and positively to Annie Tao and Qing Zhou, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley; Yun Wang, National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (Beijing Normal University). This research was supported by American Psychological Association Dissertation Award, Arizona State University Graduate and Professional Student Association Research Grant Award, and a Faculty Research Grant from the University of California, Berkeley to Qing Zhou. The authors wish to thank all the children, parents, and teachers who participated or contributed to this project. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Qing Zhou, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall #1650, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-1650. E-mail: qingzhou@berkeley.edu 135 136 TAO, ZHOU, AND WANG preschoolers’ social competence (e.g., Denham, MitchellCopeland, Strandberg, Auerbach, & Blair, 1997), whereas others found it unrelated to school-age children’s adjustment (Eisenberg et al., 1996,1999; Jones, Eisenberg, Fabes, & MacKinnon, 2002). There are at least two limitations in existing research. First, with the exception of Eisenberg et al. (1996 and 1999), most studies focused on toddlers and preschoolers. Longitudinal studies suggested as children grow older, parents change in their frequency of using various PRCNE (Eisenberg et al., 1999). Thus, one should examine whether the PRCNE-adjustment relations found among younger children can generalize to school-age children. Second, most studies were conducted with primarily EuropeanAmerican (EuA) samples. Because cultural differences exist in the values regarding emotion expression, it is important to examine whether Western findings can be generalized to Eastern families. Culture and PRCNE In Eastern cultures that value collectivism, hierarchy, and embeddedness (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Matsumoto et al., 2008), there is a general emphasis on control/inhibition of emotion expression due to the importance of maintaining interpersonal harmony. Correspondingly, cultural differences were found in the norms (means) of individuals’ emotion expressivity. For example, adults from East Asian cultures reported less intense emotions than adults from Western cultures (Soto, Levenson, & Ebling, 2005). Individualistic values were positively associated with countrylevel means of individuals’ expressivity (Matsumoto et al., 2008). Cultural values with respect to certain behaviors may influence the development of such behaviors through shaping the social interaction (Chen & French, 2008). Thus, cultural values on emotion control/inhibition may affect how parents socialize children’s emotion regulation/ expression and its effects on child adjustment. In one of the few cross-cultural studies on parental socialization of emotions, U.S. mothers scored higher than Japanese mothers on a broad dimension of encouragement of emotional expressivity (Zahn-Waxler, Friedman, Cole, Mizuta, & Hiruma, 1996). However, no studies have examined parents’ specific responses to children’s emotions and their relations to child adjustment in a non-Western sample. Although a within-culture study like the present one cannot test hypotheses on cultural differences in the means of PRCNE, we can at least test whether the hypothesized relations between PRCNE and adjustment based upon Western samples can be generalized to a non-Western sample. These analyses test whether there are cross-cultural differences/similarities in the socialization function of PRCNE. We expect that cultural differences in values on emotion expression are most likely to affect the relations of minimizing reactions and encouragement of expression to child adjustment. Specifically, in contrast to the EuA culture in which minimizing responses are negatively related to child adjustment (see Eisenberg et al., 1998 for a review), we hypothesized that it might be unrelated or positively related to Chinese children’s adjustment due to the general restraint on emotion expression. Similarly, in contrast to the EuA culture in which encouragement of expression was found to predict children’s positive adjustment (Denham et al., 1997; Fabes, Poulin, Eisenberg, & Madden-Derdich, 2002), we hypothesized that it might be unrelated or negatively related to Chinese children’s adjustment. In contrast, we hypothesized that the relations of problem-focused, emotion-focused, and punitive reactions to Chinese children’s adjustment would show similar patterns as those found in EuA samples. These hypotheses were developed largely based on the research on global parenting. First, PRCNE are expected to be associated with the “authoritative” (characterized by high parental warmth, and the use of reasoning in discipline; Baumrind, 1996) and “authoritarian” (characterized by low warmth, high restrictiveness, and harsh discipline) parenting dimensions. Problem-focused reactions are consistent with the use of reasoning in authoritative parenting, and emotion-focused reactions reflect parental warmth and responsiveness (Fabes et al., 2002). Furthermore, punitive reactions are consistent with the coercive and harsh discipline style of authoritarian parenting. Second, recent cross-cultural research shows that authoritative parenting is associated with positive child adjustment, whereas the opposite relations are found for authoritarian parenting in both Chinese and Western families. Although some researchers have argued that authoritative parenting may not have the same benefits for child adjustment in the Chinese culture (Chao, 1994, 2001), the latest empirical investigations, particularly those that utilized large samples, a longitudinal design, and examined multiple domains of adjustment, suggest that both the attributes of authoritative and authoritarian parenting and their relations to Chinese children’s adjustment are similar to those found in EuA samples (Chen, Dong, & Zhou, 1997; Sorkhabi, 2005; Zhou, Eisenberg, Wang, & Reiser, 2004; Zhou et al., 2008). Therefore, we expected problem- and emotionfocused reactions to relate positively to Chinese children’s adjustment, whereas the opposite relations were expected for punitive reactions. PRCNE and Global Parenting Dimensions Global parenting dimensions (e.g., authoritative and authoritarian parenting) reflect “the constellation of parental attitudes toward the child” (Darling & Steinberg, 1993). Holden and Miller (1999) theorized that global parenting dimensions are determinants of situation-specific parenting practices because parents’ general childrearing attitudes would influence their behaviors in specific contexts. Thus, PRCNE and global parenting are expected to relate to each other. Specifically, we expect problem- and emotionfocused reactions to be positively related to authoritative parenting and negatively related to authoritarian parenting. Punitive reactions are expected to relate positively to authoritarian parenting and negatively to authoritative parenting. Because of the cultural emphasis on emotion inhibition, Chinese parents might tend to use minimizing reactions or PARENTAL REACTIONS, NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, CHINESE avoid using encouragement of emotion expression regardless of their overall parenting tendencies. Thus, these two PRCNE might be unrelated to global parenting. In addition to the interrelations between PRCNE and global parenting, it is also important to examine whether they have unique and/or overlapping relations to child adjustment. These analyses can shed light on the potential developmental mechanisms. Prior research has indicated that children’s emotion-related processes (e.g., emotion regulation and expression) are key mechanisms through which emotion-related parenting practices (such as PRCNE) influence child adjustment (Eisenberg et al., 1998). If PRCNE and global parenting overlap with each other in predicting adjustment, the emotion-related processes might be a common/shared mechanism underlying these socialization factors. If global parenting but not PRCNE uniquely predicts adjustment, other mechanisms besides emotion-related processes (e.g., children’s neurocognitive development) might further explain the link between global parenting and adjustment. If PRCNE but not global parenting uniquely predicts adjustment, PRCNE might mediate the relation between global parenting and child adjustment. In summary, the first goal of the study was to examine the prospective relations of PRCNE to Chinese school-age children’s adjustment (externalizing and internalizing problems and social competence). We expected the relations of problem-focused, emotion-focused, and punitive reactions and Chinese children’s adjustment to be similar to those found with Western samples, whereas the Western findings on minimizing reactions and encouragement of emotion expression might not generalize to Chinese families. The second goal was to examine the associations between PRCNE and global parenting dimensions, and to test their unique relations to child adjustment. We expected problemand emotion-focused reactions to be positively related to authoritative parenting and negatively related to authoritarian parenting. Punitive reactions were expected to relate negatively to authoritative parenting and positively to authoritarian parenting. Because of the cultural expectation to inhibit emotional expression, we expected that encouragement of emotion and minimizing reactions might be unrelated to global parenting. Because research has shown that socioeconomic status (SES) likely influences the relations between parenting and child adjustment (Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1994), the effect of SES was controlled in the analyses. Method Participants The sample came from a two-wave (3.8 years apart) longitudinal study of 1st and 2nd grade children in Beijing, China (Zhou et al., 2004, 2008). At Wave 1 (W1, summer 2000), 425 children (55.5% girls, 49.4% 1st-graders, M age ! 7.7 years, SD ! .6, age range ! 6.6 –9.1 years) were recruited from two public elementary schools. Children were recruited from 14 classes, with 25– 40 students in each class. Ninety-one percent of the children had no siblings. 137 Seventy-five percent of children came from two-parent families, 22% from extended families, and 3% from singleparent families. Monthly family income ranged from 200 to 10,000 RMB (M ! 2,456.3 RMB, SD ! 1454.4). The currency exchange rate between U.S. dollar and Chinese RMB was about 1:8.3 at W1. Parental education was reported on the following scale: 1 ! ! 9 years (middle school or lower), 2 ! 10 to 12 years (high school), 3 ! 13 to 16 years (college), and 4 ! " sixteen years (graduate school). Mean maternal and paternal education were 2.46 (SD ! .66) and 2.49 (SD ! .67) (i.e., high school diploma to some college education). The sample represented primarily lowto middle-income families based on the local demographic statistics (National Bureau of Statistics of China, 2000). At Wave 2 (W2, spring 2004), 89.9% of the children from W1 were reassessed (N ! 382; 52.9% girls, M age ! 11.6 years, SD ! .6, age range ! 10.1–12.9) while either in the 5th (50%) or 6th grades. There were no new participants at W2. The children who were assessed at both waves (N ! 382) were compared to those assessed at W1 only (N ! 43) on W1 demographics, parenting, and child adjustment. Children who had only participated during W1 came from families with higher maternal education, paternal education, and family income than those who were assessed at both waves, ts (dfs ! 393, 383, and 354) ! #2.7, #3.3, and #2.2, ps $ .01, .01, and .05; their parents also reported higher authoritative parenting, t(df ! 394) ! #2.59, p $ .02. No significant group differences were found on any child adjustment variable. Most of the children who dropped out of the study (86%) could not be located because they left the original school after W1. Like many metropolitan cities in China, children are assigned to public schools according to their home residence in Beijing. However, for a higher tuition cost, some families can enroll their children in private schools or public schools (often of better quality or reputation) that are outside their area of residence. It is possible that the children from families with higher SES were more likely to change schools during the course of the study because their families had the financial resources to send them to private or public schools of better quality. Procedure At both waves, an introductory letter and consent form were given to the parents of all 1st and 2nd graders (at W1, N ! 589) or eligible 5th and 6th graders who participated at W1 (at W2, N ! 387). Four hundred and twenty-five parents (72%) at W1 and 382 parents (99%) at W2 gave consent. Data were obtained through questionnaires completed by parents, teachers, children or peers. The parent questionnaire was asked to be completed by the mother if possible. Seventy-eight percent and 82% of the parent questionnaires were completed by mothers at W1 and W2, 16% and 12 % by fathers, and 6% and 6% by other caregivers. A Box’s M test was conducted to examine whether the variance and covariance matrix of study variables differed between the children whose mothers completed the reports and those for whom fathers or other caregivers completed the reports. The test was not significant, indicating that the 138 TAO, ZHOU, AND WANG associations between parenting and child adjustment were not moderated by the parent figure. The head teacher (Ns ! 14 at both W1 and W2; children had different head teachers at W1 and W2) completed the teacher questionnaires (return rates ! 98.9% at W1 and 97.9% at W2). The child and peer questionnaires were group-administered in class by two research assistants after written assent was obtained. Adults were paid for their participation, and children were given a small gift. Measures The measures that had not been previously used in Chinese samples were forward- and back-translated by bilingual speakers fluent in both Chinese and English. Englishspeaking researchers assisted in providing clarifications regarding difficult-to-translate items. Parental reaction to children’s negative emotions (W1). The Chinese translation of the Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES; Fabes et al., 2002) was used. The scale presented parents with 12 typical situations in which children are experiencing negative affect (e.g., being scared of injections, being nervous about possibly embarrassing him/herself in public). All situations pertain to normative expressions of negative emotion for children ages 4 to 12. For each situation, parents were asked to indicate how likely (on a 7-point scale from 1 ! “very unlikely” to 7 ! “very likely”) they would be to react in each of five different ways: (a) punitive reactions that decrease parents’ exposure or need to deal with children’s negative emotions (% ! .72); (b) emotion-focused reactions that help children feel better (% ! .76); (c) problem-focused reactions that help or encourage children to solve or cope with the problem (% ! .68); (d) minimizing reactions that diminish the seriousness of the situation or devalue the child’s problem or distressed reaction (% ! .74), and (e) reactions that encourage children to express negative affect or validate children’s negative emotional states (% ! .78). Two situations were modified to make them more applicable to Chinese families (e.g., “my child is sick and can’t go to his friend’s birthday party” was changed to “my child is sick and can’t go to the school’s field trip,” “If my child is playing with other children and one of them calls him/her names” was changed to “If my child is nervous about a test”). The item scores were averaged to create the composite for each subscale. In a study of primarily EuA children (Fabes et al., 2002), the CCNES showed satisfactory alpha reliabilities (.69#.85) and testretest reliabilities (.56 to .83 over 4 months), and predicted observed children’s emotional competence. Global parenting (W1). Global parenting was assessed using the authoritative and authoritarian scales of the Chinese version of the Parenting Styles and Dimensions scale (PSD; Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 1995). The authoritative scale includes: (a) warmth/acceptance (9 items, % ! .76); (b) reasoning/induction (9 items, % ! .70); (c) democratic participation (4 items, % ! .72); and (d) easy-going/responsiveness (4 items, % ! .70). The authoritarian scale includes: (a) non-reasoning/punitive strategies (4 items, % ! .61), (b) corporal punishment (5 items, % ! .80); (c) directiveness (4 items, % ! .49); and (d) verbal hostility (4 items, % ! .72). The dimensions of authoritative parenting were positively and moderately correlated with each other, rs(dfs ! 394 to 401) ranged from .51 to .63, as were the dimensions of authoritarian parenting, rs(dfs ! 394 to 400) ranged from .38 to .61. Thus, the subscale scores were averaged to form composites of authoritative (% ! .89) and authoritarian (% ! .82) parenting. Child externalizing problems (W1 and W2). At W1, parents (% ! .87) and teachers (% ! .96) rated children’s externalizing problems (from 1 ! never to 4 ! often) on the 24-item Child Behavior Checklist (CBC; Lochman & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 1995). Peer nominations of aggressive/disruptive behaviors were obtained using a 7-item subscale from the Chinese version of the Revised Class Play (Chen, Rubin, & Sun, 1992; Masten, Morison, & Pelligrini, 1985). Children were provided with a form containing 21 behavioral descriptors and a list of the names of all students in the class. After the research assistant read each descriptor, children nominated up to three classmates who could best play the role if they were to direct a class play. Nominations of all classmates were used to compute each item score for each child. The item scores were standardized within the class to adjust for differences in the number of nominators. A composite (% ! .92) was computed by averaging the item scores. At W2, parent-, teacher-, and child-reported externalizing problems were assessed with the externalizing subscale of the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 2001; % ! .84); Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach, 2001; % ! .92); and Behavior Problem Index (BPI; Peterson & Zill, 1986; % ! .76). The CBCL and TRF externalizing scales include two subscales: (a) rule-breaking behaviors, and (b) aggressive behaviors. The BPI externalizing scale includes 11 items, all of which are represented in the CBCL and TRF. Child internalizing problems (W2). At W2 only, the internalizing subscale of the CBCL (Achenbach, 2001; % ! .82), the TRF (Achenbach, 2001; % ! .84); and children’s self-reports of the BPI (Peterson et al., 1986; % ! .70) were used. The CBCL and TRF internalizing scales include three subscales: (a) anxious/depressed, (b) withdrawn/depressed, and (c) somatic complaints. The BPI internalizing scale includes 8 items, all of which are represented in the CBCL and TRF. Child social competence (W1 & W2). Parents and teachers completed a 4-item subscale from the adapted version of Harter’s Perceived Competence Scale for Children (HPCSC; Harter, 1979; Eisenberg et al., 1995), which assesses children’s socially appropriate behaviors. The items were rated on a 4-point scale (from 1 ! really false to 4 ! really true). The %s were .63 and .67 for parent reports at W1 and W2, and .63 and .84 for teacher reports at W1 and W2. At W1 only, peer nominations of child sociabilityleadership were obtained using the 14-item sociabilityleadership subscale (% ! .95 in our sample) in the Chinese version of the Revised Class Play (RCP; Chen et al., 1992; Masten et al., 1985). PARENTAL REACTIONS, NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, CHINESE Results Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. A stepwise approach was taken to test the hypotheses. First, zero-order correlations between PRCNE and W2 child adjustment and between PRCNE and global parenting were tested. Second, when significant zero-order correlations were found between a PRCNE and a child adjustment outcome at W2, structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted to examine whether the PRCNE predicted W2 child adjustment controlling for W1 adjustment (if available) and SES. Third, for the models indicating a significant prospective relation between one type of PRCNE and child adjustment outcome, W1 global parenting dimensions were added to test the unique predictions. Correlation Analyses W1 PRCNE and W2 child adjustment. W1 punitive reaction was positively correlated (r ! .12) with W2 parent report of externalizing problems, and negatively correlated (–.19) with W2 parent and teacher report of social competence (Table 2). W1 problem-focused reaction was negatively correlated (–.12) with W2 teacher report of internalizing problems. W1 emotion-focused reaction was negatively correlated (–.12) with W2 parent and child report of internalizing problems. W1 PRCNE and global parenting. Punitive reaction was negatively correlated (–.21) with authoritative parenting and positively correlated (.47) with authoritarian parenting. Problem- and emotion-focused reactions and encouragement of expression were positively correlated (.39 and .45) with authoritative parenting, and negatively correlated (#.24 and #.19) with authoritarian parenting. Minimizing 139 reaction was positively correlated (.30) with authoritarian parenting (Table 2). Predicting W2 Child Adjustment from W1 PRCNE: Structural Equation Modeling Based on the results of the correlation analyses, SEM was conducted to test the following models: a) W1 punitive reaction predicts W2 externalizing problems; b) W1 punitive reaction predicts W2 social competence; c) W1 problem-focused reaction relates to W2 internalizing problems; and d) W1 emotion-focused reaction relates to W2 internalizing problems. W1 SES and child adjustment (if available) were the covariates. The variables were screened for normality and outliers. Using the cutoffs of 2 and 7 (absolute value) for high skewness and kurtosis suggested by West, Finch, and Curran (1995), six variables were positively skewed (i.e., W1 peer nominations of aggression and leadership/ sociability, W2 parents’ and teachers’ reports of child externalizing and internalizing problems), and two variables had a high positive kurtosis (i.e., W1 peer nomination of aggression and W2 teacher report of externalizing problems). It is not uncommon for behavioral problem variables to be positively skewed in school-based samples. Because of the presence of non-normal variables, we conducted the SEM analyses using the MLR estimator of Mplus, which provides the standard errors and chi-square statistics for data with non-normal outcomes (the robust statistics, see Muthen & Muthen, 1998 –2008). In addition, Cook’s distance was used to screen the data for outliers, and no outliers were found using the cutoff of one (Cook, 1977; Stevens, 1984). Given that the data were clustered within school classrooms, Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for Study Variables For the whole sample For boys For girls Variable Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis Mean SD Mean SD PRCNE punitive W1 PRCNE problem-focused W1 PRCNE minimizing W1 PRCNE emotion-focused W1 PRCNE encouragement W1 Authoritative parenting W1 Authoritarian parenting W1 Externalizing problems (P) W1 Externalizing problems (T) W1 Externalizing problems (PE) W1 Externalizing problems (P) W2 Externalizing problems (T) W2 Externalizing problems (C) W2 Internalizing problems (P) W2 Internalizing problems (T) W2 Internalizing problems (C) W2 Social competence (P) W1 Social competence (T) W1 Social competence (PE) W1 Social competence (P) W2 Social competence (T) W2 3.10 5.57 4.33 5.64 4.63 3.76 2.30 2.07 1.66 #.04 .12 .09 .32 .16 .09 .30 3.15 2.90 .05 3.31 3.13 .86 .69 .98 .75 .93 .52 .46 .38 .56 .77 .14 .16 .27 .18 .14 .30 .51 .52 .79 .50 .65 .47 #.73 #.14 #.63 #.28 #.33 .73 #.02 .52 2.91 2.01 3.16 1.12 2.00 2.53 1.25 #.67 #.85 2.37 #.67 #.44 .10 1.01 .02 .74 .34 #.13 .77 #.04 #.61 9.24 5.12 11.92 1.08 4.63 8.00 1.13 .58 .31 6.39 .14 #.51 3.12 5.55 4.36 5.63 4.58 3.72 2.35 2.15 1.87 .28 .16 .13 .38 2.87 .11 .34 3.07 2.72 #.15 3.19 2.90 .85 .69 .97 .76 .88 .51 .51 .38 .58 1.03 .16 .19 .29 .65 .15 .33 .52 .58 .61 .54 .67 3.08 5.58 4.30 5.65 4.65 3.79 2.27 2.02 1.48 #.30 .09 .05 .26 3.06 .09 .26 3.22 3.04 .21 3.42 3.32 .87 .68 .99 .75 .97 .53 .42 .38 .47 .29 .10 .12 .23 .50 .13 .27 .51 .43 .88 .45 .58 Note. P ! parent report; T ! teacher report; PE ! peer report; C ! child report. 140 TAO, ZHOU, AND WANG Table 2 Correlations Among Study Variables 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. PUN-P1 PRO-P1 MIN-P1 EMO-P1 ENC-P1 AUT-P1 AUR-P1 EXT-P1 EXT-T1 EXT-PE1 EXT-P2 EXT-T2 EXT-C2 INT-P2 INT-T2 INT-C2 SC-P1 SC-T1 SC-PE1 SC-P2 SC-T2 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 — #.05 .53!!! #.10! .05 #.21!!! .47!!! .21!!! .04 .11 .12! .10 .07 .10 .06 .05 #.32!!! #.13! #.07 #.19!! #.13! — .17!! .65!!! .54!!! .43!!! #.24!!! #.07 #.02 .04 #.02 #.02 #.03 #.07 #.12! #.10 .19!!! .01 .05 .08 .01 — .17!! .23!!! .05 .30!!! .15!! .00 #.02 .03 .04 .02 #.01 .00 #.06 #.09 #.03 #.02 #.07 #.06 — .49!!! .45!!! #.23!!! #.11! #.03 .03 #.09 #.01 #.04 #.11! #.09 #.12! .21!!! #.02 .08 .09 .07 — .39!!! #.19!!! #.11! .02 #.02 .00 .01 #.02 #.07 #.08 #.03 .13!! #.04 .05 .03 #.09 — #.18!!! #.11! #.02 #.07 #.22!!! #.06 #.13! #.21!!! #.13! #.16!! .32!!! .06 .11! .25!!! .12! — .37!!! .12! .04 .26!!! .16!! .14! .19!!! .14! .09 #.37!!! #.20!!! #.15!! #.27!!! #.14! — .22!!! .11! .29!!! .14!! .21!!! .12! .11! .15!! #.42!!! #.15!! #.06 #.29!!! #.21!!! — .43!!! .32!!! .34!!! .21!!! .21!!! #.01 .14!! #.23!!! #.39!!! #.13! #.29!!! #.31!!! Note. PUN ! Punitive reactions; PRO ! Problem-focused reactions; MIN ! Minimizing reactions; EMO ! Emotion-focused reactions; ENC ! encouragement of emotion expression; AUT ! authoritative parenting; AUR ! authoritarian parenting; EXT ! externalizing problems; INT ! internalizing problems; SC ! social competence; p ! parent report; T ! teacher report; C ! child report; PE ! peer nomination; 1 ! Wave 1; 2 ! Wave 2. ! p $ .05. !! p $ .01. !!! p $ .001. the standard errors of parameter estimates and the chisquare test of model fit were computed using a special feature in Mplus 5.2 (Muthen & Muthen, 1998 –2008), which takes into account non-independence of observation. In Mplus maximum likelihood estimation, missing data due to attrition are allowed but missing values are not imputed; rather, the method uses all information that is available to estimate the model (Muthen & Muthen, 1998 –2008). The model predicting externalizing from punitive reactions fit the data well (Figure 1, Model A), &2(df ! 13, N ! 423) ! 20.4, p " .05, comparative fit index (CFI) ! .97, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) ! .037, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) ! .038. All the modelestimated loadings were significant in the positive direction. The autoregressive path for externalizing problems was significant in the positive direction. W1 SES negatively predicted W2 externalizing. As predicted, punitive reaction positively predicted W2 externalizing. The model in which child social competence is predicted by punitive reaction fit the data well, &2(df ! 7, N ! 423) ! 12.6, p " .05, CFI ! .97, RMSEA ! .043, SRMR ! .021. However, W1 punitive reaction did not uniquely predict W2 social competence. Because internalizing problems were not assessed at W1, it could not be controlled in the analyses. The model relating W1 problem-focused reaction with W2 internalizing problems fit the data well (Figure 1, Model B), &2(df ! 4, N ! 423) ! 4.75, p " .05, CFI ! .99, RMSEA ! .021, SRMR ! .025. The model-estimated loadings were at least marginally significant. Controlling for W1 SES, W1 problem-focused reaction was negatively related to W2 internalizing problems. Finally, the model relating parental emotion-focused reaction to W2 internalizing problems also fit the data well (Figure 1, Model C), &2(df ! 4, N ! 423) ! 1.73, p " .05, CFI ! 1.00, RMSEA ! .000, SRMR ! .016. Emotion-focused reaction was negatively related to internalizing problems when controlling for W1 SES.1,2 1 Because some researchers (Robert & Strayer, 1987) suggested that the relations between PRCNE and children’s adjustment might be curvilinear such that optimal child adjustment is associated with moderate (but not high) levels of supportive parental reactions, we also tested the quadratic relations between PRCNE and Chinese children’s adjustment using hierarchical regression analyses. The dependent variables were children’s W2 externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and social competence (averaged across reporters), respectively. The independent variables were entered in three steps: a) the covariates (child sex, age, family SES, and if available, the corresponding W1 adjustment), b) the linear term for a PRCNE, and c) the quadratic term of PRCNE. All the predictors and covariates were centered. None of the quadratic terms of PRCNE were significant, suggesting that a curvilinear relation between PRCNE and child adjustment was not detected. 2 To examine the unique relations of different types of PRCNE to multiple types of child adjustment outcomes, we also tested an omnibus model in which we predicted W2 child externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and social competence simultaneously from the five types of PRCNE. The relations of family SES and corresponding W1 adjustment factor on W2 adjustment factors were controlled. Although this omnibus model fit the data adequately, &2(df ! 75) ! 137.9, p $ .001, CFI ! .94, RMSEA ! .044, SRMR ! .067, none of the paths from W1 PRCNE to W2 adjustment factor were significant. Because different types of PRCNEs were moderately correlated with each other (see Table 2), these results suggested that they likely overlapped with each other in their relations to children’s adjustment. PARENTAL REACTIONS, NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, CHINESE 141 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 — .26!!! .42!!! .25!!! .13! #.06 .09 #.18!! #.42!!! .01 #.30!!! #.33!!! — .29!!! .46!!! .69!!! .13! .27!!! #.26!!! #.30!!! #.21!!! #.48!!! #.31!!! — .17!! .19!!! .46!!! .05 #.05 #.33!!! #.11! #.21!!! #.53!!! — .36!!! .06 .49!!! #.18!! #.26!!! #.18!! #.34!!! #.21!!! — .15!! .32!!! #.25!!! #.21!!! #.17!! #.32!!! #.20!!! — .12! #.09 #.08 #.13! #.11! #.32!!! — #.22!!! #.18!! #.17!! #.31!!! #.13!!! — .25!!! .24!!! .45!!! .19!!! — .32!!! .37!!! .43!!! — .31!!! .32!!! — .31!!! Testing the Unique Relations of PRCNE and Global Parenting to Child Adjustment We added W1 authoritative and authoritarian parenting as additional predictors into the models in Figure 1. Because punitive reaction did not predict child social competence, the extended model was not tested. The extended models for Models A, B, and C fit the data well, &2s(dfs ! 20, 8, and 8, Ns ! 423) ! 26.5, 5.14, and 4.50, ps " .05; CFIs ! .98, 1.00, and 1.00; RMSEAs ! .028, .000, and .000; SRMRs ! .035, .025, and .021. However, W1 PRCNE no longer predicted W2 child adjustment after controlling for W1 global parenting. Instead, W1 authoritative parenting had a unique and negative relation, and W1 authoritarian parenting had a unique and positive relation to W2 externalizing and internalizing problems. Discussion As one of first studies on parental reactions to children’s emotions in a non-Western sample, this study has several strengths. First, because multiple domains of adjustment were assessed from multiple-reporters, the study conducted a relatively objective test of the relations between PRCNE and child adjustment. Second, the longitudinal design allowed a more stringent test of the directional relations between PRCNE and child adjustment than those in crosssectional studies. Third, we also tested the relations between PRCNE and global parenting and their unique relations to child adjustment. Finally, while previous studies on PRCNE focused on toddlers or preschoolers, this study examined PRCNE for school-aged children. Parental Punitive, Problem-Focused, and Emotion-Focused Reactions Chinese parents’ punitive reactions positively predicted children’s externalizing problems 4 years later. This relation was robust after controlling for SES and baseline problems. Moreover, punitive reactions were negatively correlated with Chinese children’s social competence at W2, although it did not predict social competence controlling for baseline competence. This might be due to the high rank-order consistency in social competence over time (as indicated by the high autoregressive path coefficient). It is also possible that the relation between punitive reaction and social competence becomes established by middle-childhood and remains stable from middle to late childhood. Overall, the findings on punitive reactions are similar to previous findings based on Western samples. In two Western studies of similar child age group that used comparable measures of PRCNE and child adjustment as our study, parental punitive reactions were positively related to children’s problem behaviors and negatively related to social competence (Eisenberg et al., 1996, 1999; Jones et al., 2002). We also found that Chinese parents’ problem- and emotion-focused reactions were negatively related to internalizing problems 4 years later. However, because we did not measure internalizing problems at W1, baseline internalizing problems could not be controlled. Thus, the 142 TAO, ZHOU, AND WANG PRCNE Punitive Reactions W1 .07* Family SES W1 Parent Teacher -.19*** .28 .74*** .56*** Parent .86 Child Externalizing Problems W1 .54*** Child Externalizing Problems W2 .36*** Teacher .52*** Child Peer Model A. Predicting externalizing problems from parental punitive reactions. PRCNE EmotionFocused Reactions W1 Family SES W1 -.19*** -.30*** Child Internalizing Problems W2 Parent .70 .22** Teacher .47*** Child Model B. Predicting internalizing problems from parental emotion-focused reactions. PRCNE ProblemFocused Reactions W1 -.17* Parent .68 -.30 *** Family SES W1 Child Internalizing Problems W2 .23 + Teacher .48** * Child Model C. Predicting internalizing problems from parental problem-focused reactions. Figure 1. The models predicting W2 child adjustment from W1 PRCNE. Note. The numbers are standardized loadings or path coefficients. ! p $ .05. !! p $ .01. !!! p $ .001. PRCNE-internalizing relations might be overestimated. Although we did not find a comparable Western study that examined PRCNE and internalizing problems using similar measures as our study, the pattern of associations of problem- and emotion-focused reactions in our sample was consistent with what would be expected from Western samples. For example, emotion- and problem-focused reactions were positively related to EuA children’s emotional decoding, attention and anger control skills (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994; Fabes et al., 2002). It is important to note that the size of the significant correlations between PRCNE and child adjustment found in the present sample were in the small to medium range, which is not surprising given the 4-year time interval of the study. In fact, the size of correlations was generally similar to those in a longitudinal study of EuA children (Eisenberg et al., 1999). Moreover, there were more within-reporter than across-reporter PRCNC-adjustment correlations (although there were a few cross-reporter correlations), which is also similar to the results from Western studies (Eisenberg et al., 1996, 1999; Jones et al., 2002). The findings on the relations between punitive, problemfocused, and emotion-focused reactions and global parenting are also consistent with Western theory. Chinese par- ents’ punitive reaction was positively related to their authoritarian parenting and negatively related to authoritative parenting, whereas the opposite relations were found for problem- and emotion-focused reactions. Thus, punitive reactions to children’s negative emotions are consistent with the style of harsh and coercive discipline, and problem- and emotion-focused reactions are consistent with the style of warm, responsive, and reasoning-focused discipline in Chinese families. Together, these findings suggest that there are cross-cultural similarities in the meanings of punitive, problem-, and emotion-focused reactions and their socialization functions. Parental Minimizing Reactions and Encouragement of Emotion Expression In contrast to the Western studies that showed negative relations between parental minimizing reactions and children’s social competence, and positive relations between encouragement of expression and constructive coping and social competence (Eisenberg et al., 1996, 1999; Jones et al., 2002), we did not find any relation between minimizing reactions or encouragement of emotion expression and Chinese children’s adjustment. Our sample had sufficient PARENTAL REACTIONS, NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, CHINESE power (".95) to detect small to moderate size correlations. The measure for PRCNE showed comparable reliabilities across subscales. Descriptive statistics showed greater variability in these two types of PRCNE than others. Thus, the lack of association is unlikely to be fully explained by power, measurement error, or restricted range of variability. These results likely suggest there are some cultural differences in the meanings and socialization functions of minimizing reactions and encouragement of expression. It is interesting to note that although the Chinese parents who reported more minimizing reactions were higher on authoritarian parenting, they were not necessarily lower on authoritative parenting, which further suggests ambiguity in the meaning of minimizing reactions. Because inhibition of emotion expression is considered adaptive in Eastern cultures (Matsumoto et al., 2008), if parents use minimizing reactions (e.g., “it is not a big deal”) to make children feel better, they might be beneficial for children’s emotion regulation and adjustment. In contrast, if parents use minimizing reactions to dismiss or punish children’s negative emotions, they might be detrimental to children’s adjustment. Thus, perhaps it is not minimizing reaction per se but its socialization goal that determines its implications for child adjustment. Future research should investigate this hypothesis. The Chinese parents who reported more encouragement of expression also reported higher authoritative parenting and lower authoritarian parenting, suggesting that encouragement of expression is generally consistent with warm and supportive parenting in Chinese families. Contrary to Western findings, the Chinese children who are shy, inhibited, and reserved (who likely have lower expressivity) were found to score higher on social competence and adjustment than their peers (Chen, Dong, & Zhou, 1997). Thus, it is possible that parental encouragement of expression positively predicts adjustment if the child has low or moderate expressivity, whereas the relation might be reversed if the child is highly expressive. Because children’s expressivity was not assessed in the present study, we were unable to examine the moderation hypothesis. The Unique Relations of PRCNE and Global Parent Styles to Child Adjustment None of the PRCNE uniquely predicted adjustment controlling for global parenting. Instead, authoritative and authoritarian parenting uniquely predicted adjustment controlling for PRCNE. The finding that PRCNE overlapped with global parenting in their prediction of child adjustment suggests that the developmental mechanisms that underlie the socialization influence of PRCNE (e.g., children’s emotion regulation, emotion understanding, and expression, Eisenberg et al., 1998) might also contribute to the socialization influence of global parenting. However, the result that global parenting (but not PRCNE) had unique relations to child adjustment suggests that other developmental mechanisms (e.g., child neurocognitive processes and selfesteem) should be investigated in order to fully explain the socialization influence of global parenting. 143 Implications for Intervention and Limitations Teaching parents how to respond to children’s emotions is a widely used intervention strategy for improving parentchild relationship (e.g., Wolchik et al., 2000). The present findings have implications for adapting these interventions for families of Chinese origin. Specifically, our findings suggest that the intervention components targeting parental problem- and emotion-focused and punitive reactions are applicable to Chinese families. In contrast, the intervention components that discourage parental minimizing reactions and/or promote parental encouragement of expression should be modified for Chinese families because of the ambiguity in their socialization functions. The study has several limitations. First, the small number of children who dropped out after W1 were more likely to come from higher-SES families. However, our attrition rate is low (10% across 4 years), and no attrition-related group difference was found on child adjustment. Thus, it is unlikely that the attrition resulted in significant alternation of the study sample (which represented primarily working- to middle-class families living in industrialized urban China). Second, PRCNE were only assessed by parent report, which might not fully capture parents’ actual behaviors in real-life situations. Future research should assess PRCNE from multiple perspectives and with multiple methods. Third, PRCNE were primarily reported by mothers. Because researchers have shown differences between mothers’ and fathers’ PRCNE, future studies should aim to include reports by both parents. Fourth, because baseline internalizing problems were not assessed and could not be controlled in the analyses, the models might have overestimated the relations between PRCNE and internalizing problems. Fifth, because the instrument for PRCNE was adapted from a Western measure, it might not tap the parental reactions that are unique to Chinese families. Finally, because different PRCNE were tested in separate socialization models (rather than one omnibus model simultaneously including all dimensions), the study might have failed to capture the interrelated features of PRCNE, and how parents’ patterns of engaging in an array of PRCNE might predict children’s adjustment. References Achenbach, T. M. (2001). The Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Baumrind, D. (1996). The discipline controversy revisited. Family Relations, 45, 405– 414. Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 228 –283. Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111–1119. Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Development, 72, 1832–1843. Chen, X., Dong, Q., & Zhou, H. (1997). Authoritative and Au- 144 TAO, ZHOU, AND WANG thoritarian parenting practices and social and school performance in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 855– 873. Chen, X., & French, D. C. (2008). Children’s social competence in cultural context. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 591– 616. Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Sun, Y. (1992). Social reputation and peer relationships in Chinese and Canadian children: A crosscultural study. Child Development, 63, 1336 –1343. Cook, R. D. (1977). Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics, 19, 15–19. Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487– 496. Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 65– 86. Dodge, K. A., Petit, F. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization mediators of the relation between socioeconomic status and child conduct problems. Child Development, 65, 649 – 65. Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1994). Mothers’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s temperament and anger behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 138 – 156. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67, 2227–2247. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66, 1239 –1261. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child Development, 70, 513–534. Fabes, R. A., Poulin, R. E., Eisenberg, N., & Madden-Derdich, D. A. (2002). The Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES): Psychometric properties and relations with children’s emotional competence. Marriage & Family Review, 34, 285–310. Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Harter, S. (1979). Perceived competence scale for children: Manual. Denver: University of Denver. Holden, G. W., & Miller, P. C. (1999). Enduring and different: A meta-analysis of the similarity in parents’ child rearing. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 223–254. Jones, S., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2002). Parents’ reactions to elementary school children’s negative emotions: Relations to social and emotional functioning at school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 133–159. Lochman, J. E., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1995). Screening of child behavior problems for prevention programs at school entry. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 549 –559. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224 –253. Masten, A., Morison, P., & Pelligrini, D. (1985). A revised class play method of peer assessment. Child Development, 21, 523– 533. Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Fontaine, J., Anguas-Wong, A. M., Arriola, M., Ataca, B., Bond, M. H., et al. (2008). Mapping expressive differences around the world: The relationship between emotional display rules and individualism versus collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 55–74. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998 –2008). Mplus: Statistical Analyses with Latent Variables. User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. National Bureau of Statistics of China. (2000). China Statistical Yearbook 2000. Beijing, China: China Statistics Press. Peterson, J., & Zill, N. (1986). Marital disruption, parent-child relationships, and behavior problems in children. Journal of the Marriage & the Family, 48, 295–307. Power, T. G. (2004). Stress and coping in childhood: The parents’ role. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 271–317. Roberts, W. L., & Strayer, J. (1987). Parents’ responses to the emotional distress of their children: Relations with children’s competence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 415– 422. Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, F. S., & Hart, H. C. (1995). Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Development of a new measure. Psychological Reports, 77, 819 – 830. Sorkhabi, N. (2005). Applicability of Baumrind’s parent typology to collective cultures: Analysis of cultural explanations of parent socialization effects. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 552–563. Soto, J. A., Levenson, R. W., & Ebling, R. (2005). Cultures of moderation and expression: Emotional experience, behavior, and physiology in Chinese Americans and Mexican Americans. Emotion, 5, 154 –165. Stevens, J. P. (1984). Outliers and influential data points in regression analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 334 –344. West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 56 –75). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Wolchik, S. A., West, S. G., Sandler, I. N., Tein, J., Coatsworth, D., Lengua, L., Weiss, L., et al. (2000). An experimental evaluation of theory-based mother and mother-child programs for children of divorce. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 843– 856. Zahn-Waxler, C., Friedman, R. J., Cole, P. M., Mizuta, I., & Hiruma, N. (1996). Japanese and United States preschool children’s responses to conflict and distress. Child Development, 67, 2462–2477. Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Wang, Y., & Reiser, M. (2004). Chinese children’s effortful control and dispositional anger/frustration: Relations to parenting styles and children’s social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 352–366. Zhou, Q., Wang, Y., Deng, X., Eisenberg, N., Wolchik, S. A., & Tein, J. (2008). Relations of parenting and temperament to Chinese children’s experience of negative life events, coping efficacy, and externalizing problems. Child Development, 79, 493– 513. Received April 29, 2009 Revision received December 22, 2009 Accepted January 3, 2010 !