The Ripple Effect in Discipline Author(s): Jacob S. Kounin and Paul V. Gump Source: The Elementary School Journal , Dec., 1958, Vol. 59, No. 3 (Dec., 1958), pp. 158162 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.com/stable/999319 JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Elementary School Journal This content downloaded from 14.200.38.166 on Tue, 14 Feb 2023 23:37:23 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms JACOB S. KOUNIN PAUL V. GUMP Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan The Ripple Effect in Discipline Discipline is a serious concerngartens to of twenty-six representative many teachers, especially beginners. Detroit schools. In the study reported The teacher who seeks help in discihere, fifty-one undergraduates served as observers. The students began their pline is likely to get advice that draws observations on the first days of the heavily on lore. The counsel may carry the name of a respected authoritynew orschool year. the prestige of a widely accepted edThe observers were carefully inucational philosophy. structed on their assignment. They But how much advice on classroom were to note any incident in which discipline, even advice offered under a kindergartner watched the teacher such auspices, meets the test of correct ex- another child for misbehavior. perimentation? How many widelyThey ac- were to report in detail on three cepted beliefs and practices have been phases of each incident: the behavior upheld by careful research? of the watching child immediately be- fore the incident, the behavior of the In Detroit, we are studying classroom management (1). In one phase teacher and the child who was being of our study, we are paying special corrected during the incident, and the attention to the "ripple effect," behavior or of the watching child for two minutes after the incident. the influence that control techniques have-not on the children who are Four hundred and six such incidents being disciplined-but on the otherwere analyzed. In our analysis, we children who are watching and lis- classified the control technique itself, tening. Briefly, the problem may be put in this way: While the teacher is correcting Sally, what effect is the disciplinary measure having on Ruth, who is sitting nearby, taking in what is happening? Answers were sought in the kinder- the behavior of the watching child before the incident, and the behavior of the watching child after the incident. The control technique Three dimensions of the control techniques used by the teachers were 158 This content downloaded from 14.200.38.166 on Tue, 14 Feb 2023 23:37:23 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms DISCIPLINE 159 measured: clarity, firmness, which and the teacher expressed hostility roughness. Clarity involved the teacher's direc- tions to the children. How clearly or exasperation. If the teacher touched the child, the touch had more pressure than was necessary. If the teacher gave the child a warning look, the havior the teacher wanted to bring look was angry rather than serious. to an end? The samples in the study showed no did the directions define the misbe- A teacher might say: "Tommy, stop extremely harsh techniques. No child, it!" Or "Tommy, you can't do that!" for example, was shaken or spanked. Or "Tommy, that will do!" However emphatically uttered, these directions The children's reactions The children who watched while did not make it clear what Tommy was to stop doing. a classmate was being corrected reA teacher who wanted to make sponded in various ways, which we sure that a pupil understood what classified was in five categories. Sometimes expected of him might use one of and girls showed no reaction. boys several approaches. The teacher might They simply went about their busigive directions that defined the pupil's ness, making no observable response misbehavior: "Tommy, don't taketo the the episode. If the children hap- blocks away from Johnny while pened he's to be drawing when a classmate using them." Or the teacher might was adomished, they simply continued give the child an acceptable standard with their drawing. of behavior: "Tommy, in kindergarten At other times, children reacted we ask for things. We don't grab." sharply to the correction of a class- Or the teacher might tell Tommy mate. how They lost interest in what they to stop the misbehavior: "Tommy put had been doing and became worried, those blocks down and look at the picture books." Firmness involved how much "I- confused, and restless. This type of reaction was classified under "behavior disruption." mean-it" the teacher packed into theAt still other times, children redisciplinary technique. How did the sponded with a special effort to be teachers say "I mean it"? good. They stopped a misbehavior of By touching or guiding the child. their own, sat up taller, paid closer attention to the lesson, or tried in By speaking emphatically. By walking close to the child. Or by following some other way to show that they through, that is, by focusing steadily were not misbehaving. These reac- on the misbehaving child until he contions were grouped under "conform- formed. If the teacher brushed over ance." the trouble lightly, the correction con-Sometimes the correction had no deveyed little firmness. terrent effect whatsoever. Even though Roughness described techniques in a child had just seen a classmate cor- This content downloaded from 14.200.38.166 on Tue, 14 Feb 2023 23:37:23 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 160 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL December, 1958 rected for misbehaving, he launched rough techniques were followed by some mischief of his own. This re- an increase in behavior disruption. Sesponse was classified as "non-conformvere techniques did not make for "bet- ance." ter" behavior in the watching child. At times, children in the audience Severe techniques simply upset him. vacillated between conformance and Our study recognized that control non-conformance. During the two techniques alone do not determine how minutes after the teacher had corrected a watching child reacts. Other influa classmate, they both conformed and ences are also at work. misbehaved. We related the children's reactions The impact of the setting to the teachers' control techniques (2). We investigated three possibilities. When the teachers made it very clear First of all, we asked: "'What was the what they expected of a child, the chil-watching child doing just before the dren in the audience responded withincident?" Our next concern: Was the increased conformance and decreased watching child psychologically close non-conformance. When the teachers to the child who was being corrected? did not make it clear what they expected of the child they were correcting, the effect on the young observers was reversed, that is, they responded with less conformance and Was the child in the audience watching his misbehaving classmate with considerable interest? Finally, how long had the watching child been in kinder- garten? Children who were themselves mismore non-conformance. The probabili- ty level (3) for this difference, by the chi-square test, was .01. The clarity of the teachers' directions was plainly related to the responses of the children in the audi- behaving-or even innocently related to misbehavior-were much more re- sponsive as they watched the teachers' efforts to control than were the chil- dren who were free of any connection ence, but the firmness of the teachers' with misbehavior. Children who at technique, the researchers found, only the moment were free of misbehavior tended to be related to the reactions were quite likely to show no reacof these children. In other words, the tion. Children who were misbehaving knowledge that a control technique showed more conformance, more non- was firm or lacking in firmness did conformance, and markedly more vac- not enable us to predict how a watch- illation between conformance and non- conformance (probability level .001). Finally, we found a relation be- It was instructive to compare the tween the roughness of the control effects of clarity and firmness on the technique and the response of thevarious groups. The effects already watching child. Roughness did notnoted for clarity were obtained re- ing child would react. lead to increased conformance and de- gardless of whether or not the watch- creased non-conformance. Instead, ing child was associated with mis- This content downloaded from 14.200.38.166 on Tue, 14 Feb 2023 23:37:23 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms DISCIPLINE 161 formance and decreased non-conformbehavior. However, firmness affected ance. only groups that had some connection with misbehavior. In these groups, If high the teacher's behavior con firmness increased conformance and firmness, the watching children decreased non-conformance (probabil- times responded with increase formance and decreased non-con ity level .05). ance. This reaction occurrred if the The length of time the children had been in kindergarten, we found, af- watching children had been misbehavfected their reactions. On the first day ing or interested in a child who was the children were highly sensitive tomisbehaving. control techniques. They showed some If the teacher used rough techniques, outward reaction to 55 per cent ofthe children showed behavior disrup- all control incidents. On the next three tion but not conformance or nondays they reacted outwardly to onlyconformance. 34 per cent of the incidents (proba- It should be kept in mind that clarity in the teacher's directions led to bility level .001). greater conformance and less nonAmong our findings conformance in a new and unstructured situation. When children are new to To the extent that we can generalize kindergarten or to the teacher, they on cause and effect, the study indicates may be especially sensitive to his dithat the reaction of watching children rections and desires. As the child feels to a teacher's control of a misbehaving more at home in kindergarten and child is related to at least three factors. more at ease with the teacher, we would expect clarity to be less imOn the first day in kindergarten,portant. Several studies are now in First, the newness of the situation. watching children showed the strongprogress to check this expectation. est responses. Second, the behavior of the watch-Fact and lore ing children. Pupils who were themWhat meaning does the study have selves misbehaving or interested in for teachers of children who are just children who were misbehaving werebeginning kindergarten? It is clear that more likely to show the strongest re- a ripple effect does exist. What a actions; the particular response was teacher does to control children's bemost likely to be vacillation. havior affects the children who watch Third, the disciplinary technique it- as well as the children who are corself, that is, the clarity, the firmness, rected. and the roughness of the technique. The teacher who is interested in When the teacher made it clear controlling ripple effects can generally what behavior she objected to or what do so best by giving clear instructions behavior she expected, the watching to the child rather than by exerting children responded with increased conpressure on him. However, some in- This content downloaded from 14.200.38.166 on Tue, 14 Feb 2023 23:37:23 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 162 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL December, 1958 tensity or firmness is effective if the children who are watching are them- selves inclined to "deviancy." The study does not support the notion that the teacher must "bear down" on the first day or "make an example" of a child. Such steps are not necessary to induce conformity in children who are entering kindergarten. Nor does the study support the contention that roughness and anger are simply firmness intensified. Firmness and roughness are different qualities. Wit- ness the different effects they have on watching children. NOTE S of Education, Wayne State Unive nancial support has been provide National Institute of Mental He tional Institutes of Health, Publi Service, Grant 1066. 2. The inter-coder reliability on a control technique code was 78 p agreement; on a 34-item audience code, 83 per cent. Since the for collapsed to three dimensions and to five categories, the functional r would be even higher. To avoid bias, different teams coded the con niques used by teachers and the of the watching children. 3. Probability levels refer to the p that the differences obtained could to chance. For example, a probabil of .01 means that the difference o occur by chance less than 1. The research is sponsored bywould the Departin a hundred. ment of Educational Psychology, College This content downloaded from 14.200.38.166 on Tue, 14 Feb 2023 23:37:23 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms