1 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception The Distinction of Brain Processes Involved in Facial Recognition and Object Perception Jake Pedder (910488) Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne PSYC30018 : Neuroscience and the Mind Jesse Shapiro April 20th, 2020 Word count - 1579 Jake Pedder (910488) 2 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception The Distinctions of Brain Processes Involved in Facial Recognition and Object Perception There has been extensive debate and research into the brain mechanisms responsible for facial processing and if these mechanisms are unique, domain-specific areas or whether they are just a part of a domain-general object recognition function spread throughout the brain. Currently, the investigation into this area of study continues, with a considerable amount of papers published with alternative viewpoints, yet it is still not properly established if one specific area of the brain is responsible for facial processing or if several individual areas assist in the process. It was suggested in earlier studies that due to multiple different types of effects that facial processing was part of a separate mechanism in the brain to other object processing. Yin (1969) studied what was known as the inversion effect, whereby participants found it much easier to identify upside down non-face objects with their counterpart upright image, in contrast, they found it much more difficult to identify upsidedown faces compared to their upright position. Additionally, the composite effect which works by splitting the face image horizontally (across the nose) and sticking it together with a different person’s bottom half. Participants found it much easier to identify the top half individual when the image was misaligned with the bottom half compared to when the halves are fitted smoothly. This suggests faces undergo a holistic form of processing, again making them different to the way object images are processed (Young et al., 1987). It was widely accepted that because of the fact faces are uniquely susceptible to these effects, they must have a unique area of the brain for processing. However, Diamond & Carey (1986) found an alternative hypothesis for these previous results. They brought in participants from the general public and “dog breed experts” and found the inversion effect for images of dogs in the “dog experts” group and not in the control group. This gave way to the expertise Jake Pedder (910488) 3 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception hypothesis, which suggests that facial processing is not unique, as the mechanisms involved are also engaged by objects (with in class-similarity) in which people have become experts in. To attempt to resolve the debate Gauthier & Tarr (1997) developed Greebles, which they aimed to be face-like objects. The greebles were split into 2 genders and 5 families, identified by three differences in 3 features likened to a face (eyes, nose & mouth). Due to these features, Greebles were considered face-like objects and were therefore considered to be processed holistically like faces. The participants were then trained a group to become a “Greeble expert”, with approximately 3240 trails to achieve this status. These “Greeble experts” then showed faster and more accurate responses to greeble identification and a more prominent inversion effect, suggesting mechanisms in the brain for the processing of faces is not unique, with expertise training on face-like objects, similar results can be produce. This was a widely accepted viewpoint in research over many years, however it is made on one key basis, that Greebles accurately represent faces and therefore they process holistically. Currently, new findings, theories and case studies have brought to light enough evidence to dispute this assumption of Greebles accurately representing faces and indicates that Greebles use brain mechanisms responsible for object-processing rather than facial recognition. Firstly, it is important to consider the extensive findings of the scientific fMRI study on the fusiform face area (FFA) made in numerous experiments. The knowledge of the FFA has expanded over the recent years and is now known to play a key role in facial perception when identifying a person’s mood, age, sex and other important factors in usually a splitsecond timeframe. Advances in the field began in the 1990’s, with fusiform areas responding more greatly to faces over everyday objects, houses, letter strings and flowers (Kanwisher et al., 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997; Puce at al., 1996). There is 3 specific pieces of evidence that suggest the FFA responds to faces specifically and not other objects. The FFA responds to all Jake Pedder (910488) 4 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception kinds of facial stimuli at different levels of intensity for example drawings of faces (Spiridon & Kanwisher, 2002) and two-toned “Mooney faces” (Kanwisher et al., 1998), will still cause activation of the FFA on the fMRI. Secondly, the activation of the FFA is nearly double when shown an upright facial image to when shown an inverted facial image (Kanwisher et al. 1998). Lastly, for stimuli in which a face is presented to one of the participants eyes and nonface is presented to the other eye, the FFA has a higher response when the participants perceives a face over when they do not even though retinal stimulation is unchanged (Tong et al., 1998; Pasley et al., 2004; Williams et al. 2004). Additionally, it is key to understand the neuropsychological basis for that indicates the separation of these mechanisms. Farah’s (1990) study which reviewed all cases of visual agnosia proposed two independent recognition systems, a holistic processing system (associated with face recognition), and a structural processing system (associated with object recognition). This fits with the evidence of a double dissociation system, whereby people with different types of visual agnosia can have different symptoms. Importantly, a person with prosopagnosia shows impairment in recognising familiar faces that they have seen before yet show no impairment of recognizing objects (occurs usually with damage to fusiform gyrus). Juxtaposed to this, there exists object agnosics like the induvial termed CK, a man whose friends and family wondered after a closed-head injury why CK lost all interest in his toy soldier collection? This led to tests being conducted on CK which consisted of, inversion effects of faces, recognizing cartoon faces and perceiving faces made up of individual objects (Moscovitch, et al., 1997). The results found that CK had normal intact facial recognition and was always able to distinguish the faces in images presented but was usually unaware that they were made up of different objects or not. This led the experimenters to propose two separate mechanisms responsible for image processing, a Jake Pedder (910488) 5 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception holistic, face-specific system (intact for CK) and a part-based object-recognition system (damaged for CK), this supported the findings of Farahs’s (1990) study (Moscovitch, et al., 1997). Similarly, a case study of a man named Edward who had a life of difficulties not recognizing faces, suffered no serious head trauma and scored normally on vision tests while also being very intelligent (PhD in physics) struggled to recognize familiar faces. Edward was a prosopagnosic, however he could recognise Greebles. He was in the normal range for speed, accuracy and additionally Edward was fully trained using the Gauthier et al. (1999) rapid expertise method from their study and was able to become an expert in Greeble identification (Duchaine et al., 2004). Edward and his abilities directly contradict the findings of Gauthier et al. (1999) and their expertise hypothesis, due to the fact that Edward could become an expert in greebles, while still being prosopagnosic, this implicates that Greebles are not processed with the same mechanisms as faces. Case studies like this are very important for the understanding of the brain and the mechanisms that work in tangent, yet with all the expansive number of case studies of prosopagnosia, not one person shows impairment of facial recognition and areas of expertise, yet the expertise hypothesis implies these processes use the same area of the brain (McKone et al., 2007). This leads us to discuss are Greebles face-like objects processed in the FFA under the same mechanisms as faces if we become “Greeble experts”? Gauthier et al. (1999) reported an activation in the FFA region which increased with Greeble training to become an expert, and thus supported their expert hypothesis indicating that areas in the brain for processing faces are not unique, yet this simply does not fit the findings of the previous case studies, experiments and fMRI findings mentioned. So, what explains the findings of Gauthier et al’s. (1999) study? This can be explained by more detailed, recent research conducted by Kanwisher et al. (2006), who interpreted Gauthier et al’s. (1999) data much differently. Jake Pedder (910488) 6 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception Noting that Gauthier et al. (1999) defined the FFA as a large square ROI over a centimetre on a side, Kanwisher et al. (2006) guarantees the inclusion of voxels with these parameters in the fMRI measure, which would unintentionally be skewing the data gathered by the fMRI. This then suggest the data collected by Gauthier et al. (1999) from what they believed to be the FFA may be really arising from the neighbouring fusiform body area (FBA; Peelen & Downing 2005). Conclusion While it might not be set in stone if faces are special and have their own unique part of the brain for processing, the recent findings in key studies certainly seem to be favouring the direction of a domain-specific mechanism for facial-processing. Improved accuracy and increased use of fMRI machines to scan and evaluate the FFA while processing objects and faces have been essential in this shift in direction. The neuropsychological case studies of agnosics and prosopagnposics also have played a key role in the development of a double dissociative framework which indicates the separation of brain areas. Additionally, the counter-research into the expertise hypothesis which creates questionability of the accuracy in previous findings. The points all demonstrate different areas in the brain for processing objects like Greebles and human faces. Future research could expand on the unique case studies, aim to find more people who are visually agnosic and prosopagnosic to develop a more consistent understanding of the areas of the brain that are damaged. Jake Pedder (910488) 7 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception References Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(2), 107. Duchaine, B. C., Dingle, K., Butterworth, E., & Nakayama, K. (2004). Normal greeble learning in a severe case of developmental prosopagnosia. Neuron, 43(4), 469-473. Farah, M. J. (1990). Visual agnosia: Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision. Issues in the biology of language and cognition. The MIT Press. Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. J. (1997). Becoming a “Greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vision research, 37(12), 1673-1682. Gauthier, I., Behrmann, M., & Tarr, M. J. (1999). Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition?. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(4), 349-370. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of neuroscience, 17(11), 4302-4311. Kanwisher, N., Tong, F., & Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition, 68(1), B1-B11. Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for Jake Pedder (910488) 8 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 361(1476), 2109-2128. Mccarthy, G., Luby, M., Gore, J., & Goldman-Rakic, P. (1997). Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77(3), 1630-1634. McKone, E., Kanwisher, N., & Duchaine, B. C. (2007). Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces?. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(1), 8-15. Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., & Behrmann, M. (1997). What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 9(5), 555604. Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004). Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42(1), 163-172. Puce, A., Allison, T., Asgari, M., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of neuroscience, 16(16), 5205-5215. Spiridon, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2002). How distributed is visual category information in human occipito-temporal cortex? An fMRI study. Neuron, 35(6), 1157-1165. Jake Pedder (910488) 9 Different Psychological Mechanisms Responsible for Facial Perception and Object Perception Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Vaughan, J. T., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). Binocular rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex. Neuron, 21(4), 753-759. Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 141-145 Jake Pedder (910488)