See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/42798583 Communicative Competence Article · January 2005 DOI: 10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/01275-X · Source: OAI CITATIONS READS 80 29,491 1 author: Theresa Lillis The Open University (UK) 114 PUBLICATIONS 6,718 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Theresa Lillis on 02 January 2023. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. 1 Communicative Competence Theresa Lillis Theresa.Lillis@open.ac.uk Pre-published version 2005 Published version: Lillis, Theresa (2006) Communicative competence. in K. Brown (ed) Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics. Oxford: Elsevier. ISBN: 13: 978-0-08-044299-0. Pages 666-673. Pdf version of Encyclopedia available at https://www.pdfdrive.com/encyclopedia-oflanguage-and-linguistics-d158774309.html, November 2020 The phrase ‘communicative competence’ was introduced by the North American linguist and anthropologist, Dell Hymes, in the late 1960s (Hymes, 1962/1968, 1971). He used it to reflect the following key positions on knowledge and use of language: • • • • The ability to use a language well involves knowing (either explicitly or implicitly) how to use language appropriately in any given context. The ability to speak and understand language is not based solely on grammatical knowledge. What counts as appropriate language varies according to context and may involve a range of modes – for example, speaking, writing, singing, whistling, drumming. Learning what counts as appropriate language occurs through a process of socialization into particular ways of using language through participation in particular communities. Hymes’s juxtaposition of the word ‘communicative’ with ‘competence’ stood in sharp contrast at the time with Noam Chomsky’s influential use of the term ‘linguistic competence,’ which Chomsky used to refer to a native speaker’s implicit knowledge of the grammatical rules governing her/his language (Chomsky, 1957, 1965). Such knowledge, Chomsky argued, enables speakers to create new and grammatically correct sentences and accounts for the fact that speakers are able to recognize grammatically incorrect as well as correct sentences such as, in English She book the read, or in Spanish plaza yo a la voy (‘square I am going to’). While accepting the importance of grammatical knowledge, Hymes argued that in order to communicate effectively, speakers had to know not only what was grammatically correct/incorrect, but what was communicatively appropriate in any given context. A speaker therefore must possess more than just grammatical knowledge; for example, a multilingual speaker in a multilingual context knows which language to use in which context and users of a language where there are both formal and informal forms of address know when to use which, such as vous (formal) and tu (informal) in French. Hymes famously stated that a child who produced language without due regard for the social context would be a monster (1974b: 75). The emphasis that Hymes placed on appropriateness according to context, in his use of the term competence, challenged Chomsky’s view about what exactly counts as knowledge of a 2 language – knowledge of conventions of use in addition to knowledge of grammatical rules. In addition, and more fundamentally, Hymes problematized the dichotomy advanced by Chomsky between ‘competence’ and ‘performance’ and the related claim about what the study of linguistics proper should be. Chomsky’s interest was in the universal psycholinguistics of language, the human capacity for generating the syntactic rules of language. His interest in knowledge, captured in his use of ‘competence,’ was therefore at an ideal or abstract level rather than in any actual knowledge that any one speaker or group of speakers might possess. For Chomsky, the focus of linguistics as a discipline should be on understanding and describing the general and abstract principles that make the human capacity for language possible. In contrast, ‘performance’ or actual utterances – that is, what people actually say and hear with all the errors, false starts, unfinished sentences – could add little to an understanding of the principles underlying language use and was therefore not deemed to be a relevant focus of linguistic study. Hymes acknowledged the value of the more abstract and idealized approach that Chomsky advocated, not least because such a universalistic approach challenged any theories of language based on genetic differences or notions of racial hierarchy (Hymes, 1971: 4). However, he argued that there were other important dimensions to the study of language that should not be so readily excluded from linguistics as a scientific field. Hymes’s own interest in language was in large part driven by a concern for language questions arising in real life contexts, such as why children from economically advantaged and disadvantaged social backgrounds differ in the language they use. Chomsky’s and Hymes’s different aims for developing language theory are nowhere more clearly evident than in Hymes’s comment on Chomksy’s (1965: 3) now famous statement, on the purpose of linguistic theory: ‘‘Linguistic theory is concerned primarily with an ideal speaker-listener, in a completely homogenous speech-community, who knows its language perfectly.. . .’’ Hymes (1971:4) comments: ‘‘The theoretical notion of the ideal speaker-listener is unilluminating from the standpoint of the children we seek to understand and to help.’’ Hymes was highly critical of a theory that explicitly set out to ignore the impact of social context on how language is used and hence the competence/performance dichotomy set up by Chomsky (echoing in some ways the langue and parole distinction made by Saussure, 1916). At a specific level, his key reasons for challenging such a dichotomy can be summarized as follows (based on Hymes, 1962/1968; 1971; 1974b): • The dichotomy itself is problematic. It presupposes that knowledge can be understood without reference to use, yet analyzing actual use of language is key to exploring underlying principles for such use. Hymes argued that ‘‘performance data’’ should be considered a legitimate focus for linguistic study both in its own right and as data that reflects knowledge underlying any performance. • The dichotomy is built on a series of abstractions: ideal speaker-listener, homogenous speech community, perfect knowledge of language. • Chomsky’s notion of speaker-listener does not acknowledge or account for the differences in reception competence and production competence evident in many contexts, as in children from some social backgrounds understanding formal school language yet not producing it. 3 • • • What counts as knowledge of language is reduced to only one aspect of knowledge, namely grammatical knowledge, when there are clearly other aspects to knowledge of language that are important, such as when to use which language, or varieties of languages, and in which contexts. Within an approach that focuses on competence as idealized knowledge, it is the abstract system of language that becomes the focus rather than speakers’/groups of speakers’ use of language. Given the focus on knowledge as a set of abstract rules underlying use, actual use is relegated to only a marginal position in the scientific study of language. Hymes (1972a: 282) offers communicative competence as a more general and superordinate term to encompass the language capabilities of the individual that include both knowledge and use: ‘‘competence is dependent upon both (tacit) knowledge and (ability for) use.’’ While Hymes argued against the foundational dichotomy between competence and performance proposed by Chomsky, he was not dismissing the value of the distinction entirely. Hymes refers to communicative competence as ‘‘abilities in a broad sense’’ of how to use language, whereas performance is always a specific use of language that reflects some of that competence (2003: 321). Thus any specific performance may partially reflect the nature of the conventions governing an individual or a community’s knowledge of language. In setting up a framework for developing an adequate theory of language, Hymes argued that both what is known (competence) and what is actually done (performance) must be taken into account. Such a framework involves exploring and accounting for the following: 1. Whether (and to what degree) something is formally possible 2. Whether (and to what degree) something is feasible 3. Whether (and to what degree) something is appropriate 4. Whether (and to what degree) something is in fact done, actually performed (Hymes 1972a: 284–286). Questions 3 and 4 are central to the socially oriented approach to the study of language advocated by Hymes. In contrast to Chomsky and his claim to linguistics as a subfield of psychology and philosophy, Hymes seeks to claim a space for the study of language within ‘‘a science of social man’’ (Hymes, 1971: 6). A Key Concept in an Emerging Sociolinguistic Tradition Emphasis on the notion of communicative competence formed part of Dell Hymes’s call for a new field of study, the ethnography of communication, sometimes called the ethnography of speaking (Hymes, 1962/1968; Gumperz and Hymes, 1972/1986). There are a number of concepts and categories presupposed by the notion of communicative competence, which continue to be highly influential in sociolinguistics and in many socially oriented approaches to study of language. Sociocultural Context Given the importance attached to knowledge of the social conventions governing language use, understanding the context of language use is considered to be central. Exploring such 4 context, that is, the cultural, historical, and social practices associated with the language use of any particular group or community of people, involves detailed descriptions and classification of language use organized around the following key questions. What are the communicative events, and their components, in a community? What are the relationships among them? What capabilities and status do they have, in general and in particular cases? How do they work? (Hymes, 1974b: 25). Ethnography of Communication In order to explore how language is used in context, Hymes argued for an ethnographic approach to the study of communication or ways of speaking (Hymes, 1974a). This involves researchers setting out to systematically observe the activities of any given community, through immersing themselves in such activities and collecting a range of data, such as recordings, field notes, and documentation. In this methodology both ‘etic’ and ‘emic’ approaches are considered important and complementary; the etic approach refers to observation from the outside as it were, that is, the researcher seeks to observe in detail the communicative activities – or speech events – of participants in a community; the emic involves exploring such events, from the inside, to determine how participants make sense of and understand such events and interactions. Ethnographers emphasize the importance of emic accounts to any theory of language; for example, only an emic perspective would enable a researcher to understand that a clap of thunder may in some cultural contexts be considered to be a communicative act (as in the case of the Ojibwa reported by Hymes, 1974b: 13), or that certain types of communication are permitted to men in some contexts while proscribed in others, such as the disciplining of children (as reported by Philipsen, 1975). In an attempt to build a descriptive framework of how language is used in different contexts, Hymes, drawing on anthropologists such as Malinowski(1923, 1935), developed a series of categories to map out the relevant contextual aspects to language use, such as speech event and speech community. Speech Event This is a category (after Jakobson, 1960) that reflects the idea that all interaction is embedded in sociocultural contexts and is governed by conventions emerging from those contexts. Examples of speech events are interviews, buying and selling goods in a shop, sermons, lectures, and informal conversation. The speech event involves a number of core components identified by Hymes, which are signaled in his mnemonic device SPEAKING. [See Table 1]. Table 1 SPEAKING – acronym invented by Dell Hymes (1972b) to specify relevant features of a speech event S-settings and scenes. Setting refers to time, place, physical circumstances. Scene refers to the psychological or cultural definitions of the event: for example what ‘counts’ as a formal event varies from community to community. P-participants Who is involved, as either speaker/ listener, audience. 5 E-ends Ends can be defined in terms of goals and outcomes. Goals refer to what is expected to be achieved in any event: outcomes refers to what is actually achieved. Goals and outcomes exist at both community and individual participant level: for example, the conventional goal of a wedding ceremony may be marriage, however, individuals within that event may have other goals. A-acts Speech events involve a number and range of speech acts, particular types of utterances such as requests, commands, and greetings. K-keys The tone, manner, and spirit in which acts are done, for example, serious or playful. Specific keys may be signaled through verbal or/and non-verbal means. I-instrumentalities The particular language/language varieties used and the mode of communication (spoken, written). N-norms Norms of interaction refer to rules of speaking, who can say what, when, and how. Norms of interpretation refer to the conventions surrounding how any speech may be interpreted. G-genres Categories or types of language use, such as the sermon, the interview, or the editorial. May be the same as ‘speech event’ but may be a part of a speech event. For example, the sermon is a genre and may at the same time be a speech event (when performed conventionally in a church); a sermon may be a genre, however, that is invoked in another speech event, for example, at a party for humorous effect. Speech Community While the term speech community was not coined by Hymes (the most notable earlier use being that of Bloomfield, 1933), Hymes’s elaboration of the term certainly contributed to its prominence in sociolinguistic approaches to the study of language. The acquisition of communicative competence takes place within speech communities: speech communities are constituted not just by a shared variety or language, but shared sets of norms and conventions about how those varieties can and should be used. Through everyday interaction with others in a speech community, a child learns how to use language appropriately, that is, according to the norms of any given speech community. Some events inevitably involve people from different speech communities, which may create tensions: as in for example school classrooms where participants share a common language but may not be members of the same speech community (Hymes, 1972c). Diversity Acknowledgement of diversity and variety between and across language use, in communities and individuals, is a basic position in Hymes’s work and is a central tenet in sociolinguistics. Such diversity manifests itself in countless ways: the very existence of language varieties, both as languages and varieties within languages; the range of conventions governing the use of such varieties in different contexts (such differences have been documented in relation, notably, to social class, ethnic group, gender); the 6 different values attached to particular usages (for example, the values attached in different communities to such phenomena as silence, eloquence, and interruptions). Privileging diversity as a universal of language shifts the emphasis away from any differential status attached to varieties, or the notion that difference signals deficiency in any way. All varieties are seen as equally valid, although some are acknowledged to be more appropriate in particular contexts. Appropriateness This is a key presupposition to the notion of communicative competence and is a central notion in sociolinguistics. As discussed, communicative competence presupposes the following; that a language user’s knowledge – competence – is more than just grammarbased; that knowledge of language requires knowledge of the appropriate social conventions governing what and how something can be said, to whom and in what contexts. Appropriateness thus involves both linguistic and cultural knowledge (Hymes, 1971: 14). Within sociolinguistics, a focus on appropriateness of language use is said to indicate a descriptive (how language is used) rather than a prescriptive (how language should be used) approach to language diversity. Socialization People learn the rules of use through everyday interaction within speech communities. It is through such interaction that children acquire knowledge about appropriate language use, that is, communicative competence (Hymes, 1971: 10). Hymes indicates that socialization is not constituted by a rigid trajectory and suggests that both ‘‘a long and short range view of competency should be adopted’’ (1972a: 287). From his perspective, the short range view concerns innate capacities as they emerge in the first years of life, and the long range concerns continuing socialization through life. What this short/long range implies is that competence is not static. In some instances, quite drastic changes can be made to an individuals’ competence; as when a child whose home language variety is significantly different from the school variety. Of course, as Hymes emphasizes, such extensions or shifts in competence are not necessarily straightforward; there are plenty of opportunities for misunderstanding to occur when receivers/listeners accustomed to the language varieties of one community engage in communication with those from another. Communicative Competence in Other Domains The notion of communicative competence has been highly influential in fields beyond linguistics, such as education, sociology, and psychology. In some instances the basic assumptions surrounding the term have been maintained, and in others extended or problematized. Probably nowhere has the impact of the notion been more powerful than in the teaching of languages, including the teaching of English as a second or foreign language. Whereas the emphasis in language teaching had been on grammatical and syntactic accuracy, following the work of Hymes and others (Gumperz and Hymes, 1972/1986), there was a significant turn towards communicative language teaching: this shift involved the teaching and learning of language considered to be appropriate to specific situations, based on what speakers actually use, rather than what they are 7 presumed to use (Paulston, 1992). Assessment of language learning has been influenced accordingly, with a focus on students’ capacity to communicate, rather than the ability to produce grammatically correct sentences (Hall and Eggington, 2000). The extent to which this more situational approach to second and foreign language teaching prevails is a matter of debate, but the impact of communicative competence is widely acknowledged (Firth and Wagner, 1997). The use of the term has also been extended and modulated in other domains. For example, Culler (1975) developed the influential notion of literary competence to describe readers’ knowledge of the conventions required in order to interpret literary texts. Academic communicative competence has been used to refer to knowledge of the conventions governing the use of language in academic communication (Berkenkotter et al., 1991). Both uses refer to knowledge of specific textual features, such as metaphor in the case of literary competence and argument in academic competence, as well as knowledge about what counts as specific text types or genres (academic, literary) in particular cultural contexts. Other uses of ‘communicative competence’ have developed, alongside and in contradistinction to the Hymesian term. Habermas (1970) uses the term communicative competence more in line with Chomsky’s linguistic competence, to the extent that he is interested in theorizing an ideal speech situation, rather than elaborating a sociolinguistic description of actual situations and utterances. In contrast, Bernstein’s interest was in an elaboration of actual use of language, particularly within the context of schooling. However, he offered a critique of the way in which ‘competence’ models implied an exaggerated capacity of individual rational choice and control over language use, without due attention to ‘‘distribution of power and principles of control which selectively specialize modes of acquisition and realizations’’ (Bernstein, 1996: 56). The need to theorize power in relation to competence and language use is a key strand in other studies re-examining the notion of communicativecompetence in more recent times. Re-examining Communicative Competence The work of Hymes is central in sociolinguistics as a field and continues to reverberate across socially oriented approaches to the study of language in a range of disciplines, including applied linguistics, education, communication studies, and social psychology. In recent times, there have also been significant re-examinations of communicative competence and related notions, as they have come to be used in sociolinguistics, from both critical and post structuralist approaches. Re-examining Appropriateness The notion of appropriateness is central to communicative competence and central to the field of sociolinguistics whose empirical goal has been to explore patterns of language use, according to the norms of any given community. However, the use of such a notion has been critiqued by some because it serves to emphasize norms and underplay differences within any given community or communicative context. 8 Fairclough (1995), for example, like Bernstein mentioned above, argues that a model of language based on appropriateness assumes shared views among all users about what counts as appropriate, ignoring struggles and tensions in any given interaction; for example, tensions evident in interactions between institutional representatives and clients, men and women, or speakers from different cultural and linguistic backgrounds. Research in some socially oriented approaches to language, such as feminist linguistics and critical discourse analysis, has made visible the power dynamics in communicative events, within and across communities (Cameron, 1992; Wodak, 1992; Chouliaraki and Fairclough, 1999). In the same vein, emphasis on a normative notion of communicative competence in second and foreign language teaching has been critiqued by theorists of second language acquisition. Norton (2000) states that although it is important for learners to understand the conventions of the target language, it is also important for them to explore ‘‘whose interests these rules serve’’ (2000: 15). She argues that any definition of communicative competence should include an acknowledgement of the importance of the right to speak (Bourdieu, 1977); such a right to speak, or be heard, is not granted to all speakers in all contexts. Thus for example, immigrants using a foreign language may find that, although familiar with the conventions governing a particular use of that language, they may not be granted the right to speak or be heard in some contexts. Re-examining Speech Event and Speech Community While Hymes always indicated that he used the word ‘speech’ to mean all types of communicative modes/channels, sociolinguistic research has tended to focus on the spoken word. In more recent times, explicit attention has been paid to other modes of communication, thus extending the use of core concepts. For example, those working within literacy studies have used existing terms to signal a specific focus, such as ‘‘writing event’’ (Basso, 1974), ‘‘literacy event’’ (Heath, 1983; Barton and Hamilton, 1998). Likewise, Swales (1990) has argued that the term discourse community is more useful than speech community, as a term for describing and accounting for practices around written texts. Some theorists have argued that the word ‘speech’ signals that language is considered more significant than other practices, or that language is somehow divorced from other social purposes and activities, and have argued that the notion of practice, including the notion of ‘‘community of practice’’ is more all encompassing and powerful (Eckert and McConnellGinet, 2003; see also discussion about ways in which ‘practice’ is used in Schultz and Hull, 2002). A more fundamental challenge to the notion of speech community comes from theorists emphasizing the ways in which recent historical changes, notably globalization, powerfully influence the ways in which people engage in the world and disrupt traditional notions of community and community membership. Through a whole range of technological, social, and economic developments – shaping modes of labor, travel, and communication – individuals’ relations to others are more diverse and fluid, less restricted by time and space. The extent to which speech community with any presumed identifiable boundaries continues to be a meaningful category of observation and analysis is debatable within the context of a rapidly changing world (Rampton, 1998; Collins, 2003). 9 Re-examining the Notion of Speaker Just as the notion of speech community has been challenged, so too have prominent labels used to categorize individuals in relation to communities – such as social class, ethnicity, linguistic repertoire, and gender. Such terms, because they often denote fixed sets of attributes and capacities, have been recognized as problematic, particularly by post structuralist writers who stress that identity is always in process. Indeed, the relationship between language and identity has established itself as a key area for research. Such work tends to challenge the idea that language use reflects categories of identity (I speak as I do because I am a working class woman) and emphasizes, rather, how individuals actively construct aspects of social and personal identity through their use of language in specific contexts (in speaking as I am, I am constructing and representing myself as a working class woman). While it is recognized that such constructions of identity are not free floating but are regulated by the specific contexts and interactions in which they occur (Cameron, 1997a), the fluidity of identity tends to be emphasized. In these approaches, the term ‘performativity’ rather than ‘performance’ is used, in order to signal how identity is enacted or performed through interaction (Cameron, 1997b; Butler, 1990/1999). Re-examining Context The work of Hymes placed the importance of context centrally within the concern of linguistics and advocated ethnography as the key organizing methodological tool with which to observe language use. However, there has been considerable debate about what constitutes context and how context should be conceptualized and explored. Two significant and quite distinct approaches to the study of context can be found in conversation analysis and critical discourse analysis: the former orients inwards as it were towards language, the latter orients outwards towards the social world. Conversation analysts argue that speakers construct and represent relevant aspects of context through their actual interaction and that these can be empirically observed (Schegloff, 1997). In contrast, critical discourse analysts (Fairclough, 1995) and feminist linguists (Cameron, 1992) have signaled the limitations to approaches that seek to understand context through empirical observation alone: there have been calls to draw on social, critical, and post structuralist theorists and philosophers such as Foucault, Habermas, Bourdieu, and Bakhtin, in order to explore the ways in which language use is related to ideology and power, and in order to explore how phenomena such as globalization are influencing communicative practices. Some of this work tends to explore language use through the lens of such theory and pays only minimal attention to examining contexts empirically (Chouliaraki and Fairclough, 1999), whereas others drawing on ethnographic traditions such as Hymes’s, aim to establish an approach that draws on both empirical observation and specific aspects of social theory (Rampton, 1995; Lee, 1996; Maybin, 1999). Attempts have a been made to integrate levels of analysis at the macro level of society with micro levels of actual utterances; Gee (1996) for example uses the terms big ‘D’ discourse to refer to the former and little ‘d’ discourse to refer to the latter; Fairclough (1992) has developed a three-layered framework to explore such relations, which he refers to as a textually oriented discourse analysis (TODA). 10 See also: Assessment of Second Language Proficiency; Chomsky, Noam (b. 1928); Codes, Elaborated and Restricted (Bernstein); Communicative Language Teaching; Context, Communicative; Discourse, Foucauldian Approach; Ethnomethodology; Habermas, Jurrgen (b. 1929); Identity in Sociocultural Anthropology and Language; Identity: Second Language; Intercultural Pragmatics and Communication; Speech and Language Community. Bibliography Barton D & Hamilton M (1998). Local literacies: reading and writing in one community. London: Routledge. Basso K (1974). ‘The ethnography of writing.’ In Bauman R & Sherzer J (eds.) Explorations in the ethnography of speaking. Cambridge: CUP. 425–432. Berkenkotter C, Huchin T & Ackerman J (1991). ‘Social context and socially constructed texts.’ In Bazerman C& Paradis J (eds.) Textual dynamics and the professions. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. 191–215. Bernstein B (1996). Pedagogy, symbolic control and identity: theory, research, critique. London: Taylor and Francis. Bloomfield L (1933). Language. New York: Holt, Reinhart and Winston. Bourdieu P (1977). ‘The economics of linguistic exchanges.’ Social Science Information 16, 645–668. Butler J (1990/1999). Gender trouble: feminism and the subversion of identity. New York/London: Routledge. Cameron D (1992). Feminism and linguistic theory (2nd edn.). Basingstoke/London: Macmillan. Cameron D (1997a). ‘Theoretical debates in feminist linguistics: questions of sex and gender.’ In Wodak R (ed.) Gender and discourse. London: Sage Publications. Cameron D (1997b). ‘Performing gender identity: young men’s talk and the construction of heterosexual masculinity.’ In Johnson S & Meinhof U (eds.) Language and masculinity. Oxford: Blackwell. Chomsky N (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton. Chomsky N (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chouiaraki L & Fairclough N (1999). Discourse in late modernity: rethinking critical discourse analysis. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Collins J (2003). ‘Language, identity, and learning in the era of ‘‘expert guided’’ system.’ In Wortham S & Rymes B (eds.) The linguistic anthropology of education. Connecticut: Praeger. 31–60. Culler J (1975). Structuralist poetics. London: Routledge/ Kegan Paul. Eckert P & McConnell-Ginet S (2003). Language andgender. Cambridge: CUP. Fairclough N (1992). ‘The appropriacy of appropriateness.’ In Fairclough N (ed.) Critical language awareness.London: Longman. 233–252. Fairclough N (1995). Critical discourse analysis: the critical study of language. London: Longman. Firth A & Wagner J (1997). On discourse, communication and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. Modern Language Journal 81(3) 285–300. Gee J (1996). Social linguistics and literacies: ideologies in discourses (2nd edn.). Basingstoke: Falmer Press. Gumperz J J & Hymes D (eds.) (1972/1986). Directions in sociolinguistics: the ethnography of communication.Oxford: Basil Blackwell. 11 Habermas J (1970). ‘Towards a theory of communicative competence.’ In Dreitzel H P (ed.) Recent sociology No. 2. New York: Macmillan. 114–148. Hall J K & Eggington W G (2000). The sociopolitics of English language teaching. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. Heath S B (1983).Ways with words: language life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: CUP. Hymes D (1962/1968). ‘The ethnography of speaking.’ In Fishman J A (ed.) The ethnography of communication. The Hague: Mouton. 99–138. Hymes D (1971). ‘Competence and performance in linguistic theory.’ In Huxley R & Ingram E (eds.) Language acquisition: models and methods. New York: Academic Press. Hymes D (1972a). ‘On communicative competence.’ In Pride J B & Holmes J (eds.) Sociolinguistics. Harmondsworth:Penguin. 269–285. Hymes D (1972b). ‘Models of the interaction of language and social life.’ In Gumperz J & Hymes D (eds.) Directions in sociolinguistics: the ethnography of communication. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. 35–71. Hymes D (1972c). ‘Introduction.’ In Cazden C, John V & Hymes D (eds.) Functions of language in the classroom. New York: Teachers College Press. xi–lvii. Hymes D (1974a). ‘Ways of speaking.’ In Bauman R & Sherzer J (eds.) Explorations in the ethnography of speaking. Cambridge: CUP. Hymes D (1974b). Foundations in sociolinguistics: an ethnographic approach. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Hymes D (2003). Now I know only so far: essays in ethnopoetics. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Jakobson R (1960). ‘Closing statement: linguistics and poetics.’ In Sebeok T A (ed.) Style in language. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press. 398–429. Lee A (1996). Gender, literacy and the curriculum: rewriting school geography. London: Taylor and Francis. Maybin J (1999). ‘Framing and evaluation in 10–12 year old school children’s use of appropriated speech, in relation to their induction into educational procedures and practices.’ Text 19, 4. Norton B (2000). Identity and language learning: gender, ethnicity and educational change. London: Longman. Malinowski B (1923). ‘The problem of meaning in primitive languages.’ In Ogden C K & Richards I A (eds.) The meaning of meaning. London: Kegan Paul. 451–510. Malinowski B (1935). Coral gardens and their magic (2 vols.). London: Allen and Unwin. Paulsten C B (1992). Linguistic and communicative competence: topics in ESL. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. Philipsen G (1975). ‘Speaking ‘‘like a man’’ in teamsterville: culture patterns of role enactment in an urban neighbourhood.’ Quarterly Journal of Speech 61, 13–22. Rampton B (1995). Crossing: language and ethnicity among adolescents. London: Longman. Rampton B (1998). ‘Speech community.’ In Verschueren J et al. (eds.) Handbook of pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Saussure F de ([1916] 1959). In Bally C & Sechehaye A (eds.) Course in general linguistics. Baskin W (trans.). New York: McGraw Hill. Schultz K&Hull G (2002). ‘Locating literacy theory in outof-school contexts.’ In Hull G & Schultz K (eds.) School’sout: bridging out-of-school literacies with classroom practice. New York/London: Teachers College Press. Columbia University. 11–31. 12 Schegloff E A (1997). ‘Whose text, whose context?’ Discourse and Society 8, 165–187. Swales J (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Wodak R (ed.) (1992). Language power and ideology. Oxford: Blackwell View publication stats