What Research Says About Text Complexity and Learning to Read Article Critique Article Synthesis and Critique What Research Says About Text Complexity and Learning to Read, focuses on the how text complexity influences the acquisition of reading skills. According to the article, there is a large amount of research to suggest that texts read with 95% or greater accuracy has a direct impact on reading achievement (Allington, Mccuiston, and Billen, 2015). This article seems to be reactionary to some of the comments made by the CCSSO about text complexity. The CCSSO has made some claims that text complexity has decreased over time without supporting evidence (Allington, et al., 2015). The first part of this paper is dedicated to proving that schools have not been lowering the complexity of texts. Allington, et al., (2015) argues that: We have 70 years of evidence that children are more likely to learn to read and to learn content when the text can be read with a high level of accuracy and comprehension. (p. 492) Several researchers have been examined to provide further evidence. Emmet Betts’ levels of text complexity in 1946 are the first piece of research cited by Allington, et al., (2015). Betts’ levels suggest that students reach their independent level when they can read with 99% accuracy (Allington, et al., 2015). However, Betts’ levels have been under question over the past 40 years (Allington, et al., 2015). In one study, Anderson et al. (1979), reports that oral reading has a negative correlation with reading growth. There are several other researchers who have questioned Betts’ levels: (Ehri, Dreyer, Flugman, & Gross, 2007; Fuchs, Fuchs, & Deno, 1982; Gambrell, Wilson, & Gantt, 1981; Gickling & Armstrong, 1985; Jorgenson, Klein, & Kumar, 1977 ; Morris, Bloodgood, Perney, Frye, Kucan, & Trathen, 2011 ; O’ Connor, Bell, Harty, Larkin, Sackor, & Zigmond, 2002; Treptoe, Burns, & McComas, 2007). There are three main areas that explain why students who can read with 95% accuracy have better reading success: engagement, vocabulary, and self-regulating behaviors (Allington, et al., 2015). First Jorgenson (1977) states when less complex texts, in relation to instructional level, are presented to students they worked more independently and increased engagement. Secondly, independent reading is the best source for vocabulary acquisition according to (Nagy, Anderson, & Herman, 1987; Nagy, Herman, & Anderson, 1985; Stahl, 1999; Swanborn & DeGlopper, 1999). Lastly, Leslie and Osol (1978) explain students are able to use decoding and self-corrective strategies most effectively when reading texts with 95% accuracy. To emphasize the importance of reading text with 95% accuracy, Allington, et al., (2015) examines several studies that show that lower accuracy levels are achieved when students are presented with text that is too difficult. As a result, the authentic research presented by the author proves the following: that texts have not decreased in complexity; the research concludes the relationship between texts that can be read at 95% accuracy and reading achievement. Also, that decisions on increasing text complexity should be based on more reliable research, otherwise there could be a negative effect on reading achievement. Professional Literature Connections Allington, et al., (2015) mentions several strategies to assist in reading fluency like silent reading and assisted reading. Rasinski and Young (2017) second the notion of both. They both assert that acquiring fluency can be linked to modeling and assisted reading aids in reading achievement. Rasinski and Young (2017) also argue the importance of fluency instruction and suggest that speed should not be the main goal in acquiring fluency as the CCSS mandate. Pardo (2004) uses an earlier Rasinski work (2003) to reinforce the argument that fluency instruction aids in engagement, requires modeling, and the importance of reading aloud to students. Pardo (2004) also suggest a long list of concrete ways teachers can help develop reading skills. All of her suggestions are mentioned in Allington, et al., (2015). The main skills necessary to develop reading comprehension are decoding skills, vocabulary, and engagement (Pardo, 2004). Decoding is taught by focusing on phonemic awareness and phonics. Pardo also suggests that teaching vocabulary is necessary because presenting a text with too many unknown words, it will be interfere with the comprehension of the text as a whole. Finally, to improve engagement students should be provided text that is interesting and given a variety of choices of text to read. McLaughlin and Overturf (2012) explains that although the CCSS are predetermined set of skills students are required to learn, there is room for educators to decide how to reach these goals as well as the ability to add necessary topics. This room could allow for a teacher to include fluency instruction as mentioned in Rasinski and Young (2017) while teaching to the CCSS standards. The author describes these standards as wide and leave room for educators to implement them in a way that suites their teaching styles. Implications for Instruction/Policy There are three common skills that each of this week’s authors agrees on to develop reading skills: modeling, vocabulary, and engagement. Educators can improve reading fluency through modeling strategies like thinking allowed, demonstrating and creating meaning (Pardo, 2004). Rasinski and Young (2017) suggest that fluent peer readers as well as teachers can accomplish modeling. Allington, et al., (2015) and Rasinski and Young (2017) suggest that reading aloud is the best evidence based form of modeling. Reading aloud can take many forms like choral reading, round robin, and popcorn. Vocabulary development is directly related to reading fluency (Allington, et al., 2015). There are many ways to develop a student’s vocabulary. Independent reading time, with texts that can be read with 95% accuracy, would help develop vocabulary (Allington, et al., 2015). Providing background knowledge and opportunities to use the new vocabulary in meaningful ways (Pardo, 2004). To accomplish this a teacher can use graphic organizers, give the students a variety of examples of how to use the new words and give the students opportunities to use the new vocabulary before reading. Engagement is like a thread that is woven between each element of reading fluency. Engagement can be found in both vocabulary development and modeling. One way to improve engagement is by giving students choices in the text they read. Educators can also create better relationships with their students to align the student’s interest to reading material. Allington, et al., (2015) investigates a variety of strategies that children use to learn to read. The three techniques I chose for this are just a few and if used effectively they can help students to acquire reading skills. Conclusion This article presented a very precise and well researched idea. There are many points that I will now have to consider when confronted with changes in education. Allington, et al., (2015) made me think about what the sources are for new ideas in education. I am now more aware of the necessity to perform my own research into a new approach instead of taking it at full market value. I really appreciate all the care of research that went into proving their point. There are many points of Allington, et al., (2015) which I agree with. For example, texts read with 95% or greater accuracy has a direct impact on reading achievement. I felt this was well proven with evidencebased research over the past 40 years. This is also something you can see in action in today’s classroom. Try to give a student text above their level, and watch them struggle, and eventually lose engagement. I also agree with the importance of reading aloud. Listening is by far is one of the most important ways to begin to develop phonological awareness. Reading text repeatedly also helps students’ comprehension. Moreover, I agree with the connection between engagement, vocabulary, and self-regulating behaviors and appropriate level texts. Students are more engaged when they see they can accomplish a task, and in turn will stay on task. A few ideas that I don’t agree with were one, the idea that the CCSS is so rigid that an educator cannot add on. For example, Rasinski and Young (2017) claim that the CCSS should include fluency. However, I feel that if the standards are too specific, we will lose the freedom to choose how we teach. Second, although I do agree that the CCSS authors did make some claims without enough supporting evidence, their goals are valid. They want to make students college ready (McLaughlin and Overturf, 2012). I also like the idea of having a common standard shared between each state. Just as Allington, et al., (2015) showed me not to take everything at face value without evidence, I don’t want to throw out the CCSS completely. There were valuable insights in the article for this critique that will resonate with me for a long time.