American Journal of Orthopsychiatry © 2023 Global Alliance for Behavioral Health and Social Justice 2023, Vol. 93, No. 2, 156–165 https://doi.org/10.1037/ort0000665 Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Among Syrian Refugee Girls: Risk and Protective Factors Vivian Khamis This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Department of Education, American University of Beirut Exposure to war and forced migration have been widely linked to child subsequent adaptation. What remains sparse is research spanning multiple risk and protective factors and examining their unique, and relative implications to difficulties on emotion dysregulation in refugee girls. This study investigated the mechanisms through which emotion dysregulation in Syrian refugee girls is impacted by exposure to war traumas, comorbidities, and other risk and protective factors such as coping styles, family relationships, and school environment. The sample consisted of 539 Syrian refugee girls who ranged in age from 7 to 18 years attending public schools in various governorates in Lebanon and Jordan. Two school counselors carried out the interviews with children at school. Results indicated that war trauma and the combination of comorbidities associated with negative coping styles could lead to an overall state of emotion dysregulation in refugee girls. Enhanced understanding of the mechanistic role of risk and protective factors in contributing to emotion dysregulation in refugee girls may contribute to the development of effective interventions to target the psychological effects of the refugee experience. Public Policy Relevance Statement This study helps policymakers recognize that exposure to war traumas are risk factors for experiencing difficulties in emotion regulation among refugee girls and that the resulting comorbidities indicate a need for appropriate mental health services. In addition, policymakers should pay increased attention to the types of programs and policies that should be adopted, such as compensation laws, bills of rights, provision of services, and assistance programs. T often exposed to high stress that has the potential to negatively affect their development (Boswall & Al Akash, 2015; International Rescue Committee [IRC], 2013, 2014). They are frequently exposed to a variety of stressors such as socioeconomic disadvantages, changes in family structure and functioning, loss of social support, difficulty in accessing education, living in very crowded places, experiencing hostility and racism, and navigating problems caused by cultural differences. Furthermore, they are exposed to gender-related problems such as forced early marriage, harassment and exploitation, and survival sex (Bartels et al., 2021; Hattar-Pollara, 2019; IRC, 2014; United Nations Population Fund [UNFPA], 2017). According to UNFPA (2017), approximately 35% of Syrian refugee girls are now married before they reach the age of 18. Their dual nature as refugees and as females, often means that Syrian girls must face stressful experiences, beginning with child marriage (El Arab & Sagbakken, 2019) and continuing to current inequities in social resources and barriers to education (Hassan et al., 2016; HattarPollara, 2019). Cultural norms often limit girls from accessing medical or mental health services related to rape, sexual, or domestic violence (Hattar-Pollara, 2019; IRC, 2014). Exposure to the atrocities of war and disruption in their emotional processes (Khamis, 2019) place refugee girls at risk for maladaptive outcomes (e.g., internalizing problems, aggressive and antisocial he protracted crisis in Syria has led to the displacement of over 5.5 million Syrian people into neighboring countries. Of those, approximately 825,081 reside in Lebanon, whereas 676,496 reside in Jordan (United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees [UNHCR], 2022). Refugee children, and in particular girls, experience a wide range of psychiatric disorders related to their exposure to war, violence, torture, and forced migration (Bennouna et al., 2020; Guruge & Butt, 2015; Kirmayer et al., 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema & Aldao, 2011; Wilson et al., 2010). Syrian refugee girls in resettlement contexts are This article was published Online First January 12, 2023. Vivian Khamis received funding from Grant FAS.VK. 2 from the University Research Board at the American University of Beirut. Ethics approval for the study was obtained from the American University of Beirut; institutional review board number FAS.VK. 11. The author especially grateful to Syrian refugee children and adolescents who participated in the research. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Vivian Khamis, Department of Education, American University of Beirut, Bliss Street, P.O. Box 11-0236, Beirut 1107 2020, Lebanon. Email: vk07@ aub.edu.lb 156 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN REFUGEE GIRLS 157 behavior). The mental health and psychosocial well-being of refugee children are of particular concern because their experiences of insecurity occur at a formative stage in their development (Reed et al., 2012). The preponderance of studies indicates that girls are at highest risk of affective disorders, mental health problems, and difficulties with emotions (Andersson et al., 2010; Scherer et al., 2020). However, studies that investigate the variables that shape the lives of Syrian refugee girls as it pertains to mental health and psychosocial well-being are scarce. Therefore, this research posits that emotion regulation in refugee girls is a function of their responses to risk and protective factors, with negative outcomes linked to exposure to risk factors such as war trauma and positive outcomes linked to protective factors such as coping styles (Lustig et al., 2004) and family and school environment (Berman, 2001). inferences from them. Refugees’ psychosocial needs and ways of coping are related to cultural concepts of mental health as well as to influencing factors in host countries (Hassan et al., 2015; Silove et al., 2017), which underlines the importance of investigating the specific coping styles of Syrian refugee girls in Lebanon and Jordan. To date, only one empirical study has examined how different forms of coping may affect emotion dysregulation in refugee children (Khamis, 2019). A number of coping strategies were identified as common to emotion dysregulation, including social withdrawal, self-criticism and resignation, blaming others, and wishful thinking (Khamis, 2019). Further research on coping and emotion regulation may possibly identify resilience factors that could be incorporated and evaluated in transdiagnostic models of preventive interventions and psychological treatments for a range of psychological problems and disorders in refugee girls (Compas et al., 2013; Trosper et al., 2009). Emotion Dysregulation and Comorbidities Emotion Dysregulation and Family Relations Growing evidence indicates that exposure to war exerts a pernicious influence on refugees’ emotion regulation (Doolan et al., 2017; Khamis, 2019; Nickerson et al., 2016, 2017; Specker & Nickerson, 2019). The theoretical and empirical literature highlights the role of emotion dysregulation in diverse forms of psychopathology in children. Specifically, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) has been found to be positively associated with overall emotion dysregulation and the specific dimensions of lack of emotional acceptance; difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors and controlling impulsive behaviors when upset; limited access to emotion regulation strategies; and lack of emotional clarity (Ehring & Quack, 2010; Tull et al., 2007). Furthermore, research provides evidence of heightened emotion dysregulation among children who are at risk for high levels of neuroticism (Kokkonen & Pulkkinen, 2001; Mącik et al., 2019; Weinstock & Whisman, 2006) and emotional and behavioral disorders (e.g., Khan et al., 2005; Lahey, 2009; Martel & Nigg, 2006). The ability to manage emotions holds promise for favorable outcomes for children and adolescents in high-risk contexts (Buckner et al., 2003). Although the aforementioned findings provide support for a relationship between emotion dysregulation and children’s mental health outcomes, a better understanding of these comorbidities is important in order to promote resilience and prevent emotion regulation problems among refugee girls. Another variable that may influence the course of refugee children’s adjustment to war atrocities and displacement is family relations. Children’s emotion regulation abilities are influenced by parent, child, and family characteristics (Morris et al., 2007). Therefore, the family relationship may be critical in protecting children from maladaptive outcomes during times of stress, because this relationship offers unique opportunities to develop emotion regulation skills to manage stress and other difficult emotions (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1998; Lindsey, 2020; Morris et al., 2007). It has been postulated that family relationships affect children’s emotional development (e.g., Davies & Cummings, 2010) and that these relationships set a developmental context for emotion regulation in children (Morris et al., 2007). While family support and positive family relationships have been identified as protectors from stress during the phases of the refugee experience, family conflict negatively influenced children’s emotion regulation (Iraklis, 2021b; Pieloch et al., 2016). Families in the Arab world have been characterized as being collective and authoritarian (Dwairy, 1998). As an integral part of their families, girls may be affected by these patterns of family interactions. Research on refugee children revealed that girls reported that they were not as equally valued or supported as their male siblings and that they are perceived as a burden on their families. Their gendered role as females offered a constricted range of possibilities and opportunities, and restrained and confined their lives and behaviors within clearly prescribed expectations that were reinforced with verbal threats and physical punishment. The mere rumor of violations of these expectations was invariably and swiftly met with punishment, beatings, and severe restrictions (Hattar-Pollara, 2019). Hence, examining how refugee girls perceive their family environment and how these perceptions affect their emotion regulation remains an important gap in refugee children’s developmental research. Emotion Dysregulation and Coping Strategies The ability to successfully regulate affective states is central to mental health, and indeed, it has been noted that coping figures prominently in a healthy lifestyle and quality of life (Goldberger & Breznitz, 1993). Despite the enormity of the impact of war trauma and forced migration on children’s developmental trajectories, there is a lack of research on coping among refugees (Siriwardhana et al., 2014). The strategies refugee children use to regulate their emotions are the focus of a great deal of recent theoretical and empirical work, and a number of specific strategies have been argued to be generally adaptive or maladaptive (see reviews in Gross & Thompson, 2007; Kring & Sloan, 2010). However, these studies were qualitative in nature and had a small sample size (Iraklis, 2021a, 2021b; Maegusuku-Hewett et al., 2007; Ní Raghallaigh & Gilligan, 2010), hence limiting our capacity to draw general Emotion Dysregulation and School Environment The struggles associated with the integration of refugee children into the education system have been noted to include problems with learning the Lebanese and Jordanian curriculum as well as challenges of adapting to new ways of learning in a context where they This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 158 KHAMIS increasingly face exclusion and discrimination (Alkhawaldeh, 2018). The majority of Syrian students attend second-shift classes at public schools (Alkhawaldeh, 2018; UNHCR, 2018) which may affect not only academic aspects of school life but also extracurricular activities and general discipline as well as the ability of refugee students to socially integrate. Refugees’ experience in schools also affected whether they stayed in school or dropped out. Children and parents often cited bullying and violence in schools as contributing to dropping out (Norwegian Refugee Council, 2020). Studies of the general student population indicate strong associations between school climate and indicators of mental health across a range of academic, behavioral, and socioemotional outcomes; and in early adolescence, a positive school climate is predictive of better psychological health (Thapa et al., 2013). In addition, a growing body of literature has documented the central role that schools play in supporting the adjustment of resettled refugees (Bennouna et al., 2019; Khamis, 2019). In a systematic review, Fazel et al. (2005) noted that effective schools and teachers have the potential to promote resilience in refugee children by becoming the focal point for educational, social and emotional development. Support from teachers and peers has been identified as a protector from stress during the phases of the refugee experience (Berman, 2001), but the mechanisms of such associations are not yet fully elucidated. Alternatively, researchers have revealed that refugees are often subject to various modes of discrimination and bullying (Montgomery & Foldspang, 2008; Schwartz et al., 2010). Researchers have indicated that postmigration experiences of discrimination, hostility, and bullying in schools are commonly reported and represent a significant threat to the mental well-being of refugee children (Fazel et al., 2015; Kandemir et al., 2018; Samara et al., 2020). The influence of the school environment on refugees’ mental health is insufficiently investigated because the emphasis has been placed on what children bring with them when they enter schools (e.g., children’s social background) rather than on school environment or the complex interaction between multiple influences (Montgomery, 2010; Porter & Haslam, 2005). Research that investigates the relation between school environment and emotion regulation are limited. They indicated that the student–teacher relationship is related to emotion regulation and academic success (Fried, 2011; Graziano et al., 2007). In addition, perceived supportive school climate, support from adults in school and at home, and peer belonging were each independently associated with better emotional health (Emerson et al., 2022). To my knowledge, no study to date has examined the impact of school environment on emotion regulation in refugee girls. It is important to recognize that students’ and teachers’ emotion regulation are influenced by the emotional climate of the school (Fried, 2011; Hargreaves, 2000). Given the established importance of the postresettlement environment and the significance of school in the daily lives of refugee children, there are compelling reasons to study school environment as a predictor of emotion regulation in refugee girls. This will allow clear directives and actionable recommendations regarding how climate can support good mental health and resettlement outcomes for refugee girls. A widely used theoretical model that generates insight in the multiple factors associated with developmental outcomes in refugee children is the ecological model of Bronfenbrenner (Ager, 1993; Arakelyan & Ager, 2021). The Bronfenbrenner (1979, 1995) human ecology model provides a useful theoretical framework for this study. This model primarily focuses on the social contexts in which children live and the people who influence their development. Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory identifies five environmental systems that range from close interpersonal interactions to broadbased influences of culture (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006). The focus of this study is on the microsystem setting, which is the most immediate environmental social context containing the developing refugee girl, including the family and school. This study aims to further elucidate the mechanisms through which emotion dysregulation in refugee girls is impacted by exposure to war traumas, comorbidities and other risk and protective factors such as coping styles, family relationships and school environment. Specifically, it was hypothesized that self-reported rates of traumatic events would be positively associated with emotion regulation difficulties including lack of awareness of emotional responses, lack of clarity of emotional responses, nonacceptance of emotional responses, limited access to emotion regulation strategies perceived as effective, difficulties controlling impulses when experiencing negative emotions, and difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors when experiencing negative emotions. Risk and protective factors were then examined as predictors of emotion dysregulation using multiple regression. It was hypothesized that higher levels of war traumas and comorbidities (i.e., PTSD, neuroticism, and emotional and behavioral disorders) and negative coping styles, would predict difficulties in emotion regulation among refugee girls. Conversely, older age, a longer resettlement period, positive coping style, positive family relationships, and school environments were expected to predict more emotion regulation in refugee girls. Method Setting and Participants Data were derived from a large study on the educational and psychosocial status of Syrian refugee children and adolescents residing in two neighboring countries: Lebanon and Jordan. The study was conducted in ten public schools in Lebanon across three sites: Beirut, Bekaa, and Mount Lebanon, as well as ten public schools in Jordan across three sites: Amman, Zarqa, and Balqa. Five hundred and thirty-nine refugee girls (aged 7 to 17 years) were included in this analysis. Informed assent and consent were obtained from the girls and their parents, respectively. They were given a full explanation of the study and were assured of the anonymity and confidentiality of their responses. Ethics approval for the study was obtained from the American University of Beirut. All instruments were translated from English into Arabic and then back translated by professionals except for the trauma exposure scale, which was constructed in Arabic for the purpose of this study. Two trained psychologists between March and June 2017 administered the questionnaires in an interview format with the girls at school. Completion of the interview took approximately from 35 to 45 min. Instrumentation Personal History Form. The personal history form collected demographic data on refugee girls such as age, grade level, host country, and time spent in host country since they fled from Syria. War-Related Trauma. Trauma exposure scale was used to assess the number of war-related trauma experienced by children EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN REFUGEE GIRLS This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. and adolescents during the war (Khamis, 2019). The scale is composed of 17 items scored as 1 “‘yes”’ and 0 “‘no”’. Responses are summed to arrive at a total scale score. Scores vary from 0 (no trauma exposure at all) to 18 (high trauma exposure). Examples of the items are a family member, a relative or a close friend was killed during the war, our house was bombarded or destructed, hearing the sounds of rocket attacks, shelling and bombardment, witnessing people injured and was forced to live in a refugee camp. Cronbach is for the total scale in this sample is 0.85. Emotion Dysregulation. The present study operationalized emotion dysregulation using a short form version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). The DERS–short form (DERS-SF) is 18-item questionnaire that measures emotion regulation difficulties such as (a) lack of awareness of emotional responses, (b) lack of clarity of emotional responses, (c) nonacceptance of emotional responses, (d) limited access to emotion regulation strategies perceived as effective, (e) difficulties controlling impulses when experiencing negative emotions, and (f) difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors when experiencing negative emotions. Participants responded to each item on a 5-point scale from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always) with higher summed scores indicating greater emotion regulation difficulties. Internal consistency in this study for the total DERS-SF scale was high (α = 0.84), and total scores ranged from 18 to 90 (M = 81.69, SD = 26.24). Behavioral and Emotional Disorder. Behavioral and emotional disorders were assessed by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire–SDQ (Child Form). It contains four problem subscales with five items each assessing emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems (Goodman, 1997). Items are rated on a 3-point scale (not true; somewhat true; certainly true) and summed across the subscales to give a total problems score (0–40), with higher scores indicating poorer mental health. The SDQ has high internal reliability (α = 0.87; Khamis, 2019). Neuroticism. The neuroticism scale of Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968) assessed neuroticism. Children were asked to complete the 19-item Arabic version of the neuroticism scale (El Khalek, 1978), which asked children to answer yes or no questions about their negative affectivity. Composite scores could range from 0 to 19, with higher scores indicating higher neuroticism. Cronbach’s α for the total scale in this sample was 0.87. PTSD. PTSD was assessed by using the diagnostic criteria for an assessment of PTSD as outlined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). A structured clinical interview was used to ensure coverage of all the relevant signs and symptoms of PTSD. The PTSD module of the structured clinical interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition has been previously used on children and adolescents in the Arab world, and the interrater kappa coefficients measuring the reliability of interviewers was .90 for current and lifetime PTSD (Khamis, 2005, 2008, 2012). The characteristic symptoms of PTSD included reexperiencing the traumatic event, avoiding 159 stimuli associated with the trauma and experiencing a lack of general responsiveness, and experiencing symptoms of increased arousal. Coping Strategies. The Kidcope (Spirito et al., 1988) is an inventory designed to assess coping strategies employed by children. Children have to report the major problem experienced in their lifetimes and are then asked to indicate if they have used the 10 general coping strategies included in the inventory. The Kidcope targets positive coping styles such as cognitive restructuring, problem solving, emotional regulation, and social support, whereas the negative coping style includes distraction, withdrawal, criticizing self, blaming others, wishful thinking, and resignation. Cronbach’s α for the total scale in this sample is .79. Family Relationships. Family relationships were assessed by the relationship dimension of the Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos, 2002). The Relationship domain has three subscales that account for 27 items with a true–false response format: Cohesion (the degree of commitment and support family members provide for each other), Expressiveness (the extent to which family members are encouraged to express their feelings directly), and Conflict (the amount of openly expressed anger and conflict among family members). Representative items for the relationship domain include “In my family, we really help and support each other,” “We are a close and cohesive family,” “We tend to lose our tempers a lot in our family,” “We fight a lot,” and “Family members sometimes hit each other.” Cronbach’s α for the family relationship scale in this sample is .86. School Environment. The School Environment Scale (Khamis, 2015) is an instrument designed to evaluate the school environment based on students’ perceptions. It addresses 10 school experiences over three broad domains, including social support received from teachers, peers, and friends (emotional support, advice, encouragement, companionship, and morale boosting); meaningful and genuine relationships with peers, and instructional and academic support. Cronbach’s α for the total scale is .87. Statistical Analysis Descriptive statistics was used to investigate sample characteristics. Pearson correlation was conducted to investigate the relationship between difficulties in emotion regulation and the study variables. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was employed to examine differences between high and low levels of war trauma on the six dimensions of difficulties in emotion regulation. Then hierarchical regression was used to predict difficulties in emotion regulation. Model 1 included only the girls’ age, war trauma and time lapse since resettlement. Model 2 included the comorbidities (PTSD, behavioral and emotional disorders, and neuroticism) as well as the variables in Model 1. Model 3 included all the variables in Model 2 plus the coping strategies (i.e., positive and negative coping). Model 4 included all of the variables in Model 3 plus the family relationships processes (i.e., cohesion, expressiveness, conflict). Model 5 included all of the variables in Model 4 plus the school environment. The predictor blocks were entered in the order listed. This order was based upon chronology of occurrence. The early entry of a variable block into the regression equation provided a statistics control for the 160 KHAMIS p < .001, difficulties controlling impulses when experiencing negative emotions t(537) = −6.77, p < .001, and difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors when experiencing negative emotions t(537) = −5.93, p < .001 compared to their counterparts with lower levels of trauma (see Table 1). There was no significant main effect of war trauma on lack of awareness of emotional responses t(537) = −.93, p <. .35 (see Table 2). impact of those variables upon difficulties in emotion regulation. Analyses were conducted using PASW Statistics 27.0 (IBM Corporation Released, 2020). Results This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Preliminary Analyses Distributions of scales were normal and scatterplots revealed no multivariate outliers. Means, standard deviations, and correlations are presented in Table 1. While the bivariate correlations indicated that emotion dysregulation had small association with PTSD, it had strong association with neuroticism and emotional and behavioral disorders. In addition, a moderate positive relationship was found between emotion dysregulation and negative coping. In analyzing family relationships, family cohesion and expressiveness were negatively related to emotion dysregulation whereas family conflict was positively related to emotion dysregulation. A moderate negative relationship was found between school environment and emotion dysregulation (see Table 1). Prediction of Emotion Dysregulation Table 3 indicates that the model containing all five predictor blocks accounts for 43.9% of emotion dysregulation variance. In the first step, girls’ age, war trauma, and time spent in host country collectively accounted for 16.9% of the variance in emotion dysregulation among refugee girls. Of these variables, age and war trauma had statistically significant b weight. Adding the comorbidities in the second step did produce a significant increase in the amount of variance explained in emotion dysregulation. Only emotional and behavioral disorders and neuroticism were positively related to emotion dysregulation and accounted for 25% of additional variance, (see Table 3). However, when the comorbidities entered the model in the second step, the age of girls and their exposure to war trauma became nonsignificant. In the third regression step, coping strategies produced an increment of 1.4%, in the variance explained; while negative coping stood out as a significant predictor, positive coping was not significant. Emotion dysregulation in refugee girls was associated with greater use of coping styles such as distraction, withdrawal, criticizing self, blaming others, wishful thinking, and resignation. Adding the family relationships in the fourth step produced an increment of .003 indicating that family cohesion, expressiveness, and conflict were not significant predictors of emotion dysregulation addition. Similarly, school environment appeared to be a nonsignificant predictor (.004) of emotion dysregulation in the fifth step. Therefore, the resulting effect sizes were medium for age and war trauma and comorbidities, whereas it was small for coping strategies, family relationships, and school environment (see, Cohen, 1988). War Trauma and Emotion Dysregulation The amount of exposure to war atrocities was examined in more detail by dividing participants into two groups using median splits for high/low exposure to war trauma (based on the war trauma measure). Girls with high level of trauma exposure and low level of trauma exposure were compared to determine whether the two groups differed on the various dimensions of emotion dysregulation. MANOVA was employed to examine differences between refugee girls with high levels of trauma and low levels of trauma on the six dimensions of difficulties in emotion regulation. Results indicated a significant multivariate effect, Wilks’ λ = 0.869, F(1, 539) = 13.41, p < .001. The univariate analyses for the war trauma effect revealed that refugee girls with higher levels of trauma exposure reported greater lack of clarity of emotional responses t(537) = −5.58, p < .001, nonacceptance of emotional responses t(537) = −7.12, p < .001, limited access to emotion regulation strategies perceived as effective t(537) = −6.87, Table 1 Intercorrelations of Predictor and Outcome Variables Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. M SD Age Time lapse Trauma Positive coping Negative coping Family cohesion Family expressiveness Family conflict School environment Emotion dysregulation PTSD Behavior and emotion disorder Neuroticism 1 2 — .05 — .21** −.15** −.12** −.05 .15** −.02 −.21** −.03 −.18** .07 .20** −.02 −.30** −.05 .28** −.08 .04 −.19** .28** −.05 .34** −.10* 11.30 49.56 2.60 15.24 Note. PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder. * p < .05. ** p < .01. 3 4 5 6 — −.09* .24** −.35** −.17** −.17** −.40** .35** .25** .50** .45** 9.61 4.05 — .12** .17** .16** −.26** −.26** −.06 .04 −.16** .02 2.79 1.26 — −.15** −.15** .21** .21** .32** .12** .19** .37** 2.36 1.45 — .22** −.47** .41** −.24** −.07 −.44** −.33** 6.46 1.43 7 8 9 10 — −.48** — .26** −.45** — −.24** .28** −.36** — −.19** .06 .01 .21** −.27** .44** −.51** .53** −.26** .28** −.38** .60** 5.39 2.25 16.37 50.21 1.67 1.90 3.72 11.41 11 12 13 — .19** — .29** .57** — .43 11.11 7.81 .49 6.56 4.74 161 EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN REFUGEE GIRLS Table 2 Mean Difference of Syrian Refugee Girls on the Dimensions of Emotion Dysregulation Across High/Low Levels of War-Related Trauma (n = 539) This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Refugee girls High trauma (299) Low trauma (240) Variable M SD M SD Limited access to emotion regulation strategies perceived as effective Nonacceptance of negative emotions Difficulties controlling impulsive behaviors Inability to engage in goal-directed behaviors Lack of emotional awareness Lack of emotional clarity 9.31 2.94 7.68 2.43 9.12 8.60 9.70 8.12 8.90 3.01 2.61 3.38 3.25 3.44 7.42 7.20 8.13 7.88 7.43 2.38 2.03 2.59 2.60 2.45 Discussion This study investigated the mechanisms through which emotion dysregulation is impacted by exposure to war traumas, comorbidities, and other risk and protective factors such as coping styles, family relationships, and school environment. The present study suggests that exposure to war traumas are risk factors for emotion dysregulation in refugee girls. Consistent with previous research, trauma exposure was significantly associated with lack of clarity of emotional responses, nonacceptance of emotional responses, limited access to emotion regulation strategies perceived as effective, difficulties controlling impulses when experiencing negative emotions, and difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors when experiencing negative emotion. However, trauma exposure was not associated with lack of awareness of emotional responses (Nickerson et al., 2015). This finding calls for future research that will assess a wide range of traumatic war-related events in a broad range of Syrian refugee girls who experienced events at different times and in different geopolitical contexts (Hollifield et al., 2006). Consistent with previous studies, the results also indicated no substantial effect of the resettlement period on emotion dysregulation in refugee girls (Amin et al., 2020; Jensen et al., 2014). That the prevalence of emotion difficulties was higher in older refugee girls was in line with the literature as emotion regulation difficulties are expected to be present in normative developing adolescents to some degree, and impairing symptoms can be particularly frequent in adolescent females (Stringaris & Goodman, 2009). Consistent with previous research, emotion dysregulation (as a unitary construct) was directly associated with both neuroticism (Kokkonen & Pulkkinen, 2001; Mącik et al., 2019; Weinstock & Whisman, 2006) and emotional and behavioral disorders (Khan et al., 2005; Lahey, 2009; Martel & Nigg, 2006), though, surprisingly, it was not directly associated with PTSD. Of note, emotion dysregulation and PTSD were related at the bivariate level (Table 2). Table 3 Regression Weights for Hierarchical Models Predicting Refugee Girls’ Emotion Dysregulation From Age and Trauma, Comorbidities, Coping Strategies, Family Relationships, and School Environment Emotion dysregulation/predictor variables Age and trauma Age War trauma Time lapse Comorbidities PTSD BED Neuroticism Coping strategies Positive coping Negative coping Family relationships Cohesion Expressiveness Conflict School environment Total R2 ΔR2 F ΔF Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 95% CI .22* .30* −.05 .06 .01 −.02 .05 −.01 −.03 .05 −.01 −.02 .04 −.01 −.02 [−.12, .48] [−.26, .18] [−.07, .02] .03 .26* .42* .169 .169 36.22 36.22 .419 .250 63.93 76.33 .03 .26* .39* .02 .26* .39* .03 .25* .38* −.04 .13* −.03 .12* −.02 .13* .05 −.03 .02 .06 −.02 .01 −.07 .439 .004 37.00 3.30 .433 .014 50.56 6.50 .436 .003 37.00 .90 Note. Age is coded in years. CI = confidence interval; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; BED = behavioral and emotional disorders. * p < .001. [−.85, 2.38] [.27, .58] [.69, 1.12] [−.83, .41] [.45, 1.57] [−.13, [−.66, [−.43, [−.49, 1.10] .35] .60] .01] This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 162 KHAMIS Thus, these results indicate that, for refugee girls, PTSD did not predict residual variance in emotion dysregulation over and above that accounted for by neuroticism and emotional and behavioral disorders. To further analyze this result, hierarchical regression analysis was employed to examine the relationships among emotion dysregulation and comorbidities including PTSD in the first step, neuroticism in a second step, and emotional and behavioral disorders in the third step. The results revealed that emotion dysregulation was significantly associated with PTSD independent of the already known risk factors neuroticism, and emotional and behavioral disorders. Notably, when neuroticism was included in the second step, PTSD became nonsignificant. Each predictor block separately accounted for the following percentage of difficulties in emotion regulation variance: 4.5% by PTSD, 32.1% by neuroticism, and 4.9% by emotional and behavioral disorders. Therefore, neuroticism was of more significance in predicting emotion dysregulation compared with PTSD and emotional and behavioral disorders. Indeed, when accounting for comorbidities, the direct impact of age and war trauma were no longer significant, demonstrating that neuroticism, emotion, and behavioral disorders were so intensely overwhelming that the other variables were overshadowed. Future work will need to evaluate the mediating roles of multiple process variables among various forms of psychopathology simultaneously to examine differential pathways and specific processes that may be linked in each domain of emotion dysregulation. While the use of negative coping styles was associated with increased emotion dysregulation, the use of positive coping styles had no significant relationship with emotion dysregulation. These results are consistent with other findings that show that reliance on negative coping style contributes to emotion dysregulation in refugee children (Khamis, 2021). Contrary to previous research (Rivera et al., 2008; Sarmiento & Cardemil, 2009), the findings of this study show that family relations and school environment did not predict residual variance in emotion dysregulation over and above that accounted for by neuroticism and emotional and behavioral disorders, and negative coping styles. The mechanism of this nonsignificant effect remains unknown, and a variety of explanations is possible. One explanation for this result might be that Syrian refugee families may not be able to manage the emotional ramifications of the overwhelming refugee experience, forced displacement, and resettlement in the interfamilial subsystem; it is possible that children from these families may be less affected by family relationships (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). A second explanation might be that Syrian refugee girls are more likely to experience psychological maltreatment in their families because they perceive themselves as less valued members in the family, unwelcomed load, and a burden on the family (Hattar-Pollara, 2019). Emotional expression is impacted by ones’ cultural background and is particularly influenced by religion and ethics (Abu-Hamda et al., 2017; Dwairy). Cultural norms often limit girls from regulating their emotions. Women, as well as girls, are strictly controlled and are expected to adhere completely to the paternally dictated norms of the Arabic family (Weller et al., 1995). Hence, Syrian refugee girls’ feeling of disconnectedness in their overall family system may not provide a context that promotes the development of emotion regulation, potentially as manifested in parent–child subsystems. In a similar vein, the results of this study revealed that school environment was not a significant predictor of emotion dysregulation. There is a need for research focused on the experiences of Syrian refugee girls in school setting. The multidimensional and collective character of challenges facing refugee children and families calls for comprehensive psychosocial interventions through which healing the psychological wounds of war is complemented by restoring and supporting the social and physical environment so that it is one in which children and their families can thrive (Fazel & Betancourt, 2018). Examples for possible interventions include the caregiver support intervention which have been tested in studies with Syrian refugee children in Lebanon. This intervention focuses on strengthening parenting in adversity, which may ultimately promote positive family relations among Syrian refugees (Miller et al., 2022). Limitations The primary limitations of this study arise from the use of crosssectional and retrospective data collection. Due to the crosssectional nature of the data, the results were unable to determine the degree to which negative coping style, neuroticism, and emotion and behavioral disorders are risk factors for the development of difficulties in emotion regulation in refugee girls. Although the present study adds to the growing body of literature on emotion dysregulation and comorbidities including neuroticism and emotional and behavioral disorders, the correlational nature of the data limits our ability to determine the exact nature and direction of the relationships of interest. It is also possible that this association is bidirectional and that regular engagement in impulsive behaviors may lead to or exacerbate emotion dysregulation. Second, since the Syrian refugee girls in this study were attending the public schools in Lebanon and Jordan, the results may not be generalized to other refugee girls who resided in other host countries and those who were not attending school. Nevertheless, the results provide an important step to understanding resilience in a highly vulnerable child population with considerable barriers to research access. Conclusion Findings from this study indicated that war trauma and the combination of comorbidities associated with negative coping styles could lead to an overall state of emotion dysregulation in refugee girls. Enhanced understanding of the mechanistic role of these variables in contributing to emotion dysregulation in refugee girls may contribute to the development of effective interventions to target the psychological effects of the refugee experience. Keywords: Syrian refugee girls, war trauma, emotion dysregulation, coping style, family and school environment References Abu-Hamda, B., Soliman, A., Babekr, A., & Bellaj, T. (2017). Emotional expression and culture: Implications from nine Arab Countries. European Psychiatry, 41(Suppl. 1), S230–S230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017 .01.2237 Ager, A. (1993). Mental health issues in refugee populations: Review. Harvard Medical School. https://www.academia.edu/32952651/Mental health_issues in_refugee_populations_a_review Alkhawaldeh, A. (2018). Syrian refugees’ children instructional challenges and solutions in Jordan: Teachers’ and parents’ perspectives. Border Crossing, 8(2), 311–331. https://doi.org/10.33182/bc.v8i2.448 American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN REFUGEE GIRLS Amin, R., Rahman, S., Dorner, T. E., Björkenstam, E., Helgesson, M., Norredam, M. L., Sijbrandij, M., Sever, C. A., & Mittendorfer-Rutz, E. (2020). Country of birth, time period of resettlement and subsequent treated common mental disorders in young refugees in Sweden. The European Journal of Public Health, 30(6), 1169–1175. https://doi.org/ 10.1093/eurpub/ckaa140 Andersson, H. W., Bjørngaard, J. H., Kaspersen, S. L., Wang, C. E. A., Skre, I., & Dahl, T. (2010). The effects of individual factors and school environment on mental health and prejudiced attitudes among Norwegian adolescents. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45(5), 569– 577. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-009-0099-0 Arakelyan, S., & Ager, A. (2021). Annual Research Review: A multilevel bioecological analysis of factors influencing the mental health and psychosocial well-being of refugee children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 62(5), 484–509. https://doi.org/10 .1111/jcpp.13355 Bartels, S., Michael, S., & Bunting, A. (2021). Child marriage among Syrian refugees in Lebanon: At the gendered intersection of poverty, immigration, and safety. Journal of Immigrant & Refugee Studies, 19, 472–487. https://doi.org/10.1080/15562948.2020.1839619 Bennouna, C., Khauli, N., Basir, M., Allaf, C., Wessells, M., & Stark, L. (2019). School-based programs for Supporting the mental health and psychosocial wellbeing of adolescent forced migrants in high-income countries: A scoping review. Social Science & Medicine, 239, Article 112558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112558 Bennouna, C., Stark, L., & Wessells, M. (2020). Children and adolescents in conflict and displacement. In S. J. Song & P. Ventevogel (Eds.), Child, adolescent and family refugee mental health (pp. 17–36). Springer International Publishing. https://www.springer.com/gp/book/978303045 2773 Berman, H. (2001). Children and war: Current understandings and future directions. Public Health Nursing, 18(4), 243–252. https://doi.org/10.1046/j .1525-1446.2001.00243.x Boswall, K., & Al Akash, R. (2015). Personal perspectives of protracted displacement: An ethnographic insight into the isolation and coping mechanisms of Syrian women and girls living as urban refugees in northern Jordan. Intervention, 13, 203–215. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Harvard University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1995). Developmental ecology through time and space: A future perspective. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder, & K. Luscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human development (pp. 619–647). American Psychological Association. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 793– 828). Wiley. Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. R. (2003). Characteristics of resilient youths living in poverty: The role of self-regulatory processes. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 139–162. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0954579403000087 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. Compas, B. E., Watson, K., Reising, M., & Dunbar, J. (2013). Stress and coping: Transdiagnostic processes in child and adolescent psychopathology. In J. Ehrenrich-May & B. Chu (Eds.), Transdiagnostic mechanisms and treatment approaches of youth psychopathology (pp. 35–58). Guilford Press. Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2010). Marital conflict and children: An emotional security perspective. Guilford Press. Doolan, E. L., Bryant, R. A., Liddell, B. J., & Nickerson, A. (2017). The conceptualization of emotion regulation difficulties, and its association with 163 posttraumatic stress symptoms in traumatized refugees. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 50, 7–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.04.005 Dwairy, M. (1998). Cross-cultural counseling: The Arab–Palestinian case. Haworth Press. Ehring, T., & Quack, D. (2010). Emotion regulation difficulties in trauma survivors: The role of trauma type and PTSD symptom severity. Behavior Therapy, 41(4), 587–598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth .2010.04.004 Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241–273. https://doi.org/10 .1207/s15327965pli0904_1 El Arab, R., & Sagbakken, M. (2019). Child marriage of female Syrian refugees in Jordan and Lebanon: A literature review. Global Health Action, 12(1), Article 1585709. https://doi.org/10.1080/16549716.2019 .1585709 El Khalek, A. (1978). Eysenck personality inventories. Dar El Maaref. Emerson, S., Gagné Petteni, M., Guhn, M., Oberle, E., Georgiades, K., Milbrath, C., Janus, M., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Gadermann, A. M. (2022). Social context factors and refugee children’s emotional health. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57, 829–841. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s00127-021-02173-y Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Manual of the eysenck personality inventory. Educational Testing Service. Fazel, M., & Betancourt, T. S. (2018). Preventive mental health interventions for refugee children and adolescents in high-income settings. The Lancet: Child & Adolescent Health, 2(2), 121–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S23524642(17)30147-5 Fazel, M., Reed, R., & Stein, A. (2015). Refugee, asylum-seeking and internally displaced children and adolescents. In A. Thapar, D. S. Pine, J. F. Leckman, S. Scott, M. J. Snowling, & E. Taylor (Eds.), Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 573–585). Wiley. Fazel, M., Wheeler, J., & Danesh, J. (2005). Prevalence of serious mental disorder in 7000 refugees resettled in western countries: A systematic review. Lancet, 365(9467), 1309–1314. https://doi.org/10.1016/S01406736(05)61027-6 Fried, L. (2011). Teaching teachers about emotion regulation in the classroom. The Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 36(3). https://doi.org/ 10.14221/ajte.2011v36n3.1 Goldberger, L., & Breznitz, S. (Eds.). (1993). Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (2nd ed.). Free Press. Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 38(5), 581–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997 .tb01545.x Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 41–54. https:// doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94 Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation and children’s early academic success. Journal of School Psychology, 45(1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp .2006.09.002 Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). Guilford Press. Guruge, S., & Butt, H. (2015). A scoping review of mental health issues and concerns among immigrant and refugee youth in Canada: Looking back, moving forward. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 106(2), e72–e78. https://doi.org/10.17269/CJPH.106.4588 Hargreaves, A. (2000). Mixed emotions: Teachers’ perceptions of their interactions with students. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16, 811–826. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0742-051X(00)00028-7 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 164 KHAMIS Hassan, G., Kirmayer, L., & Mekki-Berrada, A. (2015). Culture, context and the mental health psychosocial wellbeing of Syrians: A review for mental health and psychosocial support staff working with Syrians affected by armed conflict. UNHCR. Hassan, G., Ventevogel, P., Jefee-Bahloul, H., Barkil-Oteo, A., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Mental health and psychosocial wellbeing of Syrians affected by armed conflict. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25(2), 129–141. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796016000044 Hattar-Pollara, M. (2019). Barriers to education of Syrian refugee girls in Jordan: Gender-based threats and challenges. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 51(3), 241–251. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnu.12480 Hollifield, M., Warner, T., Jenkins, J., Sinclair-Lian, N., Krakow, B., Eckert, V., Karadaghi, P., & Westermeyer, J. (2006). Assessing war trauma in refugees: Properties of the Comprehensive Trauma Inventory-104. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19(4), 527–540. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20137 IBM Corporation Released. (2020). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (Version 27.0) [Computer software]. IBM Corporation. International Rescue Committee. (2013). Cross-sectoral assessment of Syrian Refugees in Urban Areas of South and Central Jordan. https:// data.unhcr.org/en/documents/details/38299 International Rescue Committee. (2014). Are we listening? Acting on our commitments to women and girls affected by the Syrian conflict. Iraklis, G. (2021a). Facing forced displacement: Overcoming adverse childhood experiences. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 14(2), 261–269. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-020-00320-2 Iraklis, G. (2021b). Family bonds in the midst of adversity: Insights into refugee children’s coping ways. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26(1), 222–230. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104520964508 Jensen, T. K., Skårdalsmo, E. M., & Fjermestad, K. W. (2014). Development of mental health problems-a follow-up study of unaccompanied refugee minors. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 8, Article 29. https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-8-29 Kandemir, H., Karataş, H., Çeri, V., Solmaz, F., Kandemir, S. B., & Solmaz, A. (2018). Prevalence of war-related adverse events, depression and anxiety among Syrian refugee children settled in Turkey. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(11), 1513–1517. https://doi.org/10 .1007/s00787-018-1178-0 Khamis, V. (2005). Post-traumatic stress disorder among school age Palestinian children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29(1), 81–95. https://doi.org/10 .1016/j.chiabu.2004.06.013 Khamis, V. (2008). Post-traumatic stress and psychiatric disorders in Palestinian adolescents following intifada-related injuries. Social Science & Medicine, 67(8), 1199–1207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.06.013 Khamis, V. (2012). Impact of war, religiosity and ideology on PTSD and psychiatric disorders in adolescents from Gaza Strip and South Lebanon. Social Science & Medicine, 74(12), 2005–2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j .socscimed.2012.02.025 Khamis, V. (2015). Coping with war trauma and psychological distress among school-age Palestinian children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 85(1), 72–79. https://doi.org/10.1037/ort0000039 Khamis, V. (2019). Posttraumatic stress disorder and emotion dysregulation among Syrian refugee children and adolescents resettled in Lebanon and Jordan. Child Abuse & Neglect, 89, 29–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chia bu.2018.12.013 Khamis, V. (2021). Psychological distress and neuroticism among Syrian refugee parents in post-resettlement contexts. Journal of Health Psychology, 27(5), 1149–1164. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105320988321 Khan, A. A., Jacobson, K. C., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2005). Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 190–196. https://doi.org/10 .1192/bjp.186.3.190 Kirmayer, L. J., Narasiah, L., Munoz, M., Rashid, M., Ryder, A. G., Guzder, J., Hassan, G., Rousseau, C., Pottie, K., & the Canadian Collaboration for Immigrant and Refugee Health (CCIRH). (2011). Common mental health problems in immigrants and refugees: General approach in primary care. CMAJ: Canadian Medical Association Journal, 183(12), E959–E967. https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.090292 Kokkonen, M., & Pulkkinen, L. (2001). Extraversion and neuroticism as antecedents of emotion regulation and dysregulation in adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 15, 407–424. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.425 Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. S. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Guilford Press. Lahey, B. B. (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist, 64(4), 241–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015309 Lindsey, E. W. (2020). Relationship context and emotion regulation across the life span. Emotion, 20(1), 59–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000666 Lustig, S. L., Kia-Keating, M., Knight, W. G., Geltman, P., Ellis, H., Kinzie, J. D., Keane, T., & Saxe, G. N. (2004). Review of child and adolescent refugee mental health. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(1), 24–36. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583200401000-00012 Mącik, D., Łysiak, M., & Mącik, R. (2019). Mediating role of neuroticism between early maladaptive schemas and negative emotionality. Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 7, 220–231. https://doi.org/10.5114/ cipp.2019.88583 Maegusuku-Hewett, T., Dunkerley, D., Scourfield, J., & Smalley, N. (2007). Refugee children in Wales: Coping and Adaptation in the face of adversity. Children & Society, 21, 309–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1099-0860 .2007.00102.x Martel, M. M., & Nigg, J. T. (2006). Child ADHD and personality/temperament traits of reactive and effortful control, resiliency, and emotionality. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 47(11), 1175–1183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01629.x Miller, K., Chen, A., Koppenol-Gonzalez, G., Bakolis, I., Arnous, M., Tossyeh, F., El Hassan, A., & Saleh, A. ( 2022). Supporting parenting among Syrian refugees in Lebanon: A randomized controlled trial of the caregiver support intervention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13668 Montgomery, E. (2010). Trauma and resilience in young refugees: A 9-year follow-up study. Development and Psychopathology, 22(2), 477–489. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000180 Montgomery, E., & Foldspang, A. (2008). Discrimination, mental problems and social adaptation in young refugees. The European Journal of Public Health, 18(2), 156–161. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckm073 Moos, R., & Moos, B. (2002). Family environment scale. Mind Garden. Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j .1467-9507.2007.00389.x Ní Raghallaigh, M. N., & Gilligan, R. (2010). Active survival in the lives of unaccompanied minors: Coping strategies, resilience, and the relevance of religion. Child & Family Social Work, 15, 226–237. https://doi.org/10 .1111/j.1365-2206.2009.00663.x Nickerson, A., Bryant, R. A., Schnyder, U., Schick, M., Mueller, J., & Morina, N. (2015). Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between trauma exposure, post-migration living difficulties and psychological outcomes in traumatized refugees. Journal of Affective Disorders, 173, 185–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.043 Nickerson, A., Garber, B., Ahmed, O., Asnaani, A., Cheung, J., Hofmann, S. G., Huynh, L., Liddell, B., Litz, B. T., Pajak, R., & Bryant, R. A. (2016). Emotional suppression in torture survivors: Relationship to posttraumatic stress symptoms and trauma-related negative affect. Psychiatry Research, 242, 233–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.048 Nickerson, A., Garber, B., Liddell, B. J., Litz, B. T., Hofmann, S. G., Asnaani, A., Ahmed, O., Cheung, J., Huynh, L., Pajak, R., & Bryant, R. A. (2017). Impact of cognitive reappraisal on negative affect, heart rate, and intrusive memories in traumatized refugees. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(3), 497–512. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617690857 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN REFUGEE GIRLS Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 704–708. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012 Norwegian Refugee Council. (2020). The obstacle course: Barriers to education for syrian refugee children in Lebanon. https://www.nrc.no/ globalassets/pdf/reports/the-obstacle-course-barriers-to-education/exe cutive-summary---the-obstacle-course_barriers-to-education.pdf Pieloch, K. A., McCullough, M. B., & Marks, A. K. (2016). Resilience of children with refugee statuses: A research review. Canadian Psychology, 57(4), 330–339. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000073 Porter, M., & Haslam, N. (2005). Predisplacement and postdisplacement factors associated with mental health of refugees and internally displaced persons: A meta-analysis. JAMA, 294(5), 602–612. https://doi.org/10.1001/ jama.294.5.602 Reed, R. V., Fazel, M., Jones, L., Panter-Brick, C., & Stein, A. (2012). Mental health of displaced and refugee children resettled in low-income and middle-income countries: Risk and protective factors. Lancet, 379(9812), 250–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60050-0 Rivera, F. I., Guarnaccia, P. J., Mulvaney-Day, N., Lin, J. Y., Torres, M., & Alegria, M. (2008). Family cohesion and its relationship to psychological distress among Latino groups. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 30(3), 357–378. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986308318713 Samara, M., El Asam, A., Khadaroo, A., & Hammuda, S. (2020). Examining the psychological well-being of refugee children and the role of friendship and bullying. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 301– 329. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12282 Sarmiento, I. A., & Cardemil, E. V. (2009). Family functioning and depression in low-income Latino couples. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 35(4), 432–445. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.2009 .00139.x Scherer, N., Hameed, S., Acarturk, C., Deniz, G., Sheikhani, A., Volkan, S., Örücü, A., Pivato, I., Akıncı, İ., Patterson, A., & Polack, S. (2020). Prevalence of common mental disorders among Syrian refugee children and adolescents in Sultanbeyli district, Istanbul: Results of a populationbased survey. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, Article e192. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796020001079 Schwartz, S. J., Unger, J. B., Zamboanga, B. L., & Szapocznik, J. (2010). Rethinking the concept of acculturation: Implications for theory and research. American Psychologist, 65(4), 237–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0019330 Silove, D., Ventevogel, P., & Rees, S. (2017). The contemporary refugee crisis: An overview of mental health challenges. World Psychiatry, 16(2), 130–139. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20438 Siriwardhana, C., Ali, S. S., Roberts, B., & Stewart, R. (2014). A systematic review of resilience and mental health outcomes of conflict-driven adult 165 forced migrants. Conflict and Health, 8, Article 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/ 1752-1505-8-13 Specker, P., & Nickerson, A. (2019). Investigating the relationship between distinctive patterns of emotion regulation, trauma exposure and psychopathology among refugees resettled in Australia: A latent class analysis. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1), Article 1661814. https:// doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1661814 Spirito, A., Stark, L. J., & Williams, C. (1988). Development of a brief coping checklist for use with pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 13(4), 555–574. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/13 .4.555 Stringaris, A., & Goodman, R. (2009). Mood lability and psychopathology in youth. Psychological Medicine, 39(8), 1237–1245. https://doi.org/10 .1017/S0033291708004662 Thapa, A., Cohen, J., Guffey, S., & Higgins-D’Alessandro, A. (2013). A review of school climate research. Review of Educational Research, 83, 357–385. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654313483907 Trosper, S. E., Buzzella, B. A., Bennett, S. M., & Ehrenreich, J. T. (2009). Emotion regulation in youth with emotional disorders: Implications for a unified treatment approach. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 12(3), 234–254. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-009-0043-6 Tull, M. T., Barrett, H. M., McMillan, E. S., & Roemer, L. (2007). A preliminary investigation of the relationship between emotion regulation difficulties and posttraumatic stress symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 303–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.001 United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2018). Lebanon puts in an extra shift to get Syrian refugees into school Lebanon puts in an extra shift to get Syrian refugees into school. https://reliefweb.int/report/leba non/lebanon-puts-extra-shift-get-syrian-refugees-school United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2022). Operational portal data. Retrieved August 23, 2022, from https://data.unhcr.org/en/ situations/syria/location/36 United Nations Population Fund. (2017). New study finds child marriage rising among most vulnerable Syrian refugees. http://www.unfpa.org/ne ws/new-study-finds-child-marriage-rising-among-most-vulnerable-syrianrefugees Weinstock, L. M., & Whisman, M. A. (2006). Neuroticism as a common feature of the depressive and anxiety disorders: A test of the revised integrative hierarchical model in a national sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(1), 68–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.68 Weller, A., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1995). Adolescents’ reports of parental division of power in a multicultural society. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 5(4), 387–411. Wilson, R. M., Murtaza, R., & Shakya, Y. B. (2010). Pre-migration and post-migration determinants of mental health for newly arrived refugees in Toronto. Canadian Issues: Immigrant Mental Health, 45–50.