Acta Psychiatr Scand 2018: 138: 558–570 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/acps.12953 © 2018 The Authors Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA Social media and its relationship with mood, self-esteem and paranoia in psychosis Berry N, Emsley R, Lobban F, Bucci S. Social media and its relationship with mood, self-esteem and paranoia in psychosis. Objective: An evidence-base is emerging indicating detrimental and beneficial effects of social media. Little is known about the impact of social media use on people who experience psychosis. Method: Forty-four participants with and without psychosis completed 1084 assessments of social media use, perceived social rank, mood, selfesteem and paranoia over a 6-day period using an experience sampling method (ESM). Results: Social media use predicted low mood, but did not predict selfesteem and paranoia. Posting about feelings and venting on social media predicted low mood and self-esteem and high paranoia, whilst posting about daily activities predicted increases in positive affect and self-esteem and viewing social media newsfeeds predicted reductions in negative affect and paranoia. Perceptions of low social rank when using social media predicted low mood and self-esteem and high paranoia. The impact of social media use did not differ between participants with and without psychosis; although, experiencing psychosis moderated the relationship between venting and negative affect. Social media use frequency was lower in people with psychosis. Conclusion: Findings show the potential detrimental impact of social media use for people with and without psychosis. Despite few betweengroup differences, overall negative psychological consequences highlight the need to consider use in clinical practice. N. Berry1 , R. Emsley2 , F. Lobban3 , S. Bucci1,4 1 Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, Faculty of Biology, Medicine and Health, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, Manchester, 2Department of Biostatistics and Health Informatics, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, 3 Spectrum Centre for Mental Health Research, Lancaster University, Lancaster, and 4Greater Manchester Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Key words: psychosis; schizophrenia; behaviour Natalie Berry, Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, University of Manchester, Zochonis Building, Brunswick Street, M13 9PL Manchester, UK. E-mail: natalie.berry@manchester.ac.uk Accepted for publication August 3, 2018 Significant outcomes • This is the first study to use ESM to highlight the significant association between self-reported social media • • engagement and subsequent reductions in mood at the next time-point in a sample of individuals with and without experience of psychosis. Types of self-reported behaviours on social media were associated with subsequent changes in mood and paranoia at the next time-point. Specifically, posting about daily activities was associated with subsequent improvements in positive affect and self-esteem, whilst viewing social media newsfeeds led to significant reductions in negative affect and paranoia and content consumption was associated with subsequent reduction in negative affect. Conversely, posting about feelings and venting on social media were associated with subsequent reductions in mood and self-esteem and increases in paranoia and viewing profiles of individuals who were not ‘friends’ on social media and commenting on the posts/pictures of others led to increases in paranoia. People with psychosis are significantly less likely to use social media than people without psychosis. Limitations • The sample was limited to mostly White British participants and clinicians may have been more likely to refer • • 558 patients who had previously expressed positive or negative experiences of engaging with social media, which may limit the generalisability of the findings. The design of the study was correlational in nature, so other factors may have also contributed towards the impact of social media use. The study was reliant on participants self-reporting social media use, which may be subject to inaccurate recall. However, the use of ESM for self-reported use over a short time-scale is likely to have helped to mitigate this limitation. Introduction The use of social media websites such as Twitter, Facebook and Instagram is widespread. Social media websites allow individuals to construct profiles in which they can maintain and create social networks, circulate details about their daily lives and respond to posts written by others (1). Rates of social media use by people with severe mental health problems such as psychosis are lower than the general population based on small-scale studies (2, 3). People with mental health problems already use social media websites to self-manage their mental health. For example, social media can be a helpful coping mechanism to facilitate self-expression and communication with others with similar experiences and to access motivational content (4–6). Clinicians have observed occasions where online communication has been beneficial for clients’ with SMI through accessible peer support and ability for anonymous self-expression (7). Individuals are amenable to the idea of receiving mental health support via social media websites (8) and have suggested the inclusion of social media components, such as moderated discussion forums, in future interventions (9). Despite some evidence for the potential therapeutic benefits of social media use, social media engagement may also be harmful for an individual’s mental health and wellbeing. For example, several studies have reported a significant link between high social media use and low mood and depression (10–12). However, others have found no evidence of a link between social media use and mood (13, 14). Mixed findings have also been reported for the relationship between social media use and self-esteem (15–18). Systematic reviews have highlighted that conclusions cannot yet be drawn and further robust research is warranted (19, 20). It has been proposed that people with psychosis may be particularly vulnerable to paranoid ideas after using social media websites (21), and evidence from case reports suggests the development and exacerbation of symptoms associated with severe mental health problems after social media engagement (22–25). Individuals with psychosis may be more affected by content consumption on social media in comparison with those without psychosis due to the posts written by others often being open to individual interpretation. Specifically, people with psychosis can have cognitive biases that can lead them to misinterpret the actions and behaviours of others as threatening or self-referent. Therefore, a virtual world where one is continuously observing the content written by others may facilitate individuals with psychosis to observe and become suspicious by others actions online. However, much of the current research has relied on participants with severe mental health problems retrospectively self-reporting whether they feel their use of social media leads to paranoia (3, 22, 25). More recently, Bird et al. (26) reported that the experience of negative affect during social media use correlates with paranoia severity. The lack of robust study designs and larger-scale research prevents conclusions from being drawn. Recent findings suggest there may be specific aspects of social media use that determine whether it is beneficial or detrimental. One such explanation is that social media may elicit downward online social comparisons; that is, comparing oneself less favourably to others, leading to negative feelings (27). Festinger (28) highlighted the importance of social comparison to explain the inherent drive for individuals to achieve accurate self-evaluation of opinions and abilities. Social comparisons lead to the development of social ranks (SRs), whereby individuals compare themselves to others on relative power and social attractiveness (29). Social media may facilitate the formation of SRs due to the tendency for people to present themselves and their experiences in a positive light (30, 31). Researchers working in the field of social comparisons and psychopathology have proposed that perceived SR is associated with mood and self-esteem (32, 33) and it has previously been reported that negative social comparisons on social media websites are associated with depression and low self-esteem (34–37). Indeed, a recent editorial proposed that individuals who experience mental health problems may be more likely to be affected by the social comparisons that social media elicits due to a negative cognitive bias (38). Researchers have now begun to explore whether social media behaviours contribute to psychological outcomes. Burke et al. (39) separated social media behaviours into (i) directed communication such as posting on another user’s profile; (ii) content production such as posting status updates; and (iii) content consumption such as scrolling through social media newsfeeds. Additionally, a recent review highlighted that active social media use can enhance subjective wellbeing, whereas passive social media use reduces subjective wellbeing (40). Therefore, consequences of social media use may relate to social media activities, rather than levels of social media use per se. In order to examine the real-time relationship between social media use and mental health and wellbeing, some researchers have employed 559 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Social media use in psychosis experience sampling methodology. For example, Kross et al. (41) used ESM to demonstrate that Facebook use predicted subsequent declines in subjective wellbeing; although, this effect was not predicted by direct communication on Facebook. Further research used ESM to explore the relationship between passive Facebook use and wellbeing, reporting that envy mediated the relationship between passive Facebook use and declines in affective wellbeing (42). Finally, Jelenchick et al. (14) demonstrated no association between social media use and depression in adolescents through ESM data collection. Aims Previous research has mainly employed retrospective accounts of social media use, SR, mood, selfesteem and paranoia, with many studies conducted in adolescent non-clinical samples. This study aimed to explore in real time the impact of social media use on mood, self-esteem and paranoia. Specifically, we hypothesised: H1: Social media use will predict low mood and self-esteem and high paranoia. H2: Passive social media use (content consumption) will predict low mood and selfesteem and high paranoia, whilst active social media use (content posting and direct communication) will predict high mood and self-esteem and low paranoia. H3: Higher perceived SR in comparison with others on social media will predict high mood and self-esteem and low paranoia. H4: The impact of self-reported social media use and behaviours on mood, self-esteem and paranoia will be moderated by a diagnosis of psychosis, with a stronger relationship evident in the clinical group. Method Design Experience sampling method was used to capture momentary assessments of social media use, mood, self-esteem and paranoia (withinsubjects), and paper-based questionnaires were used to capture retrospective reports of these variables (between-subjects). Experience sampling methodology (ESM) was chosen to explore the impact of social media use because it involves the repeated assessment of variables over a specified time-period, which is more ecologically valid than traditional retrospective measures (43). 560 Participants Participants were recruited via National Health Service (NHS) secondary care mental health services in the UK and through promoting the study on research volunteering websites. Clinical participants were eligible to participate if they had a clinician-verified experience of first episode psychosis or had received a diagnosis of DSM-IV schizophrenia-spectrum disorder or bipolar disorder. Non-clinical participants were eligible to participate if they self-reported no current experiences of mental health problems. Further eligibility criteria for all participants were as follows: (i) 18 years of age or over; (ii) able to speak and read English; (iii) able to provide informed consent; (iv) available for a week-long study; (v) self-report Facebook or Twitter account; and (vi) self-report social media use at least three times per week. This paper reports findings from the non-clinical and psychosis participants only (n = 44) due to the small number of participants in the sample with bipolar disorder (n = 6). Findings including participants experiencing bipolar disorder are available from the author on request. Baseline and trait-level measures Trait-level mood was measured using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (44). The PANAS consists of 20 adjectives associated with positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) and respondents are asked to indicate their levels of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = very slightly or not at all; 5 = extremely). The scale can be used to measure both trait- and state-levels of PA and NA depending on question phrasing (44). The scale has excellent internal consistency for both PA (a = 0.86–0.90) and NA (a = 0.84–0.87). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.90 for PA and 0.91 for NA. Trait self-esteem was measured using the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES), which consists of 10 statements on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly agree; 4 = strongly disagree) and can be used to measure both trait and state self-esteem (45). The scale has also previously shown excellent internal consistency and test–retest reliability (r = 0.85– 0.88) (46). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.95. Trait paranoia was measured using the Paranoia Scale, which comprises 20 statements on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all applicable to me; 5 = extremely applicable to me). The scale has been used in both clinical and non-clinical samples 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Berry et al. and has shown good construct validity (a = 0.84) and test–retest reliability (r = 0.70) (47) and demonstrated excellent internal consistency in this study (a = 0.95). Baseline perceptions of SR were measured using the Social Comparison Scale (SCS, which consists of a list of 11 pairs of antonyms) (48). Participants are asked to indicate on a 10-point scale how they feel in comparison with others (e.g. 1 = inferior, 10 = superior). The higher the SCS score, the higher the perceived SR relative to others. The SCS has demonstrated good internal consistency in both clinical (a = 0.91) and non-clinical (a = 0.88) samples (48) and showed good internal consistency in this study (a = 0.83). Baseline assessments of social media use were produced after a review of the literature to identify items that had been used in previous studies. The Social Media Use Integration Scale (SMUIS) was included to measure participants’ emotional connection to social media and integration into their daily lives (48). The SMUIS is a 10-item scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) and has shown strong internal consistency (a = 0.91) and good test–retest reliability (r = 0.80) and can be modified for other social media platforms (49). The subsequent six questions focussed on social media privacy settings and were taken from research published by Ross et al. (50). A further 14 questions were adapted from the same questionnaire to assess how often participants reported engagement in certain social media behaviours (eight response options, range: ‘never’ to ‘more than once a day’) and assigned to the overarching activities of content posting and direct communication (active social media use) and content consumption (passive social media use). Social media behaviours were assigned to these overarching categories based on the criteria published by Burke et al. (39). Specifically, the activities of writing status updates (including status updates about daily activities, feelings, opinions and venting) and posting pictures/videos were assigned as content posting; social media newsfeed/timeline scrolls and viewing friends or strangers profiles were assigned as content consumption; commenting on another person’s post/picture, liking another person’s post/picture and sharing another person’s post/picture were assigned as direct communication. These activities were defined and agreed during the development of the ESM items and prior to data collection. State-level (ESM) measures The first question of the ESM assessments asked participants whether they had used social media since the last assessment. If participants selected ‘yes’, they were asked additional sets of questions regarding social media use prior to completing the remaining ESM assessments: (i) social media website used; (ii) social media activities; and (iii) feelings in comparison with others on social media using the SCS (44). The responses from question 2 were characterised under content posting, direct communication and content consumption also using the criteria developed by Burke et al. (39). Responses for question 3 were combined to produce the SCS scores for each participant at each time-point. In this study, the momentary assessment of SR using the SCS had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.96. To account for the potential that other forms of personal interaction may lead to changes in the variables of interest, participants were asked whether they had spoken with another person since the last assessment (yes/no) and to indicate whether this communication was face-to-face, online, text-message, telephone or a messaging smartphone application. Participants were presented with nine mood-related adjectives on a 7point Likert scale (51) and asked the degree to which the adjective described their current feelings (1 = not at all; 7 = very). Responses to negative and positive adjectives were combined for each participant to give a total value for NA and PA at each time-point and demonstrated excellent internal consistency (a = 0.93; a = 0.94 respectively). The subsequent two questions used four ESM selfesteem items and four paranoia items (51–53). For both questions, participants were asked to indicate on a 7-point Likert scale the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with the items (1 = not at all; 7 = very). Both the state self-esteem scale (a = 0.96) and state paranoia scale (a = 0.92) demonstrated excellent internal consistency in this study. A full list of ESM items is available in Appendix S1. Procedure Ethical approval was granted by the local research ethics committee. After consent, participants completed a demographics questionnaire and the baseline and trait-level measures in order to provide a description of the sample. Participants were given a unique username and password to access the ESM assessments and completed a trial run either on their own smartphone or a smartphone loaned to them for the duration of the study. Participants who were loaned a smartphone for the study were asked to only use the smartphone for activities 561 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Social media use in psychosis associated with the study. Specifically, participants were asked to only use the smartphones to contact the researcher with any technical questions that arose during the study period and to complete the momentary assessments. ESM assessments commenced the following morning. Text-messages containing a link to a secure online site were sent to participants at six pseudo-random times a day over a 6-day period. Participants had up to 15 min to click the link and complete the assessments after each text-message received. The data collection period ranged from 10:00 and 21:00; although, this could be adapted to allow for individual differences in waking hours. Participants were asked to complete an exit evaluation detailing reasons for missed assessments, debriefed by the researcher about the study and provided £20 vouchers (not contingent on the number of assessments completed). Data analysis Trait-level mood, self-esteem, paranoia and perceived SR were analysed by comparing betweengroup means using independent t-tests in SPSS Version 22 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). ESM data were assessed for normality through visual inspection of histograms and analysis of skewness and kurtosis. Analyses of ESM data were performed using Stata, version 14 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). The hierarchical structure of ESM data (observations are nested within days, within participants) requires multilevel modelling to be used due to the violation of the assumption of independence of observations. Using maximum likelihood, we fitted 3-level random intercept models a random intercept for each participant, a random intercept for each participant-day and participant-beep error term. To test whether self-reported social media use (H1), social media behaviours (H2) and perceived SR (H3) predicted mood, self-esteem and paranoia at the next time-point, separate multilevel linear regression analyses were estimated with PA, NA, self-esteem and paranoia as the outcome variables, and self-reported social media use, behaviours and perceived SR as the predictor variables. In all models, socialisation (whether or not an individual had spoken with another person) and group (clinical and non-clinical) were included as covariates to ensure any associations with the outcome variables could be attributed to the use of social media, rather than other forms of socialisation or group. To investigate whether the impact of social media use was moderated by a diagnosis of psychosis (H4), a further set of 562 multilevel linear regressions were estimated by looking at the two-way interaction between group and social media use, with employment status and socialisation included as covariates. Finally, whilst we did not make any specific hypotheses relating to between-group differences in social media use, odds ratios (ORs) and the corresponding 95% confidence intervals were calculated through a multilevel logistic regression to compare the likelihood of social media use between the clinical and non-clinical groups. Results Sample characteristics Fifty-one people (26 clinical and 25 non-clinical) consented to participate. Data provided from one clinical participant were excluded from analyses as they did not complete any assessments over the study period. Data from participants with bipolar disorder (n = 6) were excluded due to the small sample size. Therefore, the data from a total of 44 participants (25 non-clinical; 19 psychosis) are reported in this paper. Demographic information is presented in Table 1. The mean age of participants in the psychosis (M = 33.7, SD = 9.7, range = 22–54) and nonclinical (M = 35.4, SD = 14.7, range = 20–62) groups did not significantly differ (t(42) = 0.45, P = 0.655). Three participants (16%) in the clinical group and one participant (4%) in the nonclinical group borrowed a smartphone for the duration of the study. All borrowed smartphones were returned undamaged. Two participants in the clinical group and one participant in the non-clinical group borrowed a smartphone because they did not own a smartphone; one participant in the clinical group borrowed a smartphone because their own smartphone had poor data connectivity. Completion rates Out of the total 1584 assessments that were possible during the study period, 1084 were fully completed by participants (n = 458 clinical; n = 626 non-clinical; 68.4% response rate). The proportion of assessments completed per participant ranged between 25% and 91.6%. The mean number of completed assessments did not significantly differ between clinical and non-clinical groups [t(42) = 0.63, P = 0.532], males and females [t(42) = 1.00, P = 0.322], on the basis of whether a participant borrowed or owned the smartphone that was used for data entry [t(42) = 0.33, 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Berry et al. Table 1. Participant demographic information, clinical characteristics and social media use Clinical group Gender Male Female Employment status Working full time Working part time Working voluntary Student Unemployed Ethnicity Asian or Asian British White British White Other Mixed Race Highest level of education High school College Some University Undergraduate degree Postgraduate degree Diagnosis First episode psychosis Schizophrenia Schizoaffective disorder Paranoid schizophrenia Psychosis NOS Social media websites currently used Facebook Twitter Instagram Google+ Tumblr Flickr Social media apps currently used Facebook Twitter Instagram WhatsApp Snapchat Google+ Devices used to access social media Laptop Desktop Smartphone Tablet n (%) 7 (37) 12 (63) 0 (0) 2 (11) 2 (11) 2 (11) 13 (68) 1 (4) 18 (95) 0 (0) 0 (0) 5 (26) 9 (47) 3 (16) 2 (11) 0 (0) 9 (47) 3 (16) 4 (21) 2 (11) 1 (5) 19 (100) 91 (47) 9 (47) 10 (53) 3 (16) 2 (11) 17 (89) 9 (47) 8 (42) 11 (58) 6 (32) 7 (37) 8 (42) 5 (26) 18 (95) 8 (42) Non-clinical group Gender Male Female Employment status Working full time Working part time Working voluntary Student Unemployed Ethnicity Asian or Asian British White British White other Mixed Race Highest level of education High school College Some University Undergraduate degree Postgraduate degree n (%) 11 (44) 14 (56) v2 = 0.23, df = 1 13 (52) 3 (12) 0 (0) 7 (28) 2 (8) v2 = 25.70, df = 4 1 (4) 21 (84) 2 (8) 1 (4) 4 (16) 4 (16) 4 (16) 10 (40) 3 (12) – – – – – Social media websites currently used Facebook 25 (100) Twitter 15 (60) Instagram 10 (40) Google+ 6 (24) Tumblr 1 (4) Flickr 1 (4) Social media apps currently used Facebook 23 (92) Twitter 13 (52) Instagram 11 (44) WhatsApp 18 (72) Snapchat 12 (48) Google+ 5 (20) Devices used to access social media Laptop 17 (68) Desktop 10 (40) Smartphone 23 (92) Tablet 6 (24) P = 0.745], employment status (F(4, 39) = 0.86, P = 0.495) or level of education (F(4, 39) = 0.22, P = 0.928). Finally, there was no correlation between assessment completion rate and age (r = 0.64, P = 0.678). It is generally accepted in ESM studies that participants should complete at least a third of assessments for the data to be included in analyses (54). However, this value is arbitrary and given that ESM is not affected by issues relating to missing data, a specific completion rate of assessments is not required (55). One person in the study completed less than the specified value (25%), but the data they provided were still usable for the purpose of this study due to the variations in the timing of P Test statistic Total n (%) 0.632 18 (41) 26 (59) <0.001 13 (30) 5 (11) 2 (5) 9 (21) 15 (30) v2 = 2.46, df = 3 0.483 2 (5) 39 (89) 2 (5) 1 (2) v2 = 9.88, df = 4 0.043 9 (21) 13 (30) 7 (16) 12 (27) 3 (7) 1.000 0.405 0.625 0.051 0.178 0.395 44 (100) 24 (55) 19 (43) 16 (37) 4 (9) 3 (7) – – – – – – – – – – v2 v2 v2 v2 v2 = = = = = 0.70, df = 1 0.24, df = 1 3.82, df = 1 1.82, df = 1 0.72 df = 1 v2 v2 v2 v2 v2 v2 = = = = = = 0.83, df 0.93, df 0.16, df 0.96, df 1.20, df 1.54, df = = = = = = 1 1 1 1 1 1 0.773 0.761 0.900 0.328 0.272 0.214 40 (91) 22 (50) 19 (43) 29 (66) 18 (41) 12 (27) v2 v2 v2 v2 = = = = 2.95, df 0.90, df 0.13, df 1.63, df = = = = 1 1 1 1 0.086 0.343 0.721 0.202 25 (57) 15 (34) 41 (93) 14 (32) the responses and was, therefore, included in all analyses. Demographic characteristics and social media use General social media use across the study duration was not significantly related to participant age (r = 0.24, P = 0.116), but was associated with mental health status, with participants in the clinical group reporting lower levels of social media use in comparison with participants in the non-clinical group [t(42) = 2.15, P = 0.037]. Social media use was not significantly associated with gender [t (42) = 1.41, P = 0.166], whether a participant borrowed or owned the smartphone that was used 563 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Social media use in psychosis for data entry [t(42) = 1.17, P = 0.249], employment status (F(4, 39) = 2.00, P = 0.114) or level of education (F(4, 42) = 0.64, P = 0.638). Between-group differences in trait measures Trait-level self-esteem, perceived SR, mood and paranoia are presented in Table 2. Participants in the non-clinical group had significantly higher scores for trait-level self-esteem and PA in comparison with the clinical group, whilst participants in the clinical group had significantly higher scores for trait-level NA and paranoia. There were no significant between-group differences in trait-level SR scores. Does social media use predict subsequent mood, self-esteem and paranoia? Table 3 demonstrates that H1 was partially supported. Social media use negatively predicted PA and positively predicted NA. However, social media use did not predict self-esteem or paranoia. Multilevel logistic regression analyses were conducted to explore whether trait variables predicted social media use across the study period. Traitlevel PA (b = 0.0021, SE = 0.0190, p = 0.913, 95% CI [ 0.035 to 0.039]), NA (b = 0.0079, SE = 0.0173, P = 0.646, 95% CI [ 0.042 to 0.026]), self-esteem (b = 0.0018, SE = 0.0226, P = 0.938, 95% CI [ 0.042 to 0.046]), paranoia (b = 0.0127, SE = 0.0074, P = 0.085, 95% CI [ 0.027 to 0.002]) and SR (b = 0.0096, SE = 0.0115, P = 0.403, 95% CI [ 0.032 to 0.013]) did not predict social media use. Therefore, it likely that social media use predicted mood, rather than being predicted by mood. Do reported social media behaviours predict subsequent mood, self-esteem, paranoia and perceived SR? Table 4 shows that content posting did not predict PA, self-esteem or perceived SR, but did positively predict NA and paranoia. Content consumption and direct communication were not found to predict PA, self-esteem, paranoia or perceived SR. However, content consumption did significantly negatively predict NA. Therefore, H2 was not supported. Posting about daily activities led to increases in PA and self-esteem at the next time-point. Conversely, posting about feelings led to subsequent increases in NA and paranoia and reductions in PA, self-esteem and perceived SR; venting on social media negatively predicted PA and selfesteem and positively predicted NA and paranoia; looking through social media newsfeeds negatively predicted NA and paranoia; viewing a ‘friends’ social media profile positively predicted perceived SR; viewing profiles of people who were not ‘friends’ on social media positively predicted paranoia; and commenting on other peoples’ statuses positively predicted paranoia. Does perceived SR when using social media predict subsequent mood, self-esteem and paranoia? Table 5 shows that higher perceived SR when using social media positively predicted PA and self-esteem and negatively predicted NA and paranoia. These findings support H3 and demonstrate that perceptions of high SR when using social media predict subsequent increases in mood and self-esteem and decreases in paranoia. Are lower scores for positive affect and higher scores for negative affect after social media use moderated by experiencing psychosis? Psychosis was not found to moderate the relationship between social media use and PA (b = 0.1974, SE = 0.4969, P = 0.691, 95% CI [ 0.776 to 1.171]) or NA (b = 0.0876, SE = 0.5291, P = 0.869, 95% CI [ 1.125 to 0.949]). This finding does not support H4 that psychosis would moderate associations between social media use and mood. We also explored whether psychosis moderated the relationship between specific types of social media use that had led to significant changes in outcome variables. Psychosis did not moderate the Table 2. Participant trait-level scores of self-esteem, perceived social rank, positive affect, negative affect and paranoia Clinical group n M SD Non-clinical group n M SD Test statistic Self-esteem Social rank Positive affect Negative affect Paranoia 25 25 25 25 25 12.6 54.2 29.5 25.6 58.6 6.1 16.6 9.5 8.3 20.3 Self-esteem Social rank Positive affect Negative affect Paranoia 24* 25 25 25 25 22.3 61.3 36.7 17.7 32.4 3.5 9.6 4.9 7.6 10.3 t (41) t (42) t (42) t (42) t (42) *Missing data from 1 participant. 564 = 6.5 = 1.8 = 3.2 = 3.3 = 5.6 P <0.001 0.079 0.002 0.002 <0.001 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Berry et al. Table 3. Effect of social media use on positive and negative affect, self-esteem and paranoia. Effect is unstandardised b coefficient from separate models Table 5. Effect of perceived social rank on positive affect, self-esteem, negative affect and paranoia. Effect is unstandardised b coefficient Dependent variable Effect Dependent variable Effect Positive affect Negative affect Self-esteem Paranoia 0.5027 0.5593 0.0498 0.1272 Positive affect Self-esteem Negative affect Paranoia 0.1232 0.0901 0.1186 0.0780 Standard error P 0.2469 0.2628 0.1783 0.1794 0.042 0.033 0.780 0.478 95% confidence interval 0.9866, 0.0188 0.0442, 1.0744 0.2996, 0.3993 0.2245, 0.4789 relationship between: (i) content posting and NA (b = 0.7558, SE =0.9746, P = 0.438, CI [ 1.154 to 2.666]) or paranoia (b = 0.0882, SE = 0.6580, P = 0.894, CI [ 1.380 to 1.203]); (ii) posting about daily activities and PA (b = 0.7114, SE = 1.0824, P = 0.511, CI [ 1.410 to 2.833]) or self-esteem (b = 1.1927, SE = 0.7537, P = 0.114, CI [ 0.284 to 2.670]); (iii) posting about feelings and PA (b = 1.0790, SE = 1.7409, P = 0.535, CI [ 4.491 to 2.333]), NA (b = 3.2388, SE = 1.8877, P = 0.086, CI [ 0.461 to 6.939]), self-esteem (b = 0.7087, SE = 1.2316, P = 0.565, CI [ 3.123 to 1.705]), paranoia (b = 1.1283, SE = 1.2722, P = 0.375, CI [ 3.622 to 1.365]) or perceived SR (b = 3.1986, SE = 4.7332, P = 0.499, CI [ 6.078 to 12.476]); (iv) venting on social media and PA (b = 2.3189, SE = 2.1563, P = 0.282, CI [ 6.545 to 1.907]), self-esteem (b = 0.4254, SE = 1.5018, P = 0.777, CI [ 2.518 to 3.369]) or paranoia (b = 0.6628, SE = 1.5572, P = 0.670, CI [ 3.715 to 2.389]); (v) content consumption and NA (b = 1.2609, SE = 1.0988, P = 0.251, CI [ 3.414 to 0.893]); (vi) viewing social media newsfeeds and NA (b = 0.4351, SE = 1.0026, P = 0.664, CI [ 1.530 Standard error P 0.0132 0.0098 0.0152 0.0105 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 95% confidence interval 0.0976, 0.1488 0.0710, 0.1092 0.1484, 0.0888 0.0987, 0.0574 to 2.40]) or paranoia (b = 0.0764, SE = 0.6698, P = 0.909, CI [ 1.389 to 1.236]); (vii) viewing profiles of friends and perceived SR (b = 0.8748, SE = 2.3684, P = 0.714, CI [ 3.802 to 5.552]); vii) viewing profiles of strangers and paranoia (b = 1.3931, SE = 0.8995, P = 0.121, CI [ 3.156 to 0.370]); and (viii) commenting on another person’s post or picture (b = 0.7896, SE = 0.6675, P = 0.237, CI [ 0.519 to 2.098]. However, the relationship between venting on social media and subsequent NA was significantly moderated by psychosis (b = 4.7100, SE = 2.3101, P = 0.041, CI [0.182–9.238]). Do social media use and behaviours differ between people with and without psychosis? Clinical participants were less likely to use social media than non-clinical participants (OR = 0.5366, SE = 0.1550, P = 0.031, 95% CI [0.305–0.945]). Separate analyses revealed that clinical participants were less likely to use Facebook (OR = 0.5394, SE = 0.1692, P = 0.049, CI [0.292–0.998]), but there were no differences in Table 4. Effect of reported social media behaviours on positive and negative affect, self-esteem, paranoia and perceived social rank Positive affect n Content posting Posting about daily activities Posting about opinions Posting about feelings Posting pictures/videos of self or others Venting on social media Content consumption Looking through newsfeed Viewing friends’ profiles Viewing profiles of people who are not friends Direct communication Commented on another person’s post/picture Liked another person’s post/picture Shared another person’s post/picture b SE Negative affect b SE Self-esteem Β Paranoia SE b Perceived social rank SE b SE 114 71 20 22 17 13 514 470 121 47 0.1148 1.3171* 1.082 4.8410*** 0.4534 4.4309*** 0.1816 0.2280 0.5025 0.7552 0.4468 0.5390 0.8993 0.8555 0.9413 1.0676 0.4710 0.4444 0.4346 0.6107 1.2316* 0.7508 0.0392 4.3416 *** 0.3502 4.3563*** 1.1180* 1.0748* 0.0271 0.6353 0.4801 0.5904 0.9713 0.9362 1.0063 1.1480 0.4993 0.4720 0.4672 0.6549 0.0499 0.9967** 0.2772 1.6865** 0.1546 1.7770* 0.5389 0.5788 0.1166 0.4298 0.3108 0.3760 0.6235 0.6066 0.6485 0.7426 0.3251 0.3078 0.3012 0.4219 0.6638* 0.1629 0.1005 2.7189 *** 0.3190 2.1293** 0.5269 0.8823** 0.1016 1.2631** 0.3237 0.3987 0.6497 0.6279 0.6727 0.7697 0.3346 0.3148 0.3133 0.4349 1.1846 0.7363 1.6827 7.9489** 2.3035 2.8936 1.7166 1.9190 2.7343* 2.9803 1.1983 1.4666 2.4000 2.2059 2.4867 2.8688 1.2481 1.1773 1.1505 1.6407 166 113 0.0148 0.0416 0.3787 0.4315 0.1451 0.0571 0.4069 0.4649 0.0255 0.0511 0.2624 0.2993 0.4852 0.6584* 0.2716 0.3102 0.9634 1.0097 1.0078 1.1515 43 32 0.1700 0.2047 0.7014 0.7112 0.2100 0.1488 0.7531 0.7612 0.0748 0.0489 0.4871 0.4900 0.0126 0.2866 0.5041 0.5099 1.0298 1.9830 1.8664 1.8829 ***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05. 565 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Social media use in psychosis Twitter (OR = 1.1484, SE=0.9881, P = 0.871, 95% CI [0.213–6.201]) or Instagram (OR = 0.6580, SE=0.7151, P = 0.700, 95% CI [0.078–5.536]) use. Discussion This study aimed to identify whether social media use predicted subsequent mood, self-esteem and paranoia, pinpointing any specific social media behaviours reported by participants that contributed towards relationships observed. Additionally, we aimed to determine whether perceptions of SR when using social media predicted these outcomes and to ascertain whether experiencing psychosis moderated any relationship between social media use and mood, self-esteem and paranoia. As hypothesised, social media use predicted reductions in PA and elevations in NA. However, contrary to expectations, social media use was not found to predict self-esteem or paranoia. Further analyses revealed that posting about daily activities predicted high PA and self-esteem, whereas posting about feelings and venting on social media predicted low PA, self-esteem and perceived SR and high NA and paranoia. Seidman (2013) proposed that posting about feelings and venting on social media is an emotional form of self-disclosure, whereas posting information about daily activities is general self-disclosure (56). Emotional self-disclosures are important for social connectedness, belonging and feelings of intimacy (57–59) and people perceive personal disclosures on social media as a helpful mechanism to connect with others, maintain relationships, exchange opinions and receive support (60–62). However, in contrast to these findings, it was general factual-based disclosures that were beneficial for increasing mood, whilst emotional-based posting was detrimental to mood and paranoia. One tentative explanation for this unexpected finding is that participants did not receive the supportive and reinforcing responses they hoped for when they posted emotional selfdisclosures. This possibility is supported by recent research that found social attraction towards social media users was lower when posts contained highly personal or negative self-disclosures (63) and that negative online disclosures by individuals with low self-esteem receive undesirable responses due to the negativity expressed (64). In contrast with previous research (37, 40), content consumption was found to lead to decreases in NA and there was no relationship between content consumption and perceived SR whilst engaging with social media. The discrepancy in findings 566 between this study and previous studies may be linked to the recent changes in content that individuals observe on social media and, in particular, Facebook. Specifically, it has been widely reported that social media websites now contain a disproportionate number of advertisements and news articles in comparison with actual status updates and posts from social media friends. Indeed, a recent announcement by Facebook in 2018 highlighted the evident reduction in the presence of personal posts on the platform due to the plethora of posts from business and the media, resulting in the aim to reduce this public content (65). Therefore, social media consumption may have been less likely to elicit comparative self-reflections than previous studies due to the reduced exposure to others lives prevalent in the past. The evolving nature of social media platforms means that future research should examine the impact of the upcoming change in website content. Experiencing psychosis did not moderate the impact of social media use or behaviours; however, this did moderate the relationship between venting and NA. This suggests that maladaptive coping strategies, such as venting, may be more detrimental for individuals who experience psychosis in comparison with the general population. Although, on the whole, a diagnosis of psychosis did not moderate the impact of social media use and behaviours, the finding remains clinically important because the impact of social media use may have more serious consequences in individuals with psychosis due to lower mood prior to engagement. Therefore, the observed impact of social media use warrants further consideration in the context of psychosis and mental health and wellbeing more generally. Additionally, clinical participants were significantly less likely to use social media than non-clinical participants. Small-scale studies demonstrate lower social media website access by individuals with severe mental health problems than the general population (2, 3); however, this is the first to identify differences in frequency of use. Higher levels of paranoia were also associated with lower levels of social media use and, given that the clinical group showed significantly higher scores for both trait- and state-level paranoia, it is likely that the experience of paranoia led to lower levels of social media use. Other potential reasons for these findings should be explored in future studies to determine whether differences in social media use frequency are due to barriers to use or other factors such as symptom occurrence or avoiding social media to prevent any associated negative outcomes. 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Berry et al. Strengths, limitations and directions for future research A strength of the current study was the ecologically valid prospective measurement of social media use and psychological outcomes. Additionally, socialisation was included to identify whether other forms of social interaction may have predicted outcomes. Trait SR, mood, self-esteem and paranoia were also measured at baseline and were not found to predict social media use. Therefore, although exact directions cannot be established, the inclusion of these trait measures improves confidence in conclusions regarding outcomes associated with social media use. Comparison of clinical and non-clinical groups allowed the between-group comparison of the impact of social media use. We decided not to actively track participants’ social media use due to (i) the costs associated with the passive monitoring of accounts; (ii) concerns that seeking permission to view social media accounts would affect recruitment rates; therefore, negatively impacting the sample size; (iii) the potential for risk identification on accounts and the resulting responsibility for the researcher to report such information; (iv) the potential for participants to change their behaviours on social media if they are aware that a researcher is monitoring their profiles; (v) monitoring would only allow the identification of content posting behaviours; not content consumption; and (vi) accessing participant social media profiles would enable the identification of posts written by others on the individual’s profile, who will not have provided consent for researchers to view their posts. ESM was used to reduce the likelihood of inaccurate retrospective recall due to the momentary nature of alerts; however, monitoring participants’ social media access and behaviours may have produced more reliable data due to the self-reported nature of the study. Additionally, we speculated that the detrimental impact of emotional self-disclosures may be related to the feedback individuals received; however, without clear knowledge of user responses to posts, firm conclusions cannot be drawn. Therefore, future research should seek to identify the responses to such disclosures to understand whether response type mediates the relationship between disclosure posting and outcome. It is also possible that the information participants were presented with when viewing social media profiles and newsfeeds may have contributed towards reductions in mood when using social media. Participants were not asked to provide details about the information they saw each time they accessed social media sites. Future research should expand on these findings by exploring whether type of content viewed contributes towards the impact of use and behaviours. The sample was limited to mostly White British participants so findings are unlikely to be generalisable. Moreover, clinicians may have been more likely to refer patients who had previously described positive or negative experiences of engaging with social media. The design of the study was correlational in nature and it is likely that other factors may have also contributed towards the impact of social media use. Therefore, future research should seek to experimentally manipulate social media use to explore whether the effect is directly attributable specifically to social media use and behaviours. Finally, a diagnosis of SMI was not found to moderate the relationship between social media use and mood; however, the sample size may have not been sufficient for moderation. Therefore, future research should explore the impact of an SMI diagnosis with a larger pool of participants. Clinical implications Despite finding that psychosis did not moderate the impact of social media use per se, reductions in mood after social media use are likely to be more damaging for people with psychosis due to reporting lower levels of mood initially. The negative consequence of social media engagement in both groups highlights the importance of continued consideration of the impact of social media in mental health settings. Specifically, clinicians should ensure that they are aware of and explore any potential issues clients face when using social media, particularly with regard to online self-disclosures. Additionally, the findings support the recent assertion made by the Royal Society for Public Health (2017) that social media websites should be used to discretely reach out to individuals who may be affected by content and signpost appropriate support options (66). Social networking components such as forums could be incorporated into digital health interventions for severe mental health problems to connect people with similar experiences and provide professional and peer support (67–72). However, our findings suggest that emotional disclosures via these platforms have the potential to elicit negative feelings. We speculated that the impact of such disclosures may be due to the absence of supportive feedback; therefore, these social networking components should be moderated to ensure individuals receive supportive responses when self-disclosing personal information. Finally, we identified significantly lower frequencies of social media use by people 567 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Social media use in psychosis who experience psychosis, which may be a potential barrier in the uptake of social networking components within DHIs. Further research is needed to explore the reasons for this comparatively low use and to identify whether these differences exist on a larger-scale. Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank Mr Austin Lockwood for his assistance in programming the survey used in this study and the participants who took part. This study formed part of a PhD studentship funded by the MRC Health eResearch Centre (HeRC), Farr Institute, UK (MR/K006665/1). Conflict of interest The authors confirm that they have no competing interests to declare. References 1. Boyd DM, Ellison NB. Social network sites: definition, history, and scholarship. J Comput Mediat Commun 2007;13:210–230. 2. Brusilovskiy E, Townley G, Snethen G, Salzer MS. Social media use, community participation and psychological well-being among individuals with serious mental illnesses. Comput Human Behav 2016;65:232–240. 3. Miller BJ, Stewart A, Schrimsher J, Peeples D, Buckley PF. How connected are people with schizophrenia? Cell phone, computer, email and social media use. Psychiatry Res 2015;225:458–463. 4. Berry N, Lobban F, Belousov M, Emsley R, Nenadic G, Bucci S. #WhyWeTweetMH. Understanding why people use Twitter to discuss mental health problems. J Med Internet Res 2017;19:e107. 5. Gay K, Torous J, Joseph A, Pandya A, Duckworth K. Digital technology use among individuals with schizophrenia: results of an online survey. JMIR Ment Health 2016;3: e15. 6. Matthews M, Murnane E, Snyder J et al. The double-edged sword: a mixed methods study of the interplay between bipolar disorder and technology use. Comput Human Behav 2017;75:288–300. 7. Berry N, Bucci S, Lobban F. Use of the internet and mobile phones for self-management of severe mental health problems: qualitative study of staff views. JMIR Ment Health 2017;4:e52. 8. Birnbaum ML, Rizvi AF, Correll CU, Kane JM, Confino J. Role of social media and the Internet in pathways to care for adolescents and young adults with psychotic disorders and non-psychotic mood disorders. Early Interv Psychiatry 2017;11:290–295. 9. Berry N, Lobban F, Emsley R, Bucci S. Acceptability of interventions delivered online and through mobile phones for people who experience severe mental health problems: a systematic review. J Med Internet Res 2016;18:e121. 10. Pantic I, Damjanovic A, Todorovic J et al. Association between online social networking and depression in high school students: behavioural physiology viewpoint. Psychiatr Danub 2012;24:90–93. 568 11. Primack BA, Shensa A, Escobar-Viera CG et al. Use of multiple social media platforms and symptoms of depression and anxiety: a nationally-representative study among U.S. young adults. Comput Human Behav 2017;69:1–9. 12. Shensa A, Escobar-Viera CG, Sidani JE, Bowman ND, Marshal MP, Primack BA. Problematic social media use and depressive symptoms among U.S. young adults: a nationally-representative study. Soc Sci Med 2017;182:150–157. 13. Benjanin N, Banjanin N, Dimitrijevic I, Pantic I. Relationship between internet use and depression: focus on physiological mood oscillations, social networking and online addictive behaviour. Comput Human Behav 2015;43:308–312. 14. Jelenchick LA, Eickhoff JC, Moreno MA. “acebook depression?” Social networking site use and depression in older adolescents. J Adolesc Health 2013;52:128–130. 15. Blomfield Neira CJ, Barber BL. Social networking site use: linked to adolescents’ social self-concept, self-esteem, and depressed mood. Austr J Psychol 2014;66:56–64. 16. Gonzales AL, Hancock JT. Mirror, mirror on my Facebook wall: effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2011;14:79–83. 17. Steinfield C, Ellison NB, Lampe C. Social capital, selfesteem, and use of online network sites: a longitudinal analysis. J Appl Dev Psychol 2008;29:434–445. 18. Vogel EA, Rose JP, Roberts LR, Eckles K. Social comparison, social media, and self-esteem. Psychol Pop Media Cult 2014;3:206–222. 19. Best P, Manktelow R, Taylor B. Online communication, social media and adolescent wellbeing: a systematic narrative review. Child Youth Serv Rev 2014;41:27–36. 20. Seabrook EM, Kern ML, Rickard NS. Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review. JMIR Ment Health 2016;3:e50. 21. Torous J, Keshavan M. The role of social media in schizophrenia: evaluating risks, benefits, and potential. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2016;29:190–195. 22. Kalbitzer J, Mell T, Bermpohl F, Rapp M, Heinz A. Twitter psychosis: a rare variation or a distinct syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis 2014;202:623. 23. Krishna N, Fischer BA, Miller M, Register-Brown K, Patchan K, Hackman A. The role of social media networks in psychotic disorders: a case report. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2013;35:323–331. 24. Nitzan U, Shoshan E, Lev-Ran S, Fennig S. Internet-related psychosis-a sign of the times. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 2011;48:207–211. 25. Veretilo P, Billick SB. Psychiatric illness and Facebook: a case report. Psychiatr Q 2012;83:385–389. 26. Bird JC, Waite F, Rowsell E, Fergusson EC, Freeman D. Cognitive, affective, and social factors maintaining paranoia in adolescents with mental health problems: a longitudinal study. Psychiatry Res 2017;257:34–39. 27. Lee SY. How do people compare themselves with others on social network sites? The case of Facebook. Comput Human Behav 2014;32:253–260. 28. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations 1954;7:117. 29. Gilbert P. Depression: the evolution of powerlessness. Guildford, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1992. 30. Manago AM, Graham MB, Greenfield PM, Salimkhan G. Self-presentation and gender on MySpace. Devl Psychol 2008;29:446–458. 31. Zhao S, Grasmuck S, Martin J. Identity construction on Facebook: digital empowerment in anchored relationships. Comput Human Behav 2008;24:1816–1836. 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Berry et al. 32. Gilbert P. The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: the role of the evaluation of social rank. Clin Psychol Psychother 2000;7:174–189. 33. Gilbert P, Price J, Allan S. Social comparison, social attractiveness and evolution: how might they be related? New Ideas Psychol 1995;13:149–165. 34. Appel H, Gerlach AL, Crusius J. The interplay between Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Curr Opin Psychol 2016;9:44–49. 35. de Vries DA, K€uhne R. Facebook and self-perception: individual susceptibility to negative social comparison on Facebook. Pers Individ Dif 2015;86:217–221. 36. Steers M-LN, Wickham RE, Acitelli LK. Seeing everyone else’s highlight reels: how Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. J Soc Clin Psychol 2014;33:701–731. 37. Vogel EA, Rose JP, Okdie BM, Eckles K, Franz B. Who compares despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation on social media use and its outcomes. Pers Individ Dif 2015;86:249–256. 38. Østergaard SD. Taking Facebook at face value: why the use of social media may cause mental disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2017;136:439–440. 39. Burke M, Marlow C, Lento T. Social network activity and social well-being. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems, Atlanta, USA, 2010. 40. Verduyn P, Ybarra O, Resibois M, Jonides J, Kross E. Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective wellbeing? A critical review. Soc. Issues Policy Rev 2017;11:274–302. 41. Kross E, Verduyn P, Demiralp E et al. Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS ONE 2013;8:e69841. 42. Verduyn P, Lee DS, Park J et al. Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: experimental and longitudinal evidence. J Exp Psychol 2015;144:480–488. 43. Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R. Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. In: Csikszentmihalyi M, ed. Flow and foundations of positive psychology. Dordrecht: Springer, 2014. 44. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988;54:1063–1070. 45. Donnellan MB, Kenny DA, Trzesniewski KH, Lucas RE, Conger RD. Using trait-state models to evaluate the longitudinal consistency of global self-esteem from adolescence to adulthood. J Res Pers 2012;46:634–645. 46. Rosenberg M. Conceiving the self. New York, NY: Basic Books, 1979. 47. Fenigstein A, Vanable PA. Paranoia and self-consciousness. J Pers Soc Psychol 1992;62:129–138. 48. Allan S, Gilbert P. A social comparison scale: psychometric properties and relationship to psychopathology. Pers Individ Diff 1995;19:293–299. 49. Jenkins-Guarnieri MA, Wright SL, Johnson B. Development and validation of a social media use integration scale. Psychol Pop Media Cult 2013;2:38–50. 50. Ross C, Orr ES, Sisic M, Arseneault JM, Simmering MG, Orr RR. Personality and motivations associated with Facebook use. Comput Human Behav 2011;2011:27. 51. Myin-Germeys I, van Os J, Schwartz JE, Stone AA, Delespaul PA. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry 2001;58:1137–1144. 52. Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M, McCabe M, Skouteris H et al. Does body satisfaction influence self-esteem in adolescents’ daily lives? An experience sampling study J Adolesc 2015;45:11– 19. 53. Thewissen V, Bentall RP, Oorschot M, Campo J, van Lierop T, Myin-Germeys I. Emotions, self-esteem, and paranoid episodes: an experience sampling study. Br J of Clin Psychol 2011;50:178–195. 54. Palmier-Claus JE, Myin-Germeys I, Barkus E et al. Experience sampling research in individuals with mental illness: reflections and guidance. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011;123:12–20. 55. Carter LA. Rigorous methods for the analysis, reporting and evaluation of ESM style data. PhD dissertation. Manchester (UK): University of Manchester, 2016. https:// www.research.manchester.ac.uk/portal/files/54589387/ FULL_TEXT.PDF 56. Seidman G. Self-presentation and belonging on Facebook: how personality influences social media use and motivations. Pers Individ Diff 2013;54:402–407. 57. Derlega VJ, Winstead BA, Wong P, Greenspan M. Self-disclosure and relationship development: an attributional analysis. In: Roloff ME, Miller GR, editors. Interpersonal processes: new directions in communication research. New York, NY: Sage, 1987. 58. Greene K, Derlega VL, Mathews A. Self-disclosure in personal relationships. In: Vangelisti A, Perlman D, editors. Cambridge handbook of personal relationships. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2006. 59. Laurenceau J-P, Barrett LF, Pietromonaco PR. Intimacy as an interpersonal process: the importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988;74:1238–1251. 60. Al-Kandari A, Melkote SR, Sharif A. Needs and motives of Instagram users that predict self-disclosure use: a case study of young adults in Kuwait. J Creative Commun 2016;11:85–101. 61. Bazarova NN, Choi YH. Self-disclosure in social media: extending the functional approach to disclosure motivations and characteristics on social network sites. J Commun 2014;64:635–657. 62. Hollenbaugh EE, Ferris AL. Facebook self-disclosure: examining the role of traits, social cohesion, and motives. Comput Human Behav 2014;30:50–58. 63. Orben AC, Dunbar RIM. Social media and relationship development: the effect of valence and intimacy of posts. Comput Human Behav 2017;73:489–498. 64. Forest AL, Wood JV. When social networking is not working: individuals with low self-esteem recognise but do not reap the benefits of self-disclosure on Facebook. Psychol Sci 2012;23:295–302. 65. Facebook. Mark Zuckerberg. Available from: https:// www.facebook.com/zuck/posts/10104413015393571 (Accessed 12 March 2018). 66. Royal Society for Public Health. 2017. #StatusOfMind, social media and young people’s mental health and wellbeing. https://www.rsph.org.uk/our-work/policy/social-med ia-and-young-people-s-mental-health-and-wellbeing.html (accessed 13 August 2017). 67. Alvarez-Jimenez M, Bendall S, Lederman R et al. On the HORYZON: moderated online social therapy for longterm recovery in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res 2014;143:143–149. 68. Aschbrenner KA, Naslund JA, Shevenell M, Kinney E, Bartels SJ. A pilot study of a peer-group lifestyle intervention enhanced with mHealth technology and social media for adults with serious mental illness. J Nerv Ment Dis 2016;204:483–486. 69. Naslund JA, Aschbrenner KA, Marsch LA, Bartels SJ. The future of mental health care: peer-to-peer support and social media. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2016;25:113–122. 569 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Social media use in psychosis 70. Naslund JA, Aschbrenner KA, Marsch LA, Bartels SJ. Feasibility and acceptability of Facebook for health promotion among people with serious mental illness. Digital Health 2016;2:1–10. 71. Naslund JA, Aschbrenner KA, Marsch LA, Bartels SJ. Facebook for supporting a lifestyle intervention for people with major depressive disorder, and schizophrenia: an exploratory study. Psychiatr Q 2018;89:81–94. 72. V€alim€aki M, Athanasopoulou C, Lahti M, Adams CE. Effectiveness of social media interventions for people with 570 schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res 2016;18:e92. Supporting Information Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of the article: Appendix S1. ESM assessments. 16000447, 2018, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/acps.12953 by Middlesex University, Wiley Online Library on [03/11/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Berry et al.